1932

Abstract

In this article we examine how contingency and synchrony during infant–caregiver interactions help children learn to pay attention to objects and how this, in turn, affects their ability to direct caregivers’ attention and to track communicative intentions in others. First, we present evidence that, early in life, child–caregiver interactions are asymmetric. Caregivers dynamically and contingently adapt to their child more than the other way around, providing higher-order semantic and contextual cues during attention episodes, which facilitate the development of specialized and integrated attentional brain networks in the infant brain. Then, we describe how social contingency also facilitates the child's development of predictive models and, through that, goal-directed behavior. Finally, we discuss how contingency and synchrony of brain and behavior can drive children's ability to direct their caregivers’ attention voluntarily and how this, in turn, paves the way for intentional communication.

Loading

Article metrics loading...

/content/journals/10.1146/annurev-devpsych-010923-110459
2024-12-09
2025-06-16
Loading full text...

Full text loading...

/deliver/fulltext/devpsych/6/1/annurev-devpsych-010923-110459.html?itemId=/content/journals/10.1146/annurev-devpsych-010923-110459&mimeType=html&fmt=ahah

Literature Cited

  1. Abney DH, Suanda SH, Smith LB, Yu C. 2020.. What are the building blocks of parent–infant coordinated attention in free-flowing interaction?. Infancy 25:(6):87187
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  2. Ahmadzadeh YI, Eley TC, Leve LD, Shaw DS, Natsuaki MN, et al. 2019.. Anxiety in the family: a genetically informed analysis of transactional associations between mother, father and child anxiety symptoms. . J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 60:(12):126977
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  3. Aktar E, Van Bockstaele B, Pérez-Edgar K, Wiers RW, Bögels SM. 2019.. Intergenerational transmission of attentional bias and anxiety. . Dev. Sci. 22:(3):e12772
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  4. Anderson EM, Seemiller ES, Smith LB. 2022.. Scene saliencies in egocentric vision and their creation by parents and infants. . Cognition 229::105256
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  5. Aureli T, Presaghi F, Garito MC. 2022.. Mother–infant co-regulation during infancy: developmental changes and influencing factors. . Infant Behav. Dev. 69::101768
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  6. Bakeman R, Adamson LB. 1984.. Coordinating attention to people and objects in mother-infant and peer-infant interaction. . Child Dev. 55::127889
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  7. Baldwin DA, Markman EM. 1989.. Establishing word-object relations: a first step. . Child Dev. 60::38198
    [Google Scholar]
  8. Bales KL, Hang S, Paulus J, Jahanfard E, Manca C, et al. 2023.. Individual differences in social homeostasis. . Front. Behav. Neurosci. 17::1068609
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  9. Bánki A, Köster M, Cichy RM, Hoehl S. 2024.. Communicative signals during joint attention promote neural processes of infants and caregivers. . Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 65::101321
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  10. Bazhydai M, Westermann G, Parise E. 2020.. “I don't know but I know who to ask”: 12-month-olds actively seek information from knowledgeable adults. . Dev. Sci. 23:(5):e12938
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  11. Beebe B, Messinger D, Bahrick LE, Margolis A, Buck KA, Chen H. 2016.. A systems view of mother–infant face-to-face communication. . Dev. Psychol. 52:(4):55671
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  12. Begus K, Southgate V. 2012.. Infant pointing serves an interrogative function. . Dev. Sci. 15:(5):61117
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  13. Begus K, Southgate V. 2018.. Curious learners: how infants’ motivation to learn shapes and is shaped by infants’ interactions with the social world. . In Active Learning from Infancy to Childhood, ed. MM Saylor, PA Ganea , pp. 1337. Cham, Switz:.: Springer
    [Google Scholar]
  14. Begus K, Southgate V, Gliga T. 2015.. Neural mechanisms of infant learning: differences in frontal theta activity during object exploration modulate subsequent object recognition. . Biol. Lett. 11:(5):20150041
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  15. Belsky J. 1980.. Mother-infant interaction at home and in the laboratory: a comparative study. . J. Genet. Psychol. 137:(1):3747
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  16. Bergson H. 2007.. L’évolution créatrice. Paris:: PUF. , 11th ed..
