1932

Abstract

Research in cultural psychology over the last three decades has revealed the profound influence of culture on cognitive, emotional, and motivational processes shaping individuals into active agents. This article aims to show cultural psychology's promise in three key steps. First, we review four notable cultural dimensions believed to underlie cultural variations: independent versus interdependent self, individualism versus collectivism, tightness versus looseness of social norms, and relational mobility. Second, we examine how ecology and geography shape human activities and give rise to organized systems of cultural practices and meanings, called eco-cultural complexes. In turn, the eco-cultural complex of each zone is instrumental in shaping a wide range of psychological processes, revealing a psychological diversity that extends beyond the scope of the current East–West literature. Finally, we examine some of the non-Western cultural zones present today, including Arab, East Asian, Latin American, and South Asian zones, and discuss how they may have contributed, to varying degrees, to the formation of the contemporary Western cultural zone.

Loading

Article metrics loading...

/content/journals/10.1146/annurev-psych-021723-063333
2024-01-18
2024-04-28
Loading full text...

Full text loading...

/deliver/fulltext/psych/75/1/annurev-psych-021723-063333.html?itemId=/content/journals/10.1146/annurev-psych-021723-063333&mimeType=html&fmt=ahah

Literature Cited

  1. Adams G. 2005. The cultural grounding of personal relationship: enemyship in North American and West African worlds. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 88:6948–68
    [Google Scholar]
  2. AlQahtani SM, Almutairi DS, BinAqeel EA, Almutairi RA, Al-Qahtani RD, Menezes RG. 2022. Honor killings in the Eastern Mediterranean region: a narrative review. Healthcare 11:174
    [Google Scholar]
  3. Asch S. 1946. Forming impressions of personality. J. Abnorm. Soc. Psychol. 41:3258–90
    [Google Scholar]
  4. Atari M, Graham J, Dehghani M. 2020. Foundations of morality in Iran. Evol. Hum. Behav. 41:5367–84
    [Google Scholar]
  5. Awad E, Dsouza S, Shariff A, Rahwan I, Bonnefon J-F. 2020. Universals and variations in moral decisions made in 42 countries by 70,000 participants. PNAS 117:52332–37
    [Google Scholar]
  6. Bath E, Edmunds D, Norman J, Atkins C, Harper L et al. 2021. Sex ratio and the evolution of aggression in fruit flies. Proc. R. Soc. B 288:194720203053
    [Google Scholar]
  7. Belsky J, Pluess M. 2009. Beyond diathesis stress: differential susceptibility to environmental influences. Psychol. Bull. 135:6885–908
    [Google Scholar]
  8. Borsboom D, Deserno MK, Rhemtulla M, Epskamp S, Fried EI et al. 2021. Network analysis of multivariate data in psychological science. Nat. Rev. Methods Primers 1:158
    [Google Scholar]
  9. Brewer MB, Yuki M. 2007. Culture and social identity. Handbook of Cultural Psychology S Kitayama, D Cohen 307–22. New York: Guilford
    [Google Scholar]
  10. Bruner J. 1990. Acts of Meaning Cambridge, MA: Harvard Univ. Press
  11. Camb. Assess. Int. Educ. 2018. Global education census report Rep. Camb. Assess. Int. Educ. Cambridge, UK: https://www.cambridgeinternational.org/Images/514611-global-education-census-survey-report.pdf
  12. Campbell WK, Sedikides C. 1999. Self-threat magnifies the self-serving bias: a meta-analytic integration. Rev. Gen. Psychol. 3:123–43
    [Google Scholar]
  13. Campos B, Kim HS. 2017. Incorporating the cultural diversity of family and close relationships into the study of health. Am. Psychol. 72:6543–54
    [Google Scholar]
  14. Cann RL, Stoneking M, Wilson AC. 1987. Mitochondrial DNA and human evolution. Nature 325:31–36
    [Google Scholar]
  15. Choi I, Nisbett RE, Norenzayan A. 1999. Causal attribution across cultures: variation and universality. Psychol. Bull. 125:47–63
    [Google Scholar]
  16. Cohen D, Gunz A. 2002. As seen by the other…: perspectives on the self in the memories and emotional perceptions of Easterners and Westerners. Psychol. Sci. 13:155–59
    [Google Scholar]
