1932

Abstract

Adults routinely make sense of others' actions by inferring the mental states that underlie these actions. Over the past two decades, developmental researchers have made significant advances in understanding the origins of this ability in infancy. This evidence indicates that when infants observe an agent act in a simple scene, they infer the agent's mental states and then use these mental states, together with a principle of rationality (and its corollaries of efficiency and consistency), to predict and interpret the agent's subsequent actions and to guide their own actions toward the agent. In this review, we first describe the initial demonstrations of infants' sensitivity to the efficiency and consistency principles. We then examine how infants identify novel entities as agents. Next, we summarize what is known about infants' ability to reason about agents' motivational, epistemic, and counterfactual states. Finally, we consider alternative interpretations of these findings and discuss the current controversy about the relation between implicit and explicit psychological reasoning.

Loading

Article metrics loading...

/content/journals/10.1146/annurev-psych-010213-115033
2016-01-04
2024-03-29
Loading full text...

Full text loading...

/deliver/fulltext/psych/67/1/annurev-psych-010213-115033.html?itemId=/content/journals/10.1146/annurev-psych-010213-115033&mimeType=html&fmt=ahah

Literature Cited

  1. Amsterlaw J, Wellman HM. 2006. Theories of mind in transition: a microgenetic study of the development of false belief understanding. J. Cogn. Dev. 7:139–72 [Google Scholar]
  2. Apperly IA, Butterfill SA. 2009. Do humans have two systems to track beliefs and belief-like states?. Psychol. Rev. 116:953–70 [Google Scholar]
  3. Arterberry ME, Bornstein MH. 2002. Infant perceptual and conceptual categorization: the roles of static and dynamic stimulus attributes. Cognition 86:1–24 [Google Scholar]
  4. Aschersleben G, Hofer T, Jovanovic B. 2008. The link between infant attention to goal-directed action and later theory of mind abilities. Dev. Sci. 11:862–68 [Google Scholar]
  5. Baillargeon R, He Z, Setoh P, Scott RM, Sloane S, Yang DYJ. 2013. False-belief understanding and why it matters: the social-acting hypothesis. Navigating the Social World: What Infants, Children, and Other Species Can Teach Us MR Banaji, SA Gelman 88–95 New York: Oxford Univ. Press [Google Scholar]
  6. Baillargeon R, Scott RM, He Z. 2010. False-belief understanding in infants. Trends Cogn. Sci. 14:110–18 [Google Scholar]
  7. Baillargeon R, Scott RM, He Z, Sloane S, Setoh P. et al. 2015. Psychological and sociomoral reasoning in infancy. APA Handbook of Personality and Social Psychology: 1 Attitudes and Social Cognition M Mikulincer, PR Shaver, E Borgida, JA Bargh 79–150 Washington, DC: Am. Psychol. Assoc. [Google Scholar]
  8. Baillargeon R, Setoh P, Sloane S, Jin K, Bian L. 2014. Infant social cognition: psychological and sociomoral reasoning. The Cognitive Neurosciences MS Gazzaniga, GR Mangun 7–14 Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 5th ed.. [Google Scholar]
  9. Baillargeon R, Wu D, Yuan S, Li J, Luo Y. 2009. Young infants' expectations about self-propelled objects. The Origins of Object Knowledge B Hood, L Santos 285–352 Oxford, UK: Oxford Univ. Press [Google Scholar]
  10. Barna J, Legerstee M. 2005. Nine- and twelve-month-old infants relate emotions to people's actions. Cogn. Emot. 19:53–67 [Google Scholar]
  11. Baron-Cohen S. 1995. Mind Blindness: An Essay on Autism and Theory of Mind Cambridge, MA: MIT Press/Bradford Books
  12. Baron-Cohen S, Leslie AM, Frith U. 1985. Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind”?. Cognition 21:37–46 [Google Scholar]
  13. Barrett HC, Broesch T, Scott RM, He Z, Baillargeon R. et al. 2013. Early false-belief understanding in traditional non-Western societies. Proc. R. Soc. B 280:20122654 [Google Scholar]
  14. Bartsch K, Wellman H. 1989. Young children's attribution of action to beliefs and desires. Child Dev. 60:946–64 [Google Scholar]