    [Google Scholar]
  17. Bernard K, Nissim G, Vaccaro S, Harris JL, Lindhiem O. 2018.. Association between maternal depression and maternal sensitivity from birth to 12 months: a meta-analysis. . Attach. Hum. Dev. 20:(6):57899
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  18. Bigelow AE, Power M. 2016.. Effect of maternal responsiveness on young infants’ social bidding-like behavior during the still face task. . Infant Child Dev. 25:(3):25676
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  19. Bornstein MH. 2013.. Mother-infant attunement: a multilevel approach via body, brain, and behavior. . In The Infant Mind: Origins of the Social Brain, ed. M Legerstee, DW Haley, MH Bornstein , pp. 24865. New York:: Guilford
    [Google Scholar]
  20. Bronson GW. 1990.. Changes in infants' visual scanning across the 2- to 14-week age period. . J. Exp. Child Psychol. 49:(1):10125
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  21. Bronson GW. 1994.. Infants transitions toward adult-like scanning. . Child Dev. 65:(5):124361
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  22. Bruner JS. 1974.. From communication to language—a psychological perspective. . Cognition 3:(3):25587
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  23. Burgess AP. 2013.. On the interpretation of synchronization in EEG hyperscanning studies: a cautionary note. . Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7::881
    [Google Scholar]
  24. Burris SE, Brown DD. 2014.. When all children comprehend: increasing the external validity of narrative comprehension development research. . Front. Psychol. 5::168
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  25. Busch NA, Dubois J, VanRullen R. 2009.. The phase of ongoing EEG oscillations predicts visual perception. . J. Neurosci. 29:(24):786976
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  26. Buschman TJ, Kastner S. 2015.. From behavior to neural dynamics: an integrated theory of attention. . Neuron 88:(1):12744
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  27. Carvalho MES, Justo JMRM, Gratier M, Tomé T, Pereira E, Rodrigues H. 2019.. Vocal responsiveness of preterm infants to maternal infant-directed speaking and singing during skin-to-skin contact (Kangaroo Care) in the NICU. . Infant Behav. Dev. 57::101332
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  28. Çetinçelik M, Rowland C, Snijders T. 2022.. Does the speaker's eye gaze facilitate infants’ word segmentation from continuous speech? An ERP study. . Dev. Sci. 27::e13436
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  29. Cheesman R, Eilertsen EM, Ahmadzadeh YI, Gjerde LC, Hannigan LJ, et al. 2020.. How important are parents in the development of child anxiety and depression? A genomic analysis of parent-offspring trios in the Norwegian Mother Father and Child Cohort Study (MoBa). . BMC Med. 18::284
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  30. Choisdealbha ÁN, Attaheri A, Rocha S, Mead N, Olawole-Scott H, et al. 2023.. Neural phase angle from two months when tracking speech and non-speech rhythm linked to language performance from 12 to 24 months. . Brain Lang. 243::105301
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  31. Clark A. 2013.. Whatever next? Predictive brains, situated agents, and the future of cognitive science. . Behav. Brain Sci. 36:(3):181204
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  32. Cohen LB. 1972.. Attention-getting and attention-holding processes of infant visual preferences. . Child Dev. 43::86979
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  33. Cohn JF, Tronick EZ. 1988.. Mother-infant face-to-face interaction: Influence is bidirectional and unrelated to periodic cycles in either partner's behavior. . Dev. Psychol. 24:(3):38692
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  34. Cole PM, Lougheed JP, Chow S-M, Ram N. 2020.. Development of emotion regulation dynamics across early childhood: a multiple time-scale approach. . Affect. Sci. 1::2841
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  35. Colombo J. 2001.. The development of visual attention in infancy. . Annu. Rev. Psychol. 52::33767
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  36. Colombo J, Cheatham CL. 2006.. The emergence and basis of endogenous attention in infancy and early childhood. . Adv. Child Dev. Behav. 34::283322
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  37. Csibra G, Gergely G. 2009.. Natural pedagogy. . Trends Cogn. Sci. 13:(4):14853
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  38. Custode SA, Tamis-LeMonda C. 2020.. Cracking the code: social and contextual cues to language input in the home environment. . Infancy 25:(6):80926
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  39. de Urabain IRS, Nuthmann A, Johnson MH, Smith TJ. 2017.. Disentangling the mechanisms underlying infant fixation durations in scene perception: a computational account. . Vis. Res. 134::4359
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  40. D'Entremont B, Morgan R. 2006.. Experience with visual barriers and its effects on subsequent gaze-following in 12- to 13-month-olds. . Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 24:(3):46575