  17. Cohen D, Kitayama S, eds. 2018. Handbook of Cultural Psychology New York: Guilford. , 2nd ed..
  18. Crockett MJ. 2017. Moral outrage in the digital age. Nat. Hum. Behav. 1:11769–71
    [Google Scholar]
  19. Diamond J. 1999. Guns, Germs, and Steel: The Fates of Human Societies New York: W.W. Norton & Co.
  20. Diamond-Smith N, Rudolph K. 2018. The association between uneven sex ratios and violence: evidence from 6 Asian countries. PLOS ONE 13:6e0197516
    [Google Scholar]
  21. Duncan LE, Keller MC. 2011. A critical review of the first 10 years of candidate gene-by-environment interaction research in psychiatry. Am. J. Psychiatry 168:101041–49
    [Google Scholar]
  22. Eckstein AM. 2007. Mediterranean Anarchy, Interstate War, and the Rise of Rome Berkeley: Univ. Calif. Press
  23. Epstein RA, Baker CI. 2019. Scene perception in the human brain. Annu. Rev. Vis. Sci. 5:373–97
    [Google Scholar]
  24. Fiske ST, Taylor SE. 1991. Social Cognition New York: McGraw-Hill
  25. Geertz C. 1975. The Interpretation of Cultures: Selected Essays New York: Basic Books
  26. Gelfand MJ, Harrington JR, Jackson JC. 2017. The strength of social norms across human groups. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 12:5800–9
    [Google Scholar]
  27. Gelfand MJ, Nishii LH, Raver JL. 2006. On the nature and importance of cultural tightness-looseness. J. Appl. Psychol. 91:61225–44
    [Google Scholar]
  28. Gelfand MJ, Raver JL, Nishii L, Leslie LM, Lun J et al. 2011. Differences between tight and loose cultures: a 33-nation study. Science 332:60331100–4
    [Google Scholar]
  29. Gilmore DG ed. 1987. Honor and Shame and the Unity of the Mediterranean Arlington, VA: Am. Anthropol. Assoc.
  30. Glazer J, King A, Yoon C, Liberzon I, Kitayama S. 2020. DRD4 polymorphisms modulate reward positivity and P3a in a gambling task: exploring a genetic basis for cultural learning. Psychophysiology 57:10e13623
    [Google Scholar]
  31. Gould SJ. 1981. The Mismeasure of Man New York: W.W. Norton & Co. , 1st ed..
  32. Graeber D, Wengrow D. 2021. The Dawn of Everything London: Macmillan
  33. Graham J, Haidt J, Koleva S, Motyl M, Iyer R et al. 2013. Moral foundations theory. Adv. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 47:55–130
    [Google Scholar]
  34. Graham J, Nosek BA, Haidt J, Iyer R, Koleva S, Ditto PH. 2011. Mapping the moral domain. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 101:366–85
    [Google Scholar]
  35. Greenfield PM, Keller H, Fuligni A, Maynard A. 2003. Cultural pathways through universal development. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 54:461–90
    [Google Scholar]
  36. Grossmann I, Jowhari N. 2018. Cognition and the self: attempt of an independent close replication of the effects of self-construal priming on spatial memory recall. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 74:65–73
    [Google Scholar]
  37. Grossmann I, Kross E. 2010. The impact of culture on adaptive versus maladaptive self-reflection. Psychol. Sci. 21:81150–57
    [Google Scholar]
  38. Gutchess AH, Welsh RC, Boduroĝlu A, Park DC. 2006. Cultural differences in neural function associated with object processing. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 6:2102–9
    [Google Scholar]
  39. Hallowell AI. 2016 (1955). Culture and Experience. Philadelphia: Univ. Pa. Press
  40. Hawks J, Wang ET, Cochran GM, Harpending HC, Moyzis RK. 2007. Recent acceleration of human adaptive evolution. PNAS 104:5220753–58
    [Google Scholar]
  41. Heine SJ, Kitayama S, Hamamura T. 2007. Inclusion of additional studies yields different conclusions: comment on Sedikides, Gaertner, & Vevea 2005, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. Asian J. Soc. Psychol. 10:249–58
    [Google Scholar]
  42. Heine SJ, Lehman DR, Markus HR, Kitayama S. 1999. Is there a universal need for positive self-regard?. Psychol. Rev. 106:4766–94
    [Google Scholar]
  43. Henrich J. 2015. The Secret of Our Success: How Culture Is Driving Human Evolution, Domesticating Our Species, and Making Us Smarter Princeton, NJ: Princeton Univ. Press
  44. Henrich J, Heine SJ, Norenzayan A. 2010. The weirdest people in the world?. Behav. Brain Sci. 33:2–361–83
