1932

Abstract

To build a science of the person, the most basic question was, and remains, how can one identify and understand the psychological invariance that distinctively characterizes an individual and that underlies the variations in the thoughts, feelings, and actions that occur across contexts and over time? This question proved particularly difficult because of the discrepancies that soon emerged between the expressions of consistency that were expected and those that were found. The resulting dilemma became known as the classic “personality paradox”: How can we reconcile our intuitions—and theories—about the invariance and stability of personality with the equally compelling empirical evidence for the variability of the person's behavior across diverse situations? Which is right: the intuitions or the research findings? In this chapter I review and discuss some of the advances made to answer this question since it was posed. These findings have allowed a resolution of the paradox, and provide the outlines for a conception of the underlying structure and dynamics of personality that seems to better account for the data.

Loading

Article metrics loading...

/content/journals/10.1146/annurev.psych.55.042902.130709
2004-02-04
2024-03-29
Loading full text...

Full text loading...

/deliver/fulltext/ps/55/1/annurev.psych.55.042902.130709.html?itemId=/content/journals/10.1146/annurev.psych.55.042902.130709&mimeType=html&fmt=ahah

Literature Cited

  1. Allport GW. 1937. Personality: A Psychological Interpretation New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston
  2. Allport GW. 1961. Pattern and Growth in Personality New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston
  3. Andersen SM, Chen S. 2002. The relational self: an interpersonal social-cognitive theory. Psychol. Rev. 109:619–45 [Google Scholar]
  4. Anderson JR, Lebiere C. 1998. The Atomic Components of Thought Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum
  5. Ayduk O, Downey G, Testa A, Yen Y, Shoda Y. 1999. Does rejection sensitivity elicit hostility in rejection-sensitive women?. Soc. Cogn. 17:245–71 [Google Scholar]
  6. Ayduk O, Mendoza-Denton R, Mischel W, Downey G, Peake PK, Rodriguez M. 2000. Regulating the interpersonal self: strategic self-regulation for coping with rejection sensitivity. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 79:776–92 [Google Scholar]
  7. Ayduk O, Mischel W, Downey G. 2002. Attentional mechanisms linking rejection to hostile reactivity: the role of “hot” vs. “cool” focus. Psychol. Sci. 13:443–48 [Google Scholar]
  8. Baumeister RF, Vohs KD. eds 2004. Handbook of Self-Regulation Research New York: Guilford
  9. Bem DJ. 1983. Constructing a theory of triple typology: some (second) thoughts on nomothetic and idiographic approaches to personality. J. Personal. 51:566–77 [Google Scholar]
  10. Cantor N, Kihlstrom JF. 1987. Personality and Social Intelligence Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Erlbaum
  11. Cantor N, Mischel W, Schwartz J. 1982. A prototype analysis of psychological situations. Cogn. Psychol. 14:45–77 [Google Scholar]
  12. Cervone D. 2003. The architecture of personality. Psychol. Rev. In press [Google Scholar]
  13. Cervone D, Mischel W. 2002. Personality science. In Advances in Personality Science ed. D Cervone, W Mischel pp. 1–26 New York: Guilford [Google Scholar]
  14. Cervone D, Shoda Y. 1999. Social cognitive theories and the coherence of personality. In The Coherence of Personality: Social-Cognitive Bases of Consistency, Variability, and Organization ed. D Cervone, Y Shoda pp. 155–81 New York: Guilford [Google Scholar]
  15. Chen S. 2003. Psychological state theories about significant others: Implications for the content and structure of significant-other representations. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. In press [Google Scholar]
  16. Chen-Idson L, Mischel W. 2001. The personality of familiar and significant people: the lay perceiver as a social cognitive theorist. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 80:585–96 [Google Scholar]