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  41. Donnellan E, Bannard C, McGillion ML, Slocombe KE, Matthews D. 2020.. Infants’ intentionally communicative vocalizations elicit responses from caregivers and are the best predictors of the transition to language: a longitudinal investigation of infants’ vocalizations, gestures and word production. . Dev. Sci. 23:(1):e12843
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  42. Duncan J, Phillips E, Mitchell DJ, Cooper PJ, Murray L. 2023.. Parental scaffolding during book-sharing predicts child general intelligence. . Psychon. Bull. Rev. 30::20029
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  43. Edmunds SR, Kover ST, Stone WL. 2019.. The relation between parent verbal responsiveness and child communication in young children with or at risk for autism spectrum disorder: a systematic review and meta-analysis. . Autism Res. 12:(5):71531
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  44. Elmlinger SL, Schwade JA, Vollmer L, Goldstein MH. 2023.. Learning how to learn from social feedback: the origins of early vocal development. . Dev. Sci. 26:(2):e13296
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  45. Evans CA, Porter CL. 2009.. The emergence of mother-infant co-regulation during the first year: links to infants’ developmental status and attachment. . Infant Behav. Dev. 32:(2):14758
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  46. Farroni T, Johnson MH, Csibra G. 2004.. Mechanisms of eye gaze perception during infancy. . J. Cogn. Neurosci. 16:(8):132026
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  47. Farroni T, Massaccesi S, Menon E, Johnson MH. 2007.. Direct gaze modulates face recognition in young infants. . Cognition 102:(3):396404
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  48. Feldman R. 2006.. From biological rhythms to social rhythms: physiological precursors of mother-infant synchrony. . Dev. Psychol. 42::17588
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  49. Feldman R, Greenbaum CW, Yirmiya N. 1999.. Mother–infant affect synchrony as an antecedent of the emergence of self-control. . Dev. Psychol. 35:(1):22331
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  50. Fogel A. 1993.. Developing Through Relationships. Chicago:: Univ. Chicago Press
    [Google Scholar]
  51. Fogel A. 2018.. Two principles of communication: co-regulation and framing. . In New Perspectives in Early Communicative Development, ed. J Nadel, L Camaioni , pp. 922. London:: Routledge
    [Google Scholar]
  52. Fogel A, Garvey A. 2007.. Alive communication. . Infant Behav. Dev. 30:(2):25157
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  53. Forcada-Guex M, Pierrehumbert B, Borghini A, Moessinger A, Muller-Nix C. 2006.. Early dyadic patterns of mother-infant interactions and outcomes of prematurity at 18 months. . Pediatrics 118:(1):e10714
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  54. Forest TA, Amso D. 2023.. Neurodevelopment of attention, learning, and memory systems in infancy. . Annu. Rev. Dev. Psychol. 5::4565
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  55. Franchak JM, Kretch KS, Adolph KE. 2018.. See and be seen: infant-caregiver social looking during locomotor free play. . Dev. Sci. 21:(4):e12626
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  56. Friston KJ. 2019.. Waves of prediction. . PLOS Biol. 17:(10):e3000426
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  57. Gardner JM, Karmel BZ. 1995.. Development of arousal-modulated visual preferences in early infancy. . Dev. Psychol. 31:(3):47382
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  58. Geeraerts SB, Hessels RS, Van der Stigchel S, Huijding J, Endendijk JJ, et al. 2019.. Individual differences in visual attention and self-regulation: a multimethod longitudinal study from infancy to toddlerhood. . J. Exp. Child Psychol. 180::10412
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  59. Ghazanfar AA, Zhang YS. 2016.. The autonomic nervous system is the engine for vocal development through social feedback. . Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 40::15560
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  60. Gibson JJ. 1979.. The Ecological Approach to Visual Perception. Boston: Houghton Mifflin:
    [Google Scholar]
  61. Glynn LM, Baram TZ. 2019.. The influence of unpredictable, fragmented parental signals on the developing brain. . Front. Neuroendocrinol. 53::100736
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  62. Goldstein MH, Schwade J, Briesch J, Syal S. 2010.. Learning while babbling: prelinguistic object-directed vocalizations indicate a readiness to learn. . Infancy 15:(4):36291
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  63. Gottlieb G. 1991.. Experiential canalization of behavioral development: theory. . Dev. Psychol. 27:(1):413
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  64. Goupil L, Dautriche I, Denman K, Henry Z, Marriott Haresign I, Wass S. 2024.. Leader-follower dynamics during early social interactions matter for infant word learning. . PNAS 121::e2321008121
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  65. Goupil L, Romand-Monnier M, Kouider S. 2016.. Infants ask for help when they know they don't know. . PNAS 113::349296