    [Google Scholar]
  45. Herrmann B, Thöni C, Gächter S. 2008. Antisocial punishment across societies. Science 319:58681362–67
    [Google Scholar]
  46. Hochschild AR. 1979. Emotion work, feeling rules, and social structure. Am. J. Sociol. 85:3551–75
    [Google Scholar]
  47. Hofstede G. 1980. Culture's Consequences: International Differences in Work-Related Values London: SAGE
  48. Hsu TW, Niiya Y, Thelwall M, Ko M, Knutson B, Tsai JL. 2021. Social media users produce more affect that supports cultural values, but are more influenced by affect that violates cultural values. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 121:5969–83
    [Google Scholar]
  49. Inglehart R, Baker WE. 2000. Modernization, cultural change, and the persistence of traditional values. Am. Sociol. Rev. 65:119–51
    [Google Scholar]
  50. Iyengar S, Lelkes Y, Levendusky M, Malhotra N, Westwood SJ. 2019. The origins and consequences of affective polarization in the United States. Annu. Rev. Political Sci. 22:129–46
    [Google Scholar]
  51. Jackson JC, Caluori N, Abrams S, Beckman E, Gelfand M, Gray K. 2021. Tight cultures and vengeful gods: how culture shapes religious belief. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 150:102057–77
    [Google Scholar]
  52. Jackson JC, Gelfand M, De S, Fox A. 2019. The loosening of American culture over 200 years is associated with a creativity-order trade-off. Nat. Hum. Behav. 3:3244–50
    [Google Scholar]
  53. Ji L-J, Zhang Z, Nisbett RE. 2004. Is it culture or is it language? Examination of language effects in cross-cultural research on categorization. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 87:157–65
    [Google Scholar]
  54. Kitayama S, Duffy S, Kawamura T, Larsen JT. 2003. Perceiving an object and its context in different cultures: a cultural look at new look. Psychol. Sci. 14:3201–6
    [Google Scholar]
  55. Kitayama S, King A, Hsu M, Liberzon I, Yoon C. 2016. Dopamine-system genes and cultural acquisition: the norm sensitivity hypothesis. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 8:167–74
    [Google Scholar]
  56. Kitayama S, Markus HR, Kurokawa M. 2000. Culture, emotion, and well-being: good feelings in Japan and the United States. Cogn. Emot. 14:193–124
    [Google Scholar]
  57. Kitayama S, Markus HR, Matsumoto H, Norasakkunkit V. 1997. Individual and collective processes in the construction of the self: self-enhancement in the United States and self-criticism in Japan. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 72:61245–67
    [Google Scholar]
  58. Kitayama S, Mesquita B, Karasawa M. 2006. Cultural affordances and emotional experience: socially engaging and disengaging emotions in Japan and the United States. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 91:5890–903
    [Google Scholar]
  59. Kitayama S, Park H, Sevincer AT, Karasawa M, Uskul AK. 2009. A cultural task analysis of implicit independence: comparing North America, Western Europe, and East Asia. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 97:2236–55
    [Google Scholar]
  60. Kitayama S, Salvador C. 2017. Culture embrained: going beyond the nature-nurture dichotomy. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 12:5841–54
    [Google Scholar]
  61. Kitayama S, Salvador CE, Nanakdewa K, Rossmaier A, San Martin A, Savani K. 2022. Varieties of interdependence and the emergence of the Modern West: toward the globalizing of psychology. Am. Psychol. 77:9991–1006
    [Google Scholar]
  62. Kitayama S, Uskul AK. 2011. Culture, mind, and the brain: current evidence and future directions. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 62:419–49
    [Google Scholar]
  63. Kitayama S, Yu Q 2020. Mutual constitution of culture and the mind: insights from cultural neuroscience. Culture, Mind, and Brain: Emerging Concepts, Models, and Applications LJ Kirmayer, CM Worthman, S Kitayama, R Lemelson, C Cummings 88–119. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge Univ. Press
    [Google Scholar]
  64. Kitayama S, Yu Q, King AP, Yoon C, Liberzon I. 2020. The gray matter volume of the temporoparietal junction varies across cultures: a moderating role of the dopamine D4 receptor gene (DRD4). Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 15:2193–202