  15. Begus K, Southgate V. 2012. Infant pointing serves an interrogative function. Dev. Sci. 15:611–17 [Google Scholar]
  16. Beier JS, Carey S. 2014. Contingency is not enough: Social context guides third-party attributions of intentional agency. Dev. Psychol. 50:889–902 [Google Scholar]
  17. Bíró S. 2013. The role of the efficiency of novel actions in infants' goal anticipation. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 116:415–27 [Google Scholar]
  18. Bíró S, Csibra G, Gergely G. 2007. The role of behavioral cues in understanding goal-directed action in infancy. Prog. Brain Res. 164:303–22 [Google Scholar]
  19. Bíró S, Verschoor S, Coalter E, Leslie AM. 2014. Outcome producing potential influences twelve-month-olds' interpretation of a novel action as goal-directed. Infant Behav. Dev. 37:729–38 [Google Scholar]
  20. Bíró S, Verschoor S, Coenen L. 2011. Evidence for a unitary goal concept in 12-month-old infants. Dev. Sci. 14:1255–60 [Google Scholar]
  21. Bloom P. 2013. Just Babies: The Origins of Good and Evil New York: Crown
  22. Brandone AC, Horwitz SR, Aslin RN, Wellman HM. 2014. Infants' goal anticipation during failed and successful reaching actions. Dev. Sci. 17:23–34 [Google Scholar]
  23. Brandone AC, Wellman HM. 2009. You can't always get what you want: Infants understand failed goal-directed actions. Psychol. Sci. 20:85–91 [Google Scholar]
  24. Brooker I, Poulin-Dubois D. 2013. Is a bird an apple? The effect of speaker labeling accuracy on infants' word learning, imitation, and helping behaviors. Infancy 18:E46–68 [Google Scholar]
  25. Brown-Schmidt S, Gunlogson C, Tanenhaus MK. 2008. Addressees distinguish shared from private information when interpreting questions during interactive conversation. Cognition 107:1122–34 [Google Scholar]
  26. Brugger A, Lariviere LA, Mumme DL, Bushnell EW. 2007. Doing the right thing: infants' selection of actions to imitate from observed event sequences. Child Dev. 78:806–24 [Google Scholar]
  27. Buresh JS, Woodward AL. 2007. Infants track action goals within and across agents. Cognition 104:287–314 [Google Scholar]
  28. Buttelmann D, Carpenter M, Tomasello M. 2009. Eighteen-month-old infants show false belief understanding in an active helping paradigm. Cognition 112:337–42 [Google Scholar]
  29. Buttelmann D, Over H, Carpenter M, Tomasello M. 2014. Eighteen-month-olds understand false beliefs in an unexpected-contents task. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 119:120–26 [Google Scholar]
  30. Buttelmann F, Suhrke J, Buttelmann D. 2015. What you get is what you believe: Eighteen-month-olds demonstrate belief understanding in an unexpected-identity task. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 131:94–103 [Google Scholar]
  31. Butterfill S, Apperly IA. 2013. How to construct a minimal theory of mind. Mind Lang. 28:606–37 [Google Scholar]
  32. Cannon E, Woodward AL. 2012. Infants generate goal-based action predictions. Dev. Sci. 15:292–98 [Google Scholar]
  33. Carey S. 1985. Conceptual Change in Childhood Cambridge, MA: MIT Press
  34. Carpenter M, Call J, Tomasello M. 2005. Twelve- and 18-month-olds copy actions in terms of goals. Dev. Sci. 8:F13–20 [Google Scholar]
  35. Carpenter M, Nagell K, Tomasello M. 1998. Social cognition, joint attention, and communicative competence from 9 to 15 months of age. Monogr. Soc. Res. Child Dev. 63:1–143 [Google Scholar]
  36. Carruthers P. 2013. Mindreading in infancy. Mind Lang. 28:141–72 [Google Scholar]
  37. Chow V, Poulin-Dubois D, Lewis J. 2008. To see or not to see: Infants prefer to follow the gaze of a reliable looker. Dev. Sci. 11:761–70 [Google Scholar]
  38. Cosmides L, Tooby J. 2013. Evolutionary psychology: new perspectives on cognition and motivation. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 64:201–29 [Google Scholar]
  39. Csibra G. 2008. Goal attribution to inanimate agents by 6.5-month-old infants. Cognition 107:705–17 [Google Scholar]