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  66. Gros-Louis J, West MJ, Goldstein MH, King AP. 2006.. Mothers provide differential feedback to infants’ prelinguistic sounds. . Int. J. Behav. Dev. 30:(6):50916
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  67. Grzadzinski R, Amso D, Landa R, Watson L, Guralnick M, et al. 2021.. Pre-symptomatic intervention for autism spectrum disorder (ASD): defining a research agenda. . J. Neurodev. Disord. 13:(1):49
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  68. Gueron-Sela N, Wagner NJ, Propper CB, Mills-Koonce WR, Moore GA, Cox MJ. 2017.. The interaction between child respiratory sinus arrhythmia and early sensitive parenting in the prediction of children's executive functions. . Infancy 22:(2):17189
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  69. Ham J, Tronick E. 2009.. Relational psychophysiology: lessons from mother–infant physiology research on dyadically expanded states of consciousness. . Psychother. Res. 19:(6):61932
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  70. Hamilton AFC. 2021.. Hyperscanning: beyond the hype. . Neuron 109:(3):4047
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  71. Hasson U, Chen J, Honey CJ. 2015.. Hierarchical process memory: memory as an integral component of information processing. . Trends Cogn. Sci. 19:(6):30413
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  72. Hasson U, Yang E, Vallines I, Heeger DJ, Rubin N. 2008.. A hierarchy of temporal receptive windows in human cortex. . J. Neurosci. 28:(10):253950
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  73. Hendry A, Johnson MH, Holmboe K. 2019.. Early development of visual attention: change, stability, and longitudinal associations. . Annu. Rev. Dev. Psychol. 1::25175
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  74. Huang N, Elhilali M. 2017.. Auditory salience using natural soundscapes. . J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 141:(3):216376
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  75. Hume D. 1896.. A Treatise of Human Nature. Oxford, UK:: Clarendon
    [Google Scholar]
  76. Itti L, Baldi P. 2009.. Bayesian surprise attracts human attention. . Vis. Res. 49:(10):1295306
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  77. Johnson MH. 1990.. Cortical maturation and the development of visual attention in early infancy. . J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2:(2):8195
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  78. Johnson MH. 2015.. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience. Chichester, UK:: Wiley-Blackwell. , 4th ed..
    [Google Scholar]
  79. Johnson MH, Posner MI, Rothbart MK. 1991.. Components of visual orienting in early infancy: contingency learning, anticipatory looking, and disengaging. . J. Cogn. Neurosci. 3:(4):33544
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  80. Just MA, Varma S. 2007.. The organization of thinking: what functional brain imaging reveals about the neuroarchitecture of complex cognition. . Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 7:(3):15391
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  81. Kaaresen PI, Rønning JA, Ulvund SE, Dahl LB. 2006.. A randomized, controlled trial of the effectiveness of an early-intervention program in reducing parenting stress after preterm birth. . Pediatrics 118:(1):e919
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  82. Karmiloff-Smith A. 1998.. Development itself is the key to understanding developmental disorders. . Trends Cogn. Sci. 2:(10):38998
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  83. Keller AS, Sydnor VJ, Pines A, Fair DA, Bassett DS, Satterthwaite TD. 2022.. Hierarchical functional system development supports executive function. . Trends Cogn. Sci. 27::16074
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  84. Keshavarzi M, Mandke K, Macfarlane A, Parvez L, Gabrielczyk F, et al. 2022.. Atypical delta-band phase consistency and atypical preferred phase in children with dyslexia during neural entrainment to rhythmic audio-visual speech. . NeuroImage Clin. 35::103054
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  85. Kidd E, Holler J. 2009.. Children's use of gesture to resolve lexical ambiguity. . Dev. Sci. 12:(6):90313
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  86. Kingsbury L, Huang S, Wang J, Gu K, Golshani P, et al. 2019.. Correlated neural activity and encoding of behavior across brains of socially interacting animals. . Cell 178::42946.e16
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  87. Köster M, Kayhan E, Langeloh M, Hoehl S. 2020.. Making sense of the world: infant learning from a predictive processing perspective. . Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 15:(3):56271
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  88. Köster M, Langeloh M, Hoehl S. 2019.. Visually entrained theta oscillations increase for unexpected events in the infant brain. . Psychol. Sci. 30:(11):165663
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  89. Kovács ÁM, Tauzin T, Téglás E, Gergely G, Csibra G. 2014.. Pointing as epistemic request: 12-month-olds point to receive new information. . Infancy 19:(6):54357
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  90. Le H, Hoch JE, Ossmy O, Adolph KE, Fern X, Fern A. 2021.. Modeling infant free play using hidden Markov models. . In 2021 IEEE International Conference on Development and Learning (ICDL). https://doi.org/10.1109/ICDL49984.2021.9515677