    [Google Scholar]
  65. Knyazev GG. 2013. EEG correlates of self-referential processing. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:264
    [Google Scholar]
  66. Kraus B, Salvador CE, Kamikubo A, Hsiao N-C, Hu J-F et al. 2021. Oscillatory alpha power at rest reveals an independent self: a cross-cultural investigation. Biol. Psychol. 163:108118
    [Google Scholar]
  67. Krys K, Vignoles VL, de Almeida I, Uchida Y. 2022. Outside the “cultural binary”: understanding why Latin American collectivist societies foster independent selves. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 17:41166–87
    [Google Scholar]
  68. Laland KN, Odling-Smee J, Myles S 2010. How culture shaped the human genome: bringing genetics and the human sciences together. Nat. Rev. Genet. 11:2137–48
    [Google Scholar]
  69. Liu SS, Morris MW, Talhelm T, Yang Q. 2019. Ingroup vigilance in collectivistic cultures. PNAS 116:2914538–46
    [Google Scholar]
  70. Lu JG. 2022. A social network perspective on the Bamboo Ceiling: Ethnic homophily explains why East Asians but not South Asians are underrepresented in leadership in multiethnic environments. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 122:6959–82
    [Google Scholar]
  71. Lu JG, Jin P, English AS. 2021. Collectivism predicts mask use during COVID-19. PNAS 118:23e2021793118
    [Google Scholar]
  72. Lu JG, Nisbett RE, Morris MW. 2020. Why East Asians but not South Asians are underrepresented in leadership positions in the United States. PNAS 117:94590–600
    [Google Scholar]
  73. Lu JG, Nisbett RE, Morris MW. 2022. The surprising underperformance of East Asians in US law and business schools: the liability of low assertiveness and the ameliorative potential of online classrooms. PNAS 119:13e2118244119
    [Google Scholar]
  74. Markus HR, Kitayama S. 1991. Culture and the self: implications for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychol. Rev. 98:2224–53
    [Google Scholar]
  75. Markus HR, Kitayama S. 2010. Cultures and selves: a cycle of mutual constitution. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 5:4420–30
    [Google Scholar]
  76. Martinez-Echazabal L. 1998. Mestizaje and the discourse of national/cultural identity in Latin America, 1845–1959. Latin Am. Perspect. 25:321–42
    [Google Scholar]
  77. Masuda T, Ellsworth PC, Mesquita B, Leu J, Tanida S, Van de Veerdonk E. 2008. Placing the face in context: cultural differences in the perception of facial emotion. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 94:3365–81
    [Google Scholar]
  78. Masuda T, Kitayama S. 2004. Perceiver-induced constraint and attitude attribution in Japan and the US: a case for the cultural dependence of the correspondence bias. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 40:3409–16
    [Google Scholar]
  79. McCarthy E. 2019. Watsuji Tetsurō: the mutuality of climate and culture and an ethics of betweenness. The Oxford Handbook of Japanese Philosophy BW Davis 502–22. Oxford, UK: Oxford Univ. Press
    [Google Scholar]
  80. Miyamoto Y, Boylan JM, Coe CL, Curhan KB, Levine CS et al. 2013. Negative emotions predict elevated interleukin-6 in the United States but not in Japan. Brain Behav. Immun. 34:79–85
    [Google Scholar]
  81. Morling B, Kitayama S, Miyamoto Y. 2002. Cultural practices emphasize influence in the United States and adjustment in Japan. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 28:3311–23
    [Google Scholar]
  82. Morris B. 1991. Western Conceptions of the Individual London: Bloomsbury
  83. Morris I. 2010. Why the West RulesFor Now London: Macmillan
    [Google Scholar]
  84. Morris I. 2022. Geography Is Destiny: Britain and the World, a 10,000 Year History London: Profile Books
  85. Mughogho W, Adhiambo J, Forscher PS. 2023. African researchers must be full participants in behavioural science research. Nat. Hum. Behav. 7:297–99
    [Google Scholar]
  86. Muthukrishna M, Henrich J, Slingerland E. 2021. Psychology as a historical science. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 72:717–49