  40. Csibra G, Bíró S, Koós O, Gergely G. 2003. One-year-old infants use teleological representations of actions productively. Cogn. Sci. 27:111–33 [Google Scholar]
  41. Csibra G, Gergely G. 2009. Natural pedagogy. Trends Cogn. Sci. 13:148–53 [Google Scholar]
  42. Deligianni F, Senju A, Gergely G, Csibra G. 2011. Automated gaze-contingent objects elicit orientation following in 8-months-old infants. Dev. Psychol. 47:1499–503 [Google Scholar]
  43. Dennett DC. 1987. The Intentional Stance Cambridge, MA: MIT Press
  44. Egyed K, Király I, Gergely G. 2013. Communicating shared knowledge in infancy. Psychol. Sci. 24:1348–53 [Google Scholar]
  45. Esseily R, Rat-Fischer L, O'Regan K, Fagard J. 2013. Understanding the experimenter's intention improves 16-month-olds' observational learning of the use of a novel tool. Cogn. Dev. 28:1–9 [Google Scholar]
  46. Gergely G, Bekkering H, Király I. 2002. Rational imitation in preverbal infants. Nature 415:755 [Google Scholar]
  47. Gergely G, Csibra G. 2003. Teleological reasoning in infancy: the naıve theory of rational action. Trends Cogn. Sci. 7:287–92 [Google Scholar]
  48. Gergely G, Nádasdy Z, Csibra G, Bíró S. 1995. Taking the intentional stance at 12 months of age. Cognition 56:165–93 [Google Scholar]
  49. German TC, Cohen AS. 2012. A cue-based approach to “theory of mind”: re-examining the notion of automaticity. Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 30:45–58 [Google Scholar]
  50. Gerson SA, Woodward AL. 2012. A claw is like my hand: Comparison supports goal analysis in infants. Cognition 122:181–92 [Google Scholar]
  51. Gerson SA, Woodward AL. 2013. The goal trumps the means: Highlighting goals is more beneficial than highlighting means in means-end training. Infancy 18:289–302 [Google Scholar]
  52. Gerson SA, Woodward AL. 2014. Learning from their own actions: the unique effect of producing actions on infants' action understanding. Child Dev. 85:264–77 [Google Scholar]
  53. Gopnik A, Astington JW. 1988. Children's understanding of representational change and its relation to the understanding of false belief and the appearance-reality distinction. Child Dev. 59:26–37 [Google Scholar]
  54. Gopnik A, Wellman HM. 1994. The theory theory. See Hirschfeld & Gelman 1994 257–93
  55. Gweon H, Tenenbaum JB, Schulz LE. 2010. Infants consider both the sample and the sampling process in inductive generalization. PNAS 107:9066–71 [Google Scholar]
  56. Hamlin JK. 2013. Moral judgment and action in preverbal infants and toddlers: evidence for an innate moral core. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 22:186–93 [Google Scholar]
  57. Hamlin JK, Wynn K. 2011. Young infants prefer prosocial to antisocial others. Cogn. Dev. 26:30–39 [Google Scholar]
  58. Hamlin JK, Wynn K, Bloom P. 2007. Social evaluation by preverbal infants. Nature 450:557–59 [Google Scholar]
  59. Hamlin JK, Wynn K, Bloom P. 2010. Three-month-olds show a negativity bias in their social evaluations. Dev. Sci. 13:923–29 [Google Scholar]
  60. He Z, Bolz M, Baillargeon R. 2011. False-belief understanding in 2.5-year-olds: evidence from violation-of-expectation change-of-location and unexpected-contents tasks. Dev. Sci. 14:292–305 [Google Scholar]
  61. He Z, Bolz M, Baillargeon R. 2012. 2.5-year-olds succeed at a verbal anticipatory-looking false-belief task. Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 30:14–29 [Google Scholar]
  62. Henderson AME, Woodward AL. 2012. Nine-month-old infants generalize object labels, but not object preferences across individuals. Dev. Sci. 15:641–52 [Google Scholar]
  63. Henrichs I, Elsner C, Elsner B, Wilkinson N, Gredebäck G. 2014. Goal certainty modulates infants' goal-directed gaze shifts. Dev. Psychol. 50:100–7 [Google Scholar]
  64. Hepach R, Westermann G. 2013. Infants' sensitivity to the congruence of others' emotions and actions. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 115:16–29 [Google Scholar]
  65. Hernik M, Southgate V. 2012. Nine-month-old infants do not need to know what the agent prefers in order to reason about its goals: on the role of preference and persistence in infants' goal-attribution. Dev. Sci. 15:714–22 [Google Scholar]