    [Google Scholar]
  91. Leong V, Byrne E, Clackson K, Georgieva SD, Lam S, Wass SV. 2017.. Speaker gaze increases information coupling between infant and adult brains. . PNAS 114:(50):1329095
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  92. Luchkina E, Xu F. 2022.. From social contingency to verbal reference: a constructivist hypothesis. . Psychol. Rev. 129:(4):890909
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  93. Marriott Haresign I, Phillips EAM, Wass SV. 2024.. Why behaviour matters: studying inter-brain coordination during child-caregiver interaction. . Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 67::101384
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  94. Marriott Haresign I, Phillips EAM, Whitehorn M, Lamagna F, Eliano M, et al. 2023.. Gaze onsets during naturalistic infant-caregiver interaction associate with ‘sender’ but not ‘receiver’ neural responses, and do not lead to changes in inter-brain synchrony. . Sci. Rep. 13:(1):3555
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  95. Masek LR, McMillan BTM, Paterson SJ, Tamis-LeMonda CS, Golinkoff RM, Hirsh-Pasek K. 2021.. Where language meets attention: how contingent interactions promote learning. . Dev. Rev. 60::100961
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  96. Mason GM. 2018.. Investigating dyadic social coordination and infant attention in typical and atypical development. PhD Diss. , Cornell Univ., Ithaca, NY:
    [Google Scholar]
  97. Mason GM, Kirkpatrick F, Schwade JA, Goldstein MH. 2019.. The role of dyadic coordination in organizing visual attention in 5-month-old infants. . Infancy 24:(2):16286
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  98. Mathewson KE, Gratton G, Fabiani M, Beck DM, Ro T. 2009.. To see or not to see: prestimulus α phase predicts visual awareness. . J. Neurosci. 29:(9):272532
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  99. McQuillan ME, Smith LB, Yu C, Bates JE. 2020.. Parents influence the visual learning environment through children's manual actions. . Child Dev. 91:(3):e70120
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  100. Menn KH, Michel C, Meyer L, Hoehl S, Männel C. 2022.. Natural infant-directed speech facilitates neural tracking of prosody. . NeuroImage 251::118991
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  101. Ménoret M, Varnet L, Fargier R, Cheylus A, Curie A, et al. 2014.. Neural correlates of non-verbal social interactions: a dual-EEG study. . Neuropsychologia 55::8597
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  102. Miller JL, Gros-Louis J. 2013.. Socially guided attention influences infants’ communicative behavior. . Infant Behav. Dev. 36:(4):62734
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  103. Mital PK, Smith TJ, Hill RL, Henderson JM. 2010.. Clustering of gaze during dynamic scene viewing is predicted by motion. . Cogn. Comput. 3:(1):524
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  104. Mitsven SG, Prince EB, Messinger DS, Tenenbaum EJ, Sheinkopf SJ, et al. 2022.. Testing the mid-range model: attachment in a high risk sample. . Dev. Sci. 25:(3):e13185
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  105. Moll H, Tomasello M. 2007.. How 14- and 18-month-olds know what others have experienced. . Dev. Psychol. 43:(2):30917
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  106. Moore C, D'Entremont B. 2001.. Developmental changes in pointing as a function of attentional focus. . J. Cogn. Dev. 2:(2):10929
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  107. Murray L, De Pascalis L, Bozicevic L, Hawkins L, Sclafani V, Ferrari PF. 2016.. The functional architecture of mother-infant communication, and the development of infant social expressiveness in the first two months. . Sci. Rep. 6::39019
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  108. Nguyen T, Banki A, Markova G, Hoehl S. 2020a.. Studying parent-child interaction with hyperscanning. . Prog. Brain Res. 254::124
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  109. Nguyen T, Schleihauf H, Kayhan E, Matthes D, Vrtička P, Hoehl S. 2020b.. The effects of interaction quality on neural synchrony during mother-child problem solving. . Cortex 124::23549
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  110. Novembre G, Iannetti GD. 2021.. Hyperscanning alone cannot prove causality. Multibrain stimulation can. . Trends Cogn. Sci. 25:(2):9699
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  111. Novembre G, Knoblich G, Dunne L, Keller PE. 2017.. Interpersonal synchrony enhanced through 20 Hz phase-coupled dual brain stimulation. . Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 12:(4):66270
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  112. Oakes LM. 2023a.. The cascading development of visual attention in infancy: learning to look and looking to learn. . Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 32:(5):41017
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  113. Oakes LM. 2023b.. The development of visual attention in infancy: a cascade approach. . Adv. Child Dev. Behav. 64::137