  17. Cheng C. 2001. Assessing coping flexibility in real-life and laboratory settings: a multimethod approach. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 80:814–33 [Google Scholar]
  18. Cheng C. 2003. Cognitive and motivational processes underlying coping flexibility: a dual-process model. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 84:425–38 [Google Scholar]
  19. Chiu C, Hong Y, Mischel W, Shoda Y. 1995. Discriminative facility in social competence: conditional versus dispositional encoding and monitoring-blunting of information. Soc. Cogn. 13:49–70 [Google Scholar]
  20. Cronbach LJ. 1957. The two disciplines of scientific psychology. Am. Psychol. 12:671–84 [Google Scholar]
  21. Derryberry D. 2002. Attention and voluntary self-control. Self Identity 1:105–11 [Google Scholar]
  22. Downey G, Feldman S, Ayduk O. 2000. Rejection sensitivity and male violence in romantic relationships. Pers. Relat. 7:45–61 [Google Scholar]
  23. Epstein S. 1979. The stability of behavior: I. on predicting most of the people much of the time. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 37:1097–126 [Google Scholar]
  24. Epstein S. 1980. The stability of behavior: II. implications for psychological research. Am. Psychol. 35:790–806 [Google Scholar]
  25. Feldman SI, Downey G. 1994. Rejection sensitivity as a mediator of the impact of childhood exposure to family violence on adult attachment behavior. Dev. Psychopathol. 6:231–47 [Google Scholar]
  26. Fleeson W. 2001. Toward a structure- and process-integrated view of personality: traits as density distribution of states. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 80:1011–27 [Google Scholar]
  27. Hartshorne H, May A. 1928. Studies in the Nature of Character: Studies in Deceit. New York: Macmillan [Google Scholar]
  28. Higgins ET. ed. LA Pervin, 1990. Personality, social psychology, and person-situation relations: standards and knowledge activation as a common language. In Handbook of Personality: Theory and Research pp. 301–38 New York: Guilford [Google Scholar]
  29. Higgins ET. 1996. Ideals, oughts, & regulatory focus: affect and motivation from distinct pains and pleasures. In The Psychology of Action: Linking Cognition and Motivation to Behavior ed. PM Gollwitzer, JA Bargh pp. 91–114 New York: Guilford [Google Scholar]
  30. Kagan J. 2003. Biology, context, and developmental inquiry. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 54:1–23 [Google Scholar]
  31. Kashima Y, Kerekes ARZ. 1994. A distributed memory model of averaging phenomena in personal impression formation. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 30:407–55 [Google Scholar]
  32. Kelley HH, Holmes JG, Kerr NL, Reis HT, Rusbult CE, Van Lange PAM. 2003. An Atlas of Interpersonal Situations New York: Cambridge Univ. Press
  33. Kelly GA. 1955. The Psychology of Personal Constructs New York: Norton
  34. Krahe B. 1990. Situation Cognition and Coherence in Personality: An Individual-Centered Approach Cambridge, UK: Cambridge Univ. Press
  35. Kunda Z. 1999. Social Cognition: Making Sense of People Cambridge, MA: MIT Press
  36. Kunda Z, Thagard P. 1996. Forming impressions from stereotypes, traits, and behaviors: a parallel-constraint-satisfaction theory. Psychol. Rev. 103:284–308 [Google Scholar]
  37. LeDoux J. 1996. The Emotional Brain New York: Simon & Schuster
  38. LeeTiernan S. 2002. Modeling and predicting stable response variation across situations Unpublished doc. diss. thesis. Univ. Wash. Seattle:
  39. Lewin K. 1946. Behavior and development as a function of the total situation. In Manual of Child Psychology ed. L Carmichael pp. 791–802 New York: Wiley [Google Scholar]
  40. Magnusson D. 2000. The individual as the organizing principle in psychological inquiry. In Developmental Sciences and the Holistic Approach ed. LR Bergman, RB Cairns, LG Nilsson, L Nystedt pp. 33–47 Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum [Google Scholar]
  41. Magnusson D, Endler NS. 1977. Interactional psychology: present status and future prospects. In Personality at the Crossroads: Current Issues in Interactional Psychology ed. D Magnusson, NS Endler pp. 3–31 Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum [Google Scholar]