    [Google Scholar]
  87. Na J, Kitayama S. 2011. Spontaneous trait inference is culture-specific: behavioral and neural evidence. Psychol. Sci. 22:81025–32
    [Google Scholar]
  88. Niedenthal PM, Rychlowska M, Zhao F, Wood A. 2019. Historical migration patterns shape contemporary cultures of emotion. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 14:4560–73
    [Google Scholar]
  89. Nisbett RE, Cohen D. 1996. Culture of Honor: The Psychology of Violence in the South London: Routledge
  90. Nisbett RE, Peng K, Choi I, Norenzayan A. 2001. Culture and systems of thought: holistic versus analytic cognition. Psychol. Rev. 108:2291–310
    [Google Scholar]
  91. Nisbett RE, Ross L. 1980. Human inference: Strategies and Shortcomings of Social Judgment Hoboken, NJ: Prentice Hall
  92. Odling-Smee FJ, Laland KN, Feldman MW 2000. Niche construction and gene-culture coevolution: an evolutionary basis for the human sciences. Perspectives in Ethology: Evolution, Culture, and Behavior F Tonneau, NS Thompson 89–111. New York: Springer
    [Google Scholar]
  93. Ogihara Y, Fujita H, Tominaga H, Ishigaki S, Kashimoto T et al. 2015. Are common names becoming less common? The rise in uniqueness and individualism in Japan. Front. Psychol. 6:1490
    [Google Scholar]
  94. Oyserman D, Lee SWS. 2008. Does culture influence what and how we think? Effects of priming individualism and collectivism. Psychol. Bull. 134:2311–42
    [Google Scholar]
  95. Park J, Kitayama S, Miyamoto Y, Coe CL. 2020. Feeling bad is not always unhealthy: Culture moderates the link between negative affect and diurnal cortisol profiles. Emotion 20:5721–33
    [Google Scholar]
  96. Peng Y, Shi H, Qi X, Xiao C, Zhong H et al. 2010. The ADH1B Arg47His polymorphism in East Asian populations and expansion of rice domestication in history. BMC Evol. Biol. 10:115
    [Google Scholar]
  97. Razavi P, Shaban-Azad H, Srivastava S. 2023. Gheirat as a complex emotional reaction to relational boundary violations: a mixed-methods investigation. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 124:1179–214
    [Google Scholar]
  98. Reich D. 2018. Who We Are and How We Got Here Oxford, UK: Oxford Univ. Press
  99. Rhoads SA, Gunter D, Ryan RM, Marsh AA. 2021. Global variation in subjective well-being predicts seven forms of altruism. Psychol. Sci. 32:81247–61
    [Google Scholar]
  100. Richerson PJ, Boyd R. 2005. Not by Genes Alone: How Culture Transformed Human Evolution Chicago: Univ. Chicago Press
  101. Rozin P, Kabnick K, Pete E, Fischler C, Shields C. 2003. The ecology of eating: Smaller portion sizes in France than in the United States help explain the French paradox. Psychol. Sci. 14:5450–54
    [Google Scholar]
  102. Rozin P, Markwith M, Stoess C. 1997. Moralization and becoming a vegetarian: the transformation of preferences into values and the recruitment of disgust. Psychol. Sci. 8:267–73
    [Google Scholar]
  103. Ruby MB, Falk CF, Heine SJ, Villa C, Silberstein O. 2012. Not all collectivisms are equal: opposing preferences for ideal affect between East Asians and Mexicans. Emotion 12:61206–9
    [Google Scholar]
  104. Sagvolden T, Johansen EB, Aase H, Russell VA. 2005. A dynamic developmental theory of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) predominantly hyperactive/impulsive and combined subtypes. Behav. Brain Sci. 28:3397–419
    [Google Scholar]
  105. Salvador CE, Berg MK, Yu Q, Martin AS, Kitayama S. 2020. Relational mobility predicts faster spread of COVID-19: a 39-country study. Psychol. Sci. 31: 10:1236–44
    [Google Scholar]
  106. Salvador CE, Carlier SI, Ishii K, Castillo CT, Nanakdewa K et al. 2023. Emotionally expressive interdependence in Latin America: triangulating through a comparison of three cultural regions. Emotion In press. https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0001302
  107. Salvador CE, Kamikubo A, Kraus B, Hsiao N-C, Hu J-F et al. 2022. Self-referential processing accounts for cultural variation in self-enhancement versus criticism: an electrocortical investigation. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 151:81904–18
    [Google Scholar]