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  114. Oakes LM, Kannass KN, Shaddy DJ. 2002.. Developmental changes in endogenous control of attention: the role of target familiarity on infants’ distraction latency. . Child Dev. 73:(6):164455
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  115. Okumura Y, Kanakogi Y, Kobayashi T, Itakura S. 2020.. Ostension affects infant learning more than attention. . Cognition 195::104082
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  116. Orekhova EV, Stroganova TA, Posikera IN, Elam M. 2006.. EEG theta rhythm in infants and preschool children. . Clin. Neurophysiol. 117:(5):104762
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  117. Pan Y, Novembre G, Song B, Zhu Y, Hu Y. 2021.. Dual brain stimulation enhances interpersonal learning through spontaneous movement synchrony. . Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 16:(1–2):21021
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  118. Parrinello RM, Ruff HA. 1988.. The influence of adult intervention on infants’ level of attention. . Child Dev. 59:(4):112535
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  119. Pempek TA, Kirkorian HL, Richards JE, Anderson DR, Lund AF, Stevens M. 2010.. Video comprehensibility and attention in very young children. . Dev. Psychol. 46:(5):128393
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  120. Perapoch Amadó M, Greenwood E, Ives J, Labendzki P, Marriott Haresign I, et al. 2023a.. The neural and physiological substrates of real-world attention change across development. . eLife 12::RP92171. Preprint
    [Google Scholar]
  121. Perapoch Amadó M, Phillips EAM, Esposito G, Greenwood E, Ives J. 2023b.. Who leads and who follows? The pathways to joint attention during free-flowing interactions. . Preprint
  122. Pereira AF, Smith LB, Yu C. 2014.. A bottom-up view of toddler word learning. . Psychon. Bull. Rev. 21::17885
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  123. Phillips EAM, Goupil L, Whitehorn M, Bruce-Gardyne E, Csolsim FA, et al. 2023a.. Endogenous oscillatory rhythms and interactive contingencies jointly influence infant attention during early infant-caregiver interaction. . bioRxiv 2023.06.19.545295. https://doi.org/10.1101/2023.06.19.545295
  124. Phillips EAM, Goupil L, Whitehorn M, Bruce-Gardyne E, Csolsim FA, et al. 2023b.. Proactive or reactive? Neural oscillatory insight into the leader-follower dynamics of early infant-caregiver interaction. . PNAS 120:(15):e2122481120
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  125. Piazza EA, Hasenfratz L, Hasson U, Lew-Williams C. 2020.. Infant and adult brains are coupled to the dynamics of natural communication. . Psychol. Sci. 31::617
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  126. Poeppel D, Assaneo MF. 2020.. Speech rhythms and their neural foundations. . Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 21:(6):32234
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  127. Rayson H, Bonaiuto JJ, Ferrari PF, Chakrabarti B, Murray L. 2019.. Building blocks of joint attention: early sensitivity to having one's own gaze followed. . Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 37::100631
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  128. Redcay E, Warnell KR. 2018.. A social-interactive neuroscience approach to understanding the developing brain. . Adv. Child Dev. Behav. 54::144
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  129. Reynolds GD, Romano AC. 2016.. The development of attention systems and working memory in infancy. . Front. Syst. Neurosci. 10::15
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  130. Richards JE. 1997.. Peripheral stimulus localization by infants: attention, age, and individual differences in heart rate variability. . J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 23:(3):66780
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  131. Richards JE. 2010.. The development of attention to simple and complex visual stimuli in infants: behavioral and psychophysiological measures. . Dev. Rev. 30:(2):20319
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  132. Richards JE, Turner ED. 2001.. Extended visual fixation and distractibility in children from six to twenty-four months of age. . Child Dev. 72:(4):96372
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  133. Robertson SS. 2014.. Empty-headed dynamical model of infant visual foraging. . Dev. Psychobiol. 56:(5):112933
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  134. Ruzzoli M, Torralba M, Moris Fernandez L, Soto-Faraco S. 2019.. The relevance of alpha phase in human perception. . Cortex 120::24968
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  135. Santamaria L, Noreika V, Georgieva S, Clackson K, Wass S, Leong V. 2020.. Emotional valence modulates the topology of the parent-infant inter-brain network. . NeuroImage 207::116341
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  136. Schneirla TC. 1946.. Problems in the bio-psychology of social organization. . J. Abnorm. Soc. Psychol. 41:(4):385402
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  137. Schroer S, Smith L, Yu C. 2019.. Examining the multimodal effects of parent speech in parent-infant interactions. . In Proceedings of the 41st Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, ed. AK Goel, CM Seifert, C Freksa , pp. 101521. Montreal:: Cogn. Sci. Soc.