  66. Heyes C. 2014. False belief in infancy: a fresh look. Dev. Sci. 17:647–59 [Google Scholar]
  67. Hirschfeld LA, Gelman SA. 1994. Mapping the Mind: Domain Specificity in Cognition New York: Cambridge Univ. Press
  68. Hoicka E, Wang S. 2011. Fifteen-month-old infants match vocal cues to intentional actions. J. Cogn. Dev. 12:299–314 [Google Scholar]
  69. Hunnius S, Bekkering H. 2010. The early development of object knowledge: a study of infants' visual anticipations during action observation. Dev. Psychol. 46:446–54 [Google Scholar]
  70. Hyde DC, Aparicio Betancourt M, Simon CE. 2015. Human temporal-parietal junction automatically tracks other's beliefs: an fNIRS study. Hum. Brain Mapp. In press
  71. Jacob P. 2015. A puzzle about belief-ascription. Mind and Society: Cognitive Science Meets the Social Sciences B Kaldis Berlin: Springer In press [Google Scholar]
  72. Johnson SC. 2005. Reasoning about intentionality in preverbal infants. The Innate Mind: 1 Structure and Contents P Carruthers, S Laurence, S Stich 254–71 Oxford, UK: Oxford Univ. Press [Google Scholar]
  73. Johnson SC, Bolz M, Carter E, Mandsanger J, Teichner A, Zettler P. 2008. Calculating the attentional orientation of an unfamiliar agent in infancy. Cogn. Dev. 23:24–37 [Google Scholar]
  74. Johnson SC, Booth A, O'Hearn K. 2001. Inferring the goals of a nonhuman agent. Cogn. Dev. 16:637–56 [Google Scholar]
  75. Johnson SC, Dweck CS, Chen FS, Stern HL, Ok S-J, Barth ME. 2010. At the intersection of social and cognitive development: internal working models of attachment in infancy. Cogn. Sci. 34:807–25 [Google Scholar]
  76. Johnson SC, Ok S-J, Luo Y. 2007a. The attribution of attention: nine-month-olds' interpretation of gaze as goal-directed action. Dev. Sci. 10:530–37 [Google Scholar]
  77. Johnson SC, Shimizu YA, Ok S-J. 2007b. Actors and actions: the role of agent behavior in infants' attribution of goals. Cogn. Dev. 22:310–22 [Google Scholar]
  78. Kamewari K, Kato M, Kanda T, Ishiguro H, Hiraki K. 2005. Six-and-a-half-month-old children positively attribute goals to human action and to humanoid-robot motion. Cogn. Dev. 20:303–20 [Google Scholar]
  79. Kanakogi Y, Itakura S. 2011. Developmental correspondence between action prediction and motor ability in early infancy. Nat. Commun. 2:341 [Google Scholar]
  80. Keil FC. 1995. The growth of causal understandings of natural kinds: modes of construal and the emergence of biological thought. Causal Cognition A Premack, D Sperber 234–67 Oxford, UK: Oxford Univ. Press [Google Scholar]
  81. Kim E, Song H. 2015. Six-month-olds actively predict others' goal-directed actions. Cogn. Dev. 33:1–13 [Google Scholar]
  82. Király I, Csibra G, Gergely G. 2013. Beyond rational imitation: learning arbitrary means actions from communicative demonstrations. J. Exp. Child Psychol 116:471–86 [Google Scholar]
  83. Király I, Jovanovic B, Prinz W, Aschersleben G, Gergely G. 2003. The early origins of goal attribution in infancy. Conscious. Cogn. 12:752–69 [Google Scholar]
  84. Knudsen B, Liszkowski U. 2012a. Eighteen- and 24-month-old infants correct others in anticipation of action mistakes. Dev. Sci. 15:113–22 [Google Scholar]
  85. Knudsen B, Liszkowski U. 2012b. 18-month-olds predict specific action mistakes through attribution of false belief, not ignorance, and intervene accordingly. Infancy 17:672–91 [Google Scholar]
  86. Koenig MA, Woodward AL. 2010. Sensitivity of 24-month-olds to the prior inaccuracy of the source: possible mechanisms. Dev. Psychol. 46:815–26 [Google Scholar]
  87. Kovács ÁM, Kuehn S, Gergely G, Csibra G, Brass M. 2014a. Are all beliefs equal? Implicit belief attributions recruiting core brain regions of theory of mind. PLOS ONE 9:e106558 [Google Scholar]
  88. Kovács ÁM, Tauzin T, Téglás E, Gergely G, Csibra G. 2014b. Pointing as epistemic request: 12-month-olds point to receive new information. Infancy 19:543–57 [Google Scholar]