  108. San Martin A, Sinaceur M, Madi A, Tompson S, Maddux WW, Kitayama S. 2018. Self-assertive interdependence in Arab culture. Nat. Hum. Behav. 2:11830–37
    [Google Scholar]
  109. Santos HC, Varnum MEW, Grossmann I. 2017. Global increases in individualism. Psychol. Sci. 28:91228–39
    [Google Scholar]
  110. Savani K, Morris MW, Naidu NVR, Kumar S, Berlia NV. 2011. Cultural conditioning: understanding interpersonal accommodation in India and the United States in terms of the modal characteristics of interpersonal influence situations. J. Pers. 100:184–102
    [Google Scholar]
  111. Schug J, Yuki M, Maddux W. 2010. Relational mobility explains between- and within-culture differences in self-disclosure to close friends. Psychol. Sci. 21:101471–78
    [Google Scholar]
  112. Schulz JF, Bahrami-Rad D, Beauchamp JP, Henrich J. 2019. The Church, intensive kinship, and global psychological variation. Science 366:6466eaau5141
    [Google Scholar]
  113. Schwartz SH. 1992. Universals in the content and structure of values: theoretical advances and empirical tests in 20 countries. Adv. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 25:1–65
    [Google Scholar]
  114. Schwartz SH. 2006. A theory of cultural value orientations: explication and applications. Comp. Sociol. 5:2–3137–82
    [Google Scholar]
  115. Sedikides C, Gaertner L, Toguchi Y. 2003. Pancultural self-enhancement. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 84:160–79
    [Google Scholar]
  116. Sedikides C, Gaertner L, Vevea JL. 2007. Inclusion of theory-relevant moderators yield the same conclusions as Sedikides, Gaertner, and Vevea 2005: a meta-analytical reply to Heine, Kitayama, and Hamamura 2007. Asian J. Soc. Psychol. 10:259–67
    [Google Scholar]
  117. Sen A. 2013. The Argumentative Indian: Writings on Indian History, Culture and Identity New York: Farrar, Straus and Giroux. , 1st ed..
  118. Senft N, Campos B, Shiota MN, Chentsova-Dutton YE. 2021. Who emphasizes positivity? An exploration of emotion values in people of Latino, Asian, and European heritage living in the United States. Emotion 21:4707–19
    [Google Scholar]
  119. Sng O, Neuberg SL, Varnum MEW, Kenrick DT. 2018. The behavioral ecology of cultural psychological variation. Psychol. Rev. 125:5714–43
    [Google Scholar]
  120. Tajfel H, Turner JC. 2004. The Social Identity Theory of Intergroup Behavior London: Psychol. Press
  121. Talhelm T, English AS. 2020. Historically rice-farming societies have tighter social norms in China and worldwide. PNAS 117:3319816–24
    [Google Scholar]
  122. Talhelm T, Lee C-S, English AS, Wang S. 2023. How rice fights pandemics: nature-crop-human interactions shaped COVID-19 outcomes. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull 49:111567–86
    [Google Scholar]
  123. Talhelm T, Zhang X, Oishi S, Shimin C, Duan D et al. 2014. Large-scale psychological differences within China explained by rice versus wheat agriculture. Science 344:6184603–8
    [Google Scholar]
  124. Taylor C. 1992. Sources of the Self: The Making of the Modern Identity Cambridge, MA: Harvard Univ. Press
  125. Thomas CC, Otis NG, Abraham JR, Markus HR, Walton GM. 2020. Toward a science of delivering aid with dignity: experimental evidence and local forecasts from Kenya. PNAS 117:2715546–53
    [Google Scholar]
  126. Thomson R, Yuki M, Talhelm T, Schug J, Kito M et al. 2018. Relational mobility predicts social behaviors in 39 countries and is tied to historical farming and threat. PNAS 115:297521–26
    [Google Scholar]
  127. Tomasello M. 2019. Becoming Human: A Theory of Ontogeny Cambridge, MA: Harvard Univ. Press
  128. Tooby J, Cosmides L 1992. The psychological foundations of culture. The Adapted Mind: Evolutionary Psychology and the Generation of Culture JH Barkow, L Cosmides, J Tooby 19–136. Oxford, UK: Oxford Univ. Press
    [Google Scholar]
  129. Triandis HC. 1995. Individualism & Collectivism Boulder, CO: Westview Press
  130. Triandis HC, Marin G, Lisansky J, Betancourt H. 1984. Simpatia as a cultural script of Hispanics. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 47:61363–75
    [Google Scholar]
  131. Tsai JL, Knutson B, Fung HH. 2006. Cultural variation in affect valuation. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 90:2288–307