  42. Mendoza-Denton R, Ayduk O, Mischel W, Shoda Y, Testa A. 2001. Person × situation interactionism in self-encoding (I am … when …): implications for affect regulation and social information processing. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 80:533–44 [Google Scholar]
  43. Mendoza-Denton R, Downey G, Purdie VJ, Davis A, Pietrzak J. 2002. Sensitivity to status-based rejection: implications for African-American students' college experience. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 83:896–918 [Google Scholar]
  44. Metcalfe J, Mischel W. 1999. A hot/cool system analysis of delay of gratification: dynamics of willpower. Psychol. Rev. 106:3–19 [Google Scholar]
  45. Miller GA. 1999. On knowing a word. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 50:1–19 [Google Scholar]
  46. Mischel W. 1968. Personality and Assessment New York: Wiley
  47. Mischel W. 1973. Toward a cognitive social learning reconceptualization of personality. Psychol. Rev. 80:252–83 [Google Scholar]
  48. Mischel W, Morf CC. 2003. The self as a psycho-social dynamic processing system: a meta-perspective on a century of the self in psychology. In Handbook of Self and Identity ed. M Leary, J Tangney pp. 15–43 New York: Guilford [Google Scholar]
  49. Mischel W, Peake PK. 1982. In search of consistency: measure for measure. In Consistency in Social Behavior: The Ontario Symposium ed. MP Zanna, ET Higgins, CP Herman pp. 187–207 Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum [Google Scholar]
  50. Mischel W, Shoda Y. 1995. A cognitive-affective system theory of personality: reconceptualizing situations, dispositions, dynamics, and invariance in personality structure. Psychol. Rev. 102:246–68 [Google Scholar]
  51. Mischel W, Shoda Y. 1998. Reconciling processing dynamics and personality dispositions. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 49:229–58 [Google Scholar]
  52. Mischel W, Shoda Y, Mendoza-Denton R. 2002. Situation-behavior profiles as a locus of consistency in personality. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 11:50–54 [Google Scholar]
  53. Mischel W, Shoda Y, Rodriguez M. 1989. Delay of gratification in children. Science 244:933–38 [Google Scholar]
  54. Mischel W, Shoda Y, Smith RE. 2004. Introduction to Personality: Toward an Integration New York: Wiley
  55. Morf CC, Rhodewalt F. 2001. Expanding the dynamic self-regulatory processing model of narcissism: research directions for the future. Psychol. Inq. 12:243–51 [Google Scholar]
  56. Moskowitz DS. 1982. Coherence and cross-situational generality in personality: a new analysis of old problems. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 43:754–68 [Google Scholar]
  57. Moskowitz DS. 1994. Cross-situational generality and the interpersonal circumplex. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 66:921–33 [Google Scholar]
  58. Newcombe TM. 1929. Consistency of Certain Extrovert-Introvert Behavior Patterns in 51 Problem Boys New York: Columbia Univ., Teachers College, Bur. Publ
  59. Nisbett RE, Ross LD. 1980. Human Inference: Strategies and Shortcomings of Social Judgment Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall
  60. Ochsner KN, Bunge SA, Gross JJ, Gabrieli JD. 2002. Rethinking feelings: an FMRI study of the cognitive regulation of emotion. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 14:1215–29 [Google Scholar]
  61. Pervin LA. 1994. A critical analysis of trait theory. Psychol. Inq. 5:103–13 [Google Scholar]
  62. Peterson DR. 1968. The Clinical Study of Social Behavior New York: Appleton
  63. Plaks JE, Shafer JL, Shoda Y. 2003. Perceiving individuals and groups as coherent: How do perceivers make sense of variable behavior?. Soc. Cogn. In press [Google Scholar]
  64. Posner MI, Rothbart MK. 2000. Developing mechanisms of self-regulation. Dev. Psychopathol. 12:427–41 [Google Scholar]
  65. Read SJ, Miller LC. 1998. On the dynamic construction of meaning: an interactive activation and competition model of social perception. In Connectionist Models of Social Reasoning and Social Behavior ed. SJ Read, LC Miller pp. 27–68 Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum [Google Scholar]