  89. Kovács ÁM, Téglás E, Endress AD. 2010. The social sense: susceptibility to others' beliefs in human infants and adults. Science 330:1830–34 [Google Scholar]
  90. Krogh-Jespersen S, Echols CH. 2012. The influence of speaker reliability on first versus second label learning. Child Dev. 83:581–90 [Google Scholar]
  91. Kuhlmeier V, Wynn K, Bloom P. 2003. Attribution of dispositional states by 12-month-olds. Psychol. Sci. 14:402–8 [Google Scholar]
  92. Kushnir T, Xu F, Wellman HM. 2010. Young children use statistical sampling to infer the preferences of other people. Psychol. Sci. 21:1134–40 [Google Scholar]
  93. Leslie AM. 1994. ToMM, ToBy, and agency: core architecture and domain specificity. See Hirschfeld & Gelman 1994 119–48
  94. Leslie AM. 2000. How to acquire a “representational theory of mind.”. Metarepresentations: A Multidisciplinary Perspective D Sperber 197–223 Oxford, UK: Oxford Univ. Press [Google Scholar]
  95. Liszkowski U, Carpenter M, Striano T, Tomasello M. 2006. Twelve- and 18-month-olds point to provide information for others. J. Cogn. Dev. 7:173–87 [Google Scholar]
  96. Liszkowski U, Carpenter M, Tomasello M. 2008. Twelve-month-olds communicate helpfully and appropriately for knowledgeable and ignorant partners. Cognition 108:732–39 [Google Scholar]
  97. LoBue V, Bloom Pickard M, Sherman K, Axford C, DeLoache JS. 2013. Young children's interest in live animals. Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 31:57–69 [Google Scholar]
  98. Low J, Drummond W, Walmsley A, Wang B. 2014. Representing how rabbits quack and competitors act: limits on preschoolers' efficient ability to track perspectives. Child Dev. 85:1519–34 [Google Scholar]
  99. Low J, Watts J. 2013. Attributing false beliefs about object identity reveals a signature blind spot in humans' efficient mind-reading system. Psychol. Sci. 24:305–11 [Google Scholar]
  100. Luo Y. 2010. Do 8-month-old infants consider situational constraints when interpreting others' gaze as goal-directed action?. Infancy 15:392–419 [Google Scholar]
  101. Luo Y. 2011a. Do 10-month-old infants understand others' false beliefs?. Cognition 121:289–98 [Google Scholar]
  102. Luo Y. 2011b. Three-month-old infants attribute goals to a non-human agent. Dev. Sci. 14:453–60 [Google Scholar]
  103. Luo Y, Baillargeon R. 2005a. Can a self-propelled box have a goal? Psychological reasoning in 5-month-old infants. Psychol. Sci. 16:601–8 [Google Scholar]
  104. Luo Y, Baillargeon R. 2005b. When the ordinary seems unexpected: evidence for incremental physical knowledge in young infants. Cognition 95:297–328 [Google Scholar]
  105. Luo Y, Baillargeon R. 2007. Do 12.5-month-old infants consider what objects others can see when interpreting their actions?. Cognition 105:489–512 [Google Scholar]
  106. Luo Y, Beck W. 2010. Do you see what I see? Infants' reasoning about others' incomplete perceptions. Dev. Sci. 13:134–42 [Google Scholar]
  107. Luo Y, Johnson SC. 2009. Recognizing the role of perception in action at 6 months. Dev. Sci. 12:142–49 [Google Scholar]
  108. Luo Y, Kaufman L, Baillargeon R. 2009. Young infants' reasoning about events involving inert and self-propelled objects. Cogn. Psychol. 58:441–86 [Google Scholar]
  109. Martin A, Onishi KH, Vouloumanos A. 2012. Understanding the abstract role of speech in communication at 12 months. Cognition 123:50–60 [Google Scholar]
  110. Meltzoff AN. 1988. Infant imitation after a 1-week delay: long-term memory for novel acts and multiple stimuli. Dev. Psychol. 24:470–76 [Google Scholar]
  111. Meltzoff AN. 1995. Understanding the intentions of others: re-enactment of intended acts by 18-month-old children. Dev. Psychol. 31:838–50 [Google Scholar]
  112. Meristo M, Morgan G, Geraci A, Iozzi L, Hjelmquist E. et al. 2012. Belief attribution in deaf and hearing infants. Dev. Sci. 15:633–40 [Google Scholar]
  113. Meristo M, Surian L. 2013. Do infants detect indirect reciprocity?. Cognition 129:102–13 [Google Scholar]