    [Google Scholar]
  138. Schroer SE, Yu C. 2021.. Multimodal attention creates the visual input for infant word learning. . In 2021 IEEE International Conference on Development and Learning (ICDL). https://doi.org/10.1109/ICDL49984.2021.9515676
    [Google Scholar]
  139. Schroer SE, Yu C. 2022.. The real-time effects of parent speech on infants’ multimodal attention and dyadic coordination. . Infancy 27:(6):115478
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  140. Senju A, Csibra G. 2008.. Gaze following in human infants depends on communicative signals. . Curr. Biol. 18::66871
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  141. Simony E, Honey CJ, Chen J, Lositsky O, Yeshurun Y, et al. 2016.. Dynamic reconfiguration of the default mode network during narrative comprehension. . Nat. Commun. 7::12141
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  142. Siposova B, Carpenter M. 2019.. A new look at joint attention and common knowledge. . Cognition 189::26074
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  143. Slone LK, Abney DH, Smith LB, Yu C. 2023.. The temporal structure of parent talk to toddlers about objects. . Cognition 230::105266
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  144. Smith CG, Jones EJH, Charman T, Clackson K, Mirza FU, Wass SV. 2021.. Anxious parents show higher physiological synchrony with their infants. . Psychol. Med. 52::304050
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  145. Smith CG, Jones EJH, Wass SV, Jacobs D, Fitzpatrick C, Charman T. 2022.. The effect of perinatal interventions on parent anxiety, infant socio-emotional development and parent-infant relationship outcomes: a systematic review. . JCPP Adv. 2:(4):e12116
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  146. Smith LB, Breazeal C. 2007.. The dynamic lift of developmental process. . Dev. Sci. 10:(1):6168
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  147. Smith MJ, Harper DGC. 1995.. Animal signals: models and terminology. . J. Theor. Biol. 177:(3):30511
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  148. Spittle A, Orton J, Anderson PJ, Boyd R, Doyle LW. 2015.. Early developmental intervention programmes provided post hospital discharge to prevent motor and cognitive impairment in preterm infants. . Cochrane Database Syst. Rev. 2015:(11):CD005495
    [Google Scholar]
  149. Stallworthy IC, Sifre R, Berry D, Lasch C, Smith TJ, Elison JT. 2020.. Infants’ gaze exhibits a fractal structure that varies by age and stimulus salience. . Sci. Rep. 10:(1):17216
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  150. Striano T, Bertin E. 2005.. Coordinated affect with mothers and strangers: a longitudinal analysis of joint engagement between 5 and 9 months of age. . Cogn. Emot. 19:(5):78190
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  151. Suarez-Rivera C, Smith LB, Yu C. 2019.. Multimodal parent behaviors within joint attention support sustained attention in infants. . Dev. Psychol. 55:(1):96109
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  152. Sun L, Francis DJ, Nagai Y, Yoshida H. 2024.. Early development of saliency-driven attention through object manipulation. . Acta Psychol. 243::104124
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  153. Szymanski C, Müller V, Brick TR, Von Oertzen T, Lindenberger U. 2017.. Hyper-transcranial alternating current stimulation: experimental manipulation of inter-brain synchrony. . Front. Hum. Neurosci. 11::539
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  154. Tamis-LeMonda CS. 2023.. The mountain stream of infant development. . Infancy 28:(3):46891
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  155. Tamis-LeMonda CS, Bornstein MH, Baumwell L. 2001.. Maternal responsiveness and children's achievement of language milestones. . Child Dev. 72:(3):74867
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  156. Tamis-LeMonda CS, Kuchirko Y, Song L. 2014.. Why is infant language learning facilitated by parental responsiveness?. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 23:(2):12126
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  157. Tamis-LeMonda CS, Kuchirko Y, Tafuro L. 2013.. From action to interaction: infant object exploration and mothers’ contingent responsiveness. . IEEE Trans. Auton. Ment. Dev. 5:(3):2029
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  158. Tan SHJ, Kalashnikova M, Di Liberto GM, Crosse MJ, Burnham D. 2022.. Seeing a talking face matters: the relationship between cortical tracking of continuous auditory-visual speech and gaze behaviour in infants, children and adults. . NeuroImage 256::119217
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  159. Thelen E. 2005.. Dynamic systems theory and the complexity of change. . Psychoanal. Dialogues 15:(2):25583
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  160. Throm E, Gui A, Haartsen R, da Costa PF, Leech R, Jones EJH. 2023.. Real-time monitoring of infant theta power during naturalistic social experiences. . Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 63::101300
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  161. Tomasello M. 2014.. Joint attention as social cognition. . In Joint Attention: Its Origins and Role in Development, ed. C Moore, PJ Dunham , pp. 10330. New York:: Psychol. Press
    [Google Scholar]
  162. Tomasello M, Farrar MJ. 1986.. Joint attention and early language. . Child Dev. 57::145463
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  163. Trevarthen C. 2001.. Intrinsic motives for companionship in understanding: their origin, development, and significance for infant mental health. . Infant Mental Health J. 22:(1–2):95131