  66. Ross L, Nisbett RE. 1991. The Person and the Situation: Perspectives of Social Psychology New York: McGraw-Hill
  67. Sethi A, Mischel W, Aber L, Shoda Y, Rodriguez M. 2000. The role of strategic attention deployment in development of self-regulation: predicting preschoolers' delay of gratification from mother-toddler interactions. Dev. Psychol. 36:767–77 [Google Scholar]
  68. Shoda Y. 2003. Individual differences in social psychology: Understanding situations to understand people, understanding people to understand situations. In Handbook of Methods in Psychology ed. C Sansone, C Morf, A Panter Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage In press [Google Scholar]
  69. Shoda Y, LeeTiernan SJ. 2002. What remains invariant? Finding order within a person's thoughts, feelings, and behaviors across situations. See Cervone & Mischel 2002 pp. 241–70 [Google Scholar]
  70. Shoda Y, LeeTiernan SJ, Mischel W. 2002. Personality as a dynamical system: emergence of stability and consistency in intra- and inter-personal interactions. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 6:316–25 [Google Scholar]
  71. Shoda Y, Mischel W. 1998. Personality as a stable cognitive-affective activation network: characteristic patterns of behavior variation emerge from a stable personality structure See Read & Miller 1998 pp. 175–208
  72. Shoda Y, Mischel W. 2000. Reconciling contextualism with the core assumptions of personality psychology. Eur. J. Personal. 14:407–28 [Google Scholar]
  73. Shoda Y, Mischel W, Wright JC. 1989. Intuitive interactionism in person perception: effects of situation-behavior relations on dispositional judgments. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 56:41–53 [Google Scholar]
  74. Shoda Y, Mischel W, Wright JC. 1993a. Links between personality judgments and contextualized behavior patterns: situation-behavior profiles of personality prototypes. Soc. Cogn. 4:399–429 [Google Scholar]
  75. Shoda Y, Mischel W, Wright JC. 1993b. The role of situational demands and cognitive competencies in behavior organization and personality coherence. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 65:1023–35 [Google Scholar]
  76. Shoda Y, Mischel W, Wright JC. 1994. Intraindividual stability in the organization and patterning of behavior: incorporating psychological situations into the ideographic analysis of personality. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 67:674–87 [Google Scholar]
  77. Shweder RA. 1999. Humans really are different. Science 283:798–99 [Google Scholar]
  78. Smith ER, DeCoster J. 1998. Person perception and stereotyping: simulation using distributed representations in a recurrent connectionist network. See Read & Miller 1998 pp. 111–40 [Google Scholar]
  79. Trope Y. 1986. Identification and inferential processes in dispositional attribution. Psychol. Rev. 93:239–57 [Google Scholar]
  80. Vansteelandt K, Van Mechelen I. 1998. Individual differences in situation-behavior profiles: a triple typology model. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 75:751–65 [Google Scholar]
  81. Vernon PE. 1964. Personality Assessment: A Critical Survey New York: Wiley
  82. Vonk R. 1998. The slime effect: suspicion and dislike of likeable behavior toward superiors. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 74:849–64 [Google Scholar]
  83. Wright JC, Mischel W. 1987. A conditional approach to dispositional constructs: the local predictability of social behavior. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 53:1159–77 [Google Scholar]
  84. Wright JC, Mischel W. 1988. Conditional hedges and the intuitive psychology of traits. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 55:454–69 [Google Scholar]
  85. Zayas V, Shoda Y, Ayduk O. 2002. Personality in context: an interpersonal systems perspective. J. Personal. 70:851–98 [Google Scholar]
/content/journals/10.1146/annurev.psych.55.042902.130709
Loading
  • Article Type: Review Article
This is a required field
Please enter a valid email address
Approval was a Success
Invalid data
An Error Occurred
Approval was partially successful, following selected items could not be processed due to error