  114. Moll H, Tomasello M. 2004. 12- and 18-month-old infants follow gaze to spaces behind barriers. Dev. Sci. 7:F1–9 [Google Scholar]
  115. Mou Y, Province JM, Luo Y. 2014. Can infants make transitive inferences?. Cogn. Psychol. 68:98–112 [Google Scholar]
  116. Needham A, Barrett T, Peterman K. 2002. A pick-me-up for infants' exploratory skills: early simulated experiences reaching for objects using “sticky mittens” enhances young infants' object exploration skills. Infant Behav. Dev. 25:279–95 [Google Scholar]
  117. Newman GE, Herrmann P, Wynn K, Keil FC. 2008. Biases towards internal features in infants' reasoning about objects. Cognition 107:420–32 [Google Scholar]
  118. Olineck KM, Poulin-Dubois D. 2005. Infants' ability to distinguish between intentional and accidental actions and its relation to internal state language. Infancy 8:91–100 [Google Scholar]
  119. Olineck KM, Poulin-Dubois D. 2009. Infants' understanding of intention from 10 to 14 months: interrelations among violation of expectancy and imitation tasks. Infant Behav. Dev. 32:404–15 [Google Scholar]
  120. Onishi KH, Baillargeon R. 2005. Do 15-month-old infants understand false beliefs?. Science 308:255–58 [Google Scholar]
  121. Paulus M, Hunnius S, Vissers M, Bekkering H. 2011. Imitation in infancy: rational or motor resonance?. Child Dev. 82:1047–57 [Google Scholar]
  122. Perner J. 2010. Who took the cog out of cognitive science? Mentalism in an era of anti-cognitivism. Cognition and Neuropsychology: International Perspectives on Psychological Science PA Frensch, R Schwarzer 241–61 Hove, UK: Psychol. Press [Google Scholar]
  123. Pinkham AM, Jaswal VK. 2011. Watch and learn? Infants privilege efficiency over pedagogy during imitative learning. Infancy 16:535–44 [Google Scholar]
  124. Poulin-Dubois D, Brooker I, Polonia A. 2011. Infants prefer to imitate a reliable person. Infant Behav. Dev. 34:303–9 [Google Scholar]
  125. Poulin-Dubois D, Chow V. 2009. The effect of looker's past reliability on infants' reasoning about beliefs. Dev. Psychol. 45:1576–82 [Google Scholar]
  126. Premack D. 1990. The infant's theory of self-propelled objects. Cognition 36:1–16 [Google Scholar]
  127. Premack D, Premack AJ. 1995. Origins of human social competence. The Cognitive Neurosciences MS Gazzaniga 205–18 Cambridge, MA: MIT Press [Google Scholar]
  128. Premack D, Premack AJ. 1997. Infants attribute value +/− to the goal-directed actions of self-propelled objects. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 9:848–56 [Google Scholar]
  129. Repacholi BM, Meltzoff AN, Olsen B. 2008. Infants' understanding of the link between visual perception and emotion: “If she can't see me doing it, she won't get angry.”. Dev. Psychol. 44:561–74 [Google Scholar]
  130. Robson SJ, Kuhlmeier VA. 2013. Selectivity promotes 9-month-old infants to encode the goals of others. Paper presented at Bienn. Meet. Soc. Res. Child Dev., April, Seattle, WA
  131. Ruffman T. 2014. To belief or not belief: children's theory of mind. Dev. Rev. 34:265–93 [Google Scholar]
  132. Schaafsma SM, Pfaff DW, Spunt RP, Adolphs R. 2015. Deconstructing and reconstructing theory of mind. Trends Cogn. Sci. 19:65–72 [Google Scholar]
  133. Schlottmann A, Ray ED. 2010. Goal attribution to schematic animals: Do 6-month-olds perceive biological motion as animate?. Dev. Sci. 13:1–10 [Google Scholar]
  134. Schlottmann A, Ray ED, Surian L. 2012. Emerging perception of causality in action-and-reaction sequences from 4 to 6 months of age: Is it domain-specific?. J. Exp. Child Psychol 112:208–30 [Google Scholar]
  135. Schwier C, van Maanen C, Carpenter M, Tomasello M. 2006. Rational imitation in 12-month-old infants. Infancy 10:303–11 [Google Scholar]
  136. Scott RM. 2014. Post hoc versus predictive accounts of children's theory of mind: a reply to Ruffman. Dev. Rev. 34:300–4 [Google Scholar]
  137. Scott RM, Baillargeon R. 2009. Which penguin is this? Attributing false beliefs about object identity at 18 months. Child Dev. 80:1172–96 [Google Scholar]