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  164. Vales C, Smith LB. 2015.. Words, shape, visual search and visual working memory in 3-year-old children. . Dev. Sci. 18:(1):6579
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  165. van Kempen J, Gieselmann MA, Boyd M, Steinmetz NA, Moore T, et al. 2021.. Top-down coordination of local cortical state during selective attention. . Neuron 109:(5):894904
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  166. Vong WK, Wang W, Orhan AE, Lake BM. 2024.. Grounded language acquisition through the eyes and ears of a single child. . Science 383:(6682):50411
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  167. Vygotskij LS. 1979.. Mind in Society: The Development of Higher Psychological Processes. Cambridge, MA:: Harvard Univ. Press
    [Google Scholar]
  168. Wan MW, Green J, Elsabbagh M, Johnson M, Charman T, et al. 2012.. Parent–infant interaction in infant siblings at risk of autism. . Res. Dev. Disabil. 33:(3):92432
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  169. Wan MW, Green J, Scott J. 2019.. A systematic review of parent–infant interaction in infants at risk of autism. . Autism 23:(4):81120
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  170. Ward E, Rutar D, Zaadnoordijk L, Poli F, Hunnius S. 2023.. Beyond the adult mind: a developmental framework for predictive processing in infancy. . PsyArXiv. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/wktz8
    [Google Scholar]
  171. Warlaumont AS, Richards JA, Gilkerson J, Oller DK. 2014.. A social feedback loop for speech development and its reduction in autism. . Psychol. Sci. 25:(7):131424
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  172. Wass SV. 2022.. Oscillators and inertial dynamics: understanding the development of real-world attention control. OSF Preprints. . https://doi.org/10.31219/osf.io/257ku
  173. Wass SV, Clackson K, Georgieva SD, Brightman L, Nutbrown R, Leong V. 2018.. Infants’ visual sustained attention is higher during joint play than solo play: Is this due to increased endogenous attention control or exogenous stimulus capture?. Dev. Sci. 21::e12667
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  174. Wass SV, Greenwood EMG, Esposito G, Smith CG, Necef I, Phillips E. 2024.. Annual research review: “There, the dance is – at the still point of the turning world” – dynamic systems perspectives on coregulation and dysregulation during early development. . J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 65::481507
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  175. Wass SV, Perapoch Amadó M, Northrop T, Marriott Haresign I, Phillips EAM. 2023.. Foraging, hysteresis and inertia: understanding the developmental dynamics of real-world attention control. . PsyArXiv. https://doi.org/10.31219/osf.io/257ku
  176. Wass SV, Phillips E, Smith C, Goupil L. 2022.. Vocalisations and the dynamics of interpersonal arousal coupling in caregiver-infant dyads. . eLife 11::e77399
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  177. Wass SV, Smith CG, Clackson K, Gibb C, Eitzenberger J, Mirza FU. 2019.. Parents mimic and influence their infant's autonomic state through dynamic affective state matching. . Curr. Biol. 29:(14):241522.e4
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  178. Wass SV, Whitehorn M, Marriott Haresign I, Phillips E, Leong V. 2020.. Interpersonal neural entrainment during early social interaction. . Trends Cogn. Sci. 24::32942
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  179. Watson JS. 1967.. Memory and “contingency analysis” in infant learning. . Merrill-Palmer Q. 13:(1):5576
    [Google Scholar]
  180. Woolard A, Lane AE, Campbell LE, Whalen OM, Swaab L, et al. 2021.. Infant and child-directed speech used with infants and children at risk or diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder: a scoping review. . Rev. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 9::290306
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  181. Wu Z, Gros-Louis J. 2014.. Infants’ prelinguistic communicative acts and maternal responses: relations to linguistic development. . First Lang. 34:(1):7290
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  182. Yu C, Smith LB. 2012.. Embodied attention and word learning by toddlers. . Cognition 125:(2):24462
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  183. Yu C, Smith LB. 2013.. Joint attention without gaze following: Human infants and their parents coordinate visual attention to objects through eye-hand coordination. . PLOS ONE 8:(11):e79659
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  184. Yu C, Smith LB. 2016.. The social origins of sustained attention in one-year-old human infants. . Curr. Biol. 26:(9):123540
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  185. Yu C, Smith LB. 2017.. Multiple sensory-motor pathways lead to coordinated visual attention. . Cogn. Sci. 41::531
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  186. Zacks JM. 2020.. Event perception and memory. . Annu. Rev. Psychol. 71::16591
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
/content/journals/10.1146/annurev-devpsych-010923-110459
Loading
/content/journals/10.1146/annurev-devpsych-010923-110459
Loading

Data & Media loading...

This is a required field
Please enter a valid email address
Approval was a Success
Invalid data
An Error Occurred
Approval was partially successful, following selected items could not be processed due to error