    [Google Scholar]
  132. Tsai JL, Miao FF, Seppala E, Fung HH, Yeung DY. 2007. Influence and adjustment goals: sources of cultural differences in ideal affect. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 92:61102–17
    [Google Scholar]
  133. Uchida Y, Kitayama S. 2009. Happiness and unhappiness in east and west: themes and variations. Emotion 9:4441–56
    [Google Scholar]
  134. Uchida Y, Takemura K, Fukushima S, Saizen I, Kawamura Y et al. 2019. Farming cultivates a community-level shared culture through collective activities: examining contextual effects with multilevel analyses. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 116:11–14
    [Google Scholar]
  135. Uskul AK, Kirchner-Häusler A, Vignoles VL, Rodriguez-Bailón R, Castillo VA et al. 2023. Neither Eastern nor Western: patterns of independence and interdependence in Mediterranean societies. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 125:3471–95
    [Google Scholar]
  136. Uskul AK, Kitayama S, Nisbett RE. 2008. Ecocultural basis of cognition: Farmers and fishermen are more holistic than herders. PNAS 105:258552–56
    [Google Scholar]
  137. Varnum M, Grossmann I, Katunar D, Nisbett R, Kitayama S. 2008. Holism in a European cultural context: differences in cognitive style between Central and East Europeans and Westerners. J. Cogn. Cult. 8:3–4321–33
    [Google Scholar]
  138. Varnum MEW, Grossmann I, Kitayama S, Nisbett RE. 2010. The origin of cultural differences in cognition: the social orientation hypothesis. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 19:19–13
    [Google Scholar]
  139. Vignoles VL, Owe E, Becker M, Smith PB, Easterbrook MJ et al. 2016. Beyond the “east-west” dichotomy: global variation in cultural models of selfhood. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 145:8966–1000
    [Google Scholar]
  140. Vishkin A, Kitayama S, Berg MK, Diener E, Gross-Manos D, Ben-Arieh A, Tamir M. 2023. Adherence to emotion norms is greater in individualist cultures than in collectivist cultures. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 124:61256–76
    [Google Scholar]
  141. Visscher PM. 2008. Sizing up human height variation. Nat. Genet. 40:5489–90
    [Google Scholar]
  142. Vosoughi S, Roy D, Aral S. 2018. The spread of true and false news online. Science 359:63801146–51
    [Google Scholar]
  143. Wang E, Ding Y-C, Flodman P, Kidd JR, Kidd KK et al. 2004. The genetic architecture of selection at the human dopamine receptor D4 (DRD4) gene locus. Am. J. Hum. Genet. 74:5931–44
    [Google Scholar]
  144. Watsuji T. 1988. Climate and Culture transl. Geoffrey Bownas New York: Greenwood Press
  145. Weidman AC, Sowden WJ, Berg MK, Kross E. 2020. Punish or protect? How close relationships shape responses to moral violations. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 46:5693–708
    [Google Scholar]
  146. Wilson DS, Dugatkin LA. 1997. Group selection and assortative interactions. Am. Nat. 149:2336–51
    [Google Scholar]
  147. Yamagishi T, Yamagishi M. 1994. Trust and commitment in the United States and Japan. Motiv. Emot. 18:2129–66
    [Google Scholar]
  148. Yu Q, Abe N, King A, Yoon C, Liberzon I, Kitayama S. 2019. Cultural variation in the gray matter volume of the prefrontal cortex is moderated by the dopamine D4 receptor gene (DRD4). Cereb. Cortex. 29:93922–31
    [Google Scholar]
  149. Yu Q, King AP, Yoon C, Liberzon I, Schaefer SM et al. 2021. Interdependent self-construal predicts increased gray matter volume of scene processing regions in the brain. Biol. Psychol. 161:108050
    [Google Scholar]
  150. Yuki M, Schug J. 2020. Psychological consequences of relational mobility. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 32:129–32
    [Google Scholar]
/content/journals/10.1146/annurev-psych-021723-063333
Loading
/content/journals/10.1146/annurev-psych-021723-063333
Loading

Data & Media loading...

  • Article Type: Review Article
This is a required field
Please enter a valid email address
Approval was a Success
Invalid data
An Error Occurred
Approval was partially successful, following selected items could not be processed due to error