  138. Scott RM, Baillargeon R. 2013. Do infants really expect others to act efficiently? A critical test of the rationality principle. Psychol. Sci. 24:466–74 [Google Scholar]
  139. Scott RM, Baillargeon R, Song H, Leslie A. 2010. Attributing false beliefs about non-obvious properties at 18 months. Cogn. Psychol. 61:366–95 [Google Scholar]
  140. Scott RM, He Z, Baillargeon R, Cummins D. 2012. False-belief understanding in 2.5-year-olds: evidence from two novel verbal spontaneous-response tasks. Dev. Sci. 15:181–93 [Google Scholar]
  141. Scott RM, Richman JC, Baillargeon R. 2015a. Infants understand deceptive intentions to implant false beliefs about identity: new evidence for early mentalistic reasoning. Cogn. Psychol. 8232–56
  142. Scott RM, Roby E, Smith M. 2015b. False-belief understanding in the first years of life. . In The Routledge Handbook of the Philosophy of the Social Mind, ed. J Kiverstein Oxford, UK: Routledge In press [Google Scholar]
  143. Senju A, Southgate V, Snape C, Leonard M, Csibra G. 2011. Do 18-month-olds really attribute mental states to others? A critical test. Psychol. Sci. 22:878–80 [Google Scholar]
  144. Senju A, Southgate V, White S, Frith U. 2009. Mindblind eyes: an absence of spontaneous theory of mind in Asperger syndrome. Science 325:883–85 [Google Scholar]
  145. Setoh P, Wu D, Baillargeon R, Gelman R. 2013. Young infants have biological expectations about animals. PNAS 110:15937–42 [Google Scholar]
  146. Shimizu YA, Johnson SC. 2004. Infants' attribution of a goal to a morphologically unfamiliar agent. Dev. Sci. 7:425–30 [Google Scholar]
  147. Skerry AE, Carey SE, Spelke ES. 2013. First-person action experience reveals sensitivity to action efficiency in prereaching infants. PNAS 110:18728–33 [Google Scholar]
  148. Skerry AE, Spelke ES. 2014. Preverbal infants identify emotional reactions that are incongruent with goal outcomes. Cognition 130:204–16 [Google Scholar]
  149. Sloane S, Baillargeon R, Premack D. 2012. Do infants have a sense of fairness?. Psychol. Sci. 23:196–204 [Google Scholar]
  150. Sodian B, Kristen SE, Licata M. 2015. The longitudinal relation of implicit and explicit false belief understanding. Presented at Bienn. Meet. Soc. Res. Child Dev., Philadelphia, PA
  151. Sodian B, Schoeppner B, Metz U. 2004. Do infants apply the principle of rational action to human agents?. Infant Behav. Dev. 27:31–41 [Google Scholar]
  152. Sommerville JA, Woodward AL, Needham A. 2005. Action experience alters 3-month-old infants' perception of others' actions. Cognition 96:B1–11 [Google Scholar]
  153. Song H, Baillargeon R. 2007. Can 9.5-month-old infants attribute to an agent a disposition to perform a particular action on objects?. Acta Psychol. 124:79–105 [Google Scholar]
  154. Song H, Baillargeon R. 2008. Infants' reasoning about others' false perceptions. Dev. Psychol. 44:1789–95 [Google Scholar]
  155. Song H, Baillargeon R, Fisher C. 2005. Can infants attribute to an agent a disposition to perform a particular action?. Cognition 98:B45–55 [Google Scholar]
  156. Song H, Baillargeon R, Fisher C. 2014. The development of infants' use of novel verbal information when reasoning about others' actions. PLOS ONE 9:e92387 [Google Scholar]
  157. Song H, Onishi KH, Baillargeon R, Fisher C. 2008. Can an agent's false belief be corrected by an appropriate communication? Psychological reasoning in 18-month-old infants. Cognition 109:295–315 [Google Scholar]
  158. Southgate V, Begus K. 2013. Motor activation during the prediction of nonexecutable actions in infants. Psychol. Sci. 24:828–35 [Google Scholar]
  159. Southgate V, Chevallier C, Csibra G. 2010. Seventeen-month-olds appeal to false beliefs to interpret others' referential communication. Dev. Sci. 16:907–12 [Google Scholar]
  160. Southgate V, Johnson MH, Csibra G. 2008. Infants attribute goals even to biomechanically impossible actions. Cognition 107:1059–69 [Google Scholar]
  161. Southgate V, Senju A, Csibra G. 2007. Action anticipation through attribution of false belief by two-year-olds. Psychol. Sci. 18:587–92 [Google Scholar]
  162. Southgate V, Vernetti A. 2014. Belief-based action prediction in preverbal infants. Cognition 130:1–10 [Google Scholar]
  163. Spaepen E, Spelke E. 2007. Will any doll do? 12-month-olds' reasoning about goal objects. Cogn. Psychol. 54:133–54 [Google Scholar]
  164. Spelke ES, Kinzler KD. 2007. Core knowledge. Dev. Sci. 10:89–96 [Google Scholar]
  165. Stahl AE, Feigenson L. 2015. Observing the unexpected enhances infants' learning and exploration. Science 348:91–94 [Google Scholar]
  166. Surian L, Caldi S, Sperber D. 2007. Attribution of beliefs by 13-month-old infants. Psychol. Sci. 18:580–86 [Google Scholar]
  167. Surian L, Geraci A. 2012. Where will the triangle look for it? Attributing false beliefs to geometric shapes at 17 months. Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 30:30–44 [Google Scholar]
  168. Thoermer C, Sodian B, Vuori M, Perst H, Kristen S. 2012. Continuity from an implicit to an explicit understanding of false belief from infancy to preschool age. Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 30:172–87 [Google Scholar]
  169. Tomasello M, Haberl K. 2003. Understanding attention: 12- and 18-month-olds know what is new for other persons. Dev. Psychol. 39:906–12 [Google Scholar]
  170. Träuble B, Marinović V, Pauen S. 2010. Early theory of mind competencies: Do infants understand others' beliefs?. Infancy 15:434–44 [Google Scholar]
  171. Träuble B, Pauen S. 2011. Cause or effect: What matters? How 12-month-old infants learn to categorize artifacts. Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 29:357–74 [Google Scholar]
  172. Vierkant T. 2012. Self-knowledge and knowing other minds: the implicit/explicit distinction as a tool in understanding theory of mind. Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 30:141–55 [Google Scholar]
  173. Vosniadou S, Brewer WF. 1992. Mental models of the earth: a study of conceptual change in childhood. Cogn. Psychol. 24:535–85 [Google Scholar]
  174. Wang S, Baillargeon R, Brueckner L. 2004. Young infants' reasoning about hidden objects: evidence from violation-of-expectation tasks with test trials only. Cognition 93:167–98 [Google Scholar]
  175. Wellman HM, Cross D, Watson J. 2001. Meta-analysis of theory of mind development: the truth about false belief. Child Dev. 72:655–84 [Google Scholar]
  176. Wellman HM, Lopez-Duran S, LaBounty J, Hamilton B. 2008. Infant attention to intentional action predicts preschool theory of mind. Dev. Psychol. 44:618–32 [Google Scholar]
  177. Wimmer H, Perner J. 1983. Beliefs about beliefs: representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children's understanding of deception. Cognition 13:103–28 [Google Scholar]
  178. Woodward AL. 1998. Infants selectively encode the goal object of an actor's reach. Cognition 69:1–34 [Google Scholar]
  179. Woodward AL. 1999. Infants' ability to distinguish between purposeful and non-purposeful behaviors. Infant Behav. Dev. 22:145–60 [Google Scholar]
  180. Woodward AL. 2003. Infants' developing understanding of the link between looker and object. Dev. Sci. 6:297–311 [Google Scholar]
  181. Woodward AL, Sommerville JA. 2000. Twelve-month-old infants interpret action in context. Psychol. Sci. 11:73–77 [Google Scholar]
  182. Yamaguchi M, Kuhlmeier VA, Wynn K, VanMarle K. 2009. Continuity in social cognition from infancy to childhood. Dev. Sci. 12:746–52 [Google Scholar]
  183. Yoon J, Johnson SC. 2009. Biological motion displays elicit social behavior in 12-month-olds. Child Dev. 80:1069–75 [Google Scholar]
  184. Zmyj N, Buttelmann D, Carpenter M, Daum MM. 2010. The reliability of a model influences 14-month-olds' imitation. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 106:208–20 [Google Scholar]
/content/journals/10.1146/annurev-psych-010213-115033
Loading
  • Article Type: Review Article
This is a required field
Please enter a valid email address
Approval was a Success
Invalid data
An Error Occurred
Approval was partially successful, following selected items could not be processed due to error