1932

Abstract

Although a reflective self-awareness is often considered to be the pinnacle of human consciousness, it takes time to develop, and behaviors indicating a conceptual self-representation appear only toward the end of the second year of life. How it develops has been the subject of much speculation, but little empirical data have targeted causal mechanisms. I first review early behaviors that likely do or do not indicate a reflective self-awareness. I then draw on data indicating a continuity between bodily and conceptual self-awareness, suggesting that the key to understanding how a reflective self-awareness develops is to look for its origins in a bodily self and in the earliest available signals of self-origin. Considering data from adult cognitive neuroscience, I review the mechanisms that could exploit early visceral cues and represent them as belonging to the self and the role that caregiver–infant interactions may play in this process. I conclude by considering how cross-cultural differences in the emergence of a self-concept provide important insights into the mechanisms that develop self-awareness.

Loading

Article metrics loading...

/content/journals/10.1146/annurev-devpsych-120621-025747
2024-12-09
2025-06-22
Loading full text...

Full text loading...

/deliver/fulltext/devpsych/6/1/annurev-devpsych-120621-025747.html?itemId=/content/journals/10.1146/annurev-devpsych-120621-025747&mimeType=html&fmt=ahah

Literature Cited

  1. Ainley V, Apps MAJ, Fotopoulou A, Tsakiris M. 2016.. ‘ Bodily precision’: a predictive coding account of individual differences in interoceptive accuracy. . Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B 371:(1708):20160003. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2016.0003
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  2. Amsterdam B. 1972.. Mirror self-image reactions before age two. . Dev. Psychobiol. 5:(4):297305. https://doi.org/10.1002/dev.42000403
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  3. Babo-Rebelo M, Buot A, Tallon-Baudry C. 2019.. Neural responses to heartbeats distinguish self from other during imagination. . NeuroImage 191::1020. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.02.012
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  4. Bahrick LE, Watson JS. 1985.. Detection of intermodal proprioceptive–visual contingency as a potential basis of self-perception in infancy. . Dev. Psychol. 21:(6):96373. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.21.6.963
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  5. Baltazar M, Hazem N, Vilarem E, Beaucousin V, Picq J-L, Conty L. 2014.. Eye contact elicits bodily self-awareness in human adults. . Cognition 133:(1):12027. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2014.06.009
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  6. Bar-Haim Y, Ziv T, Lamy D, Hodes RM. 2006.. Nature and nurture in own-race face processing. . Psychol. Sci. 17:(2):15963. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01679.x
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  7. Begus K, Gliga T, Southgate V. 2016.. Infants’ preferences for native speakers are associated with an expectation of information. . PNAS 113:(44):12397402. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1603261113
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  8. Belsky J, Bell B, Bradley RH, Stallard N, Stewart-Brown SL. 2007.. Socioeconomic risk, parenting during the preschool years and child health age 6 years. . Eur. J. Public Health 17:(5):50813. https://doi.org/10.1093/eurpub/ckl261
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  9. Bigelow AE. 1981.. The correspondence between self-and image movement as a cue to self-recognition for young children. . J. Genet. Psychol. 139:(1):1126. https://doi.org/10.1080/00221325.1981.10533432
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  10. Bischof-Köhler D. 2012.. Empathy and self-recognition in phylogenetic and ontogenetic perspective. . Emot. Rev. 4:(1):4048. https://doi.org/10.1177/1754073911421377
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  11. Botvinick M, Cohen J. 1998.. Rubber hands ‘feel’ touch that eyes see. . Nature 391::756. https://doi.org/10.1038/35784
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  12. Bremner A. 2022.. Developmental origins of bodily awareness. . In The Routledge Handbook of Bodily Awareness, ed. AJT Alsmith, MR Longo , pp. 27997. New York:: Routledge. https://doi.org/10.4324/9780429321542-25
    [Google Scholar]
  13. Broesch T, Callaghan T, Henrich J, Murphy C, Rochat P. 2011.. Cultural variations in children's mirror self-recognition. . J. Cross-Cult. Psychol. 42:(6):101829. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022022110381114
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  14. Brooks-Gunn J, Lewis M. 1984.. The development of early visual self-recognition. . Dev. Rev. 4:(3):21539. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0273-2297(84)80006-4
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  15. Brown R, Lau H, LeDoux JE. 2019.. Understanding the higher-order approach to consciousness. . Trends Cogn. Sci. 23:(9):75468. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2019.06.009
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  16. Bulgarelli C, Blasi A, de Klerk CCJM, Richards JE, Hamilton A, Southgate V. 2019.. Fronto-temporoparietal connectivity and self-awareness in 18-month-olds: a resting state fNIRS study. . Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 38::100676. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2019.100676
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  17. Bulgarelli C, de Klerk CCJM, Richards JE, Southgate V, Hamilton A, Blasi A. 2020.. The developmental trajectory of fronto-temporoparietal connectivity as a proxy of the default mode network: a longitudinal fNIRS investigation. . Hum. Brain Mapp. 41:(10):271740. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.24974
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  18. Carruthers P. 2008.. Meta-cognition in animals: a skeptical look. . Mind Lang. 23:(1):5889. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-0017.2007.00329.x
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  19. Carruthers P. 2009.. How we know our own minds: the relationship between mindreading and metacognition. . Behav. Brain Sci. 32:(2):12138. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X09000545
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  20. Carver CS, Scheier MF. 1978.. Self-focusing effects of dispositional self-consciousness, mirror presence, and audience presence. . J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 36:(3):32432. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.36.3.324
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  21. Cebioğlu S, Broesch T. 2021.. Explaining cross-cultural variation in mirror self-recognition: new insights into the ontogeny of objective self-awareness. . Dev. Psychol. 57:(5):62538. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0001171
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  22. Cebioğlu S, Marin KA, Broesch T. 2022.. Variation in caregivers’ references to their toddlers: child-directed speech in Vanuatu and Canada. . Child Dev. 93:(6):e62238. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.13833
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  23. Chinn LK, Noonan CF, Patton KS, Lockman JJ. 2024.. Tactile localization promotes infant self-recognition in the mirror-mark test. . Curr. Biol. 34:(6):137075.e2. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2024.02.028
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  24. Ciaunica A, Crucianelli L. 2019.. Minimal self-awareness: from within a developmental perspective. . J. Conscious. Stud. 26:(3–4):20726
    [Google Scholar]
  25. Cirelli LK, Einarson KM, Trainor LJ. 2014.. Interpersonal synchrony increases prosocial behavior in infants. . Dev. Sci. 17:(6):100311. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12193
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  26. Clark A, Karmiloff-Smith A. 1993.. The cognizer's innards: a psychological and philosophical perspective on the development of thought. . Mind Lang. 8::487519. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-0017.1993.tb00299.x
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  27. Cleeremans A, Achoui D, Beauny A, Keuninckx L, Martin J-R, et al. 2020.. Learning to be conscious. . Trends Cogn. Sci. 24:(2):11223. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2019.11.011
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  28. Cleeremans A, Timmermans B, Pasquali A. 2007.. Consciousness and metarepresentation: a computational sketch. . Neural Netw. 20:(9):103239. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neunet.2007.09.011
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  29. Coll M-P, Hobson H, Bird G, Murphy J. 2021.. Systematic review and meta-analysis of the relationship between the heartbeat-evoked potential and interoception. . Neurosci. Behav. Physiol. 122::190200. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2020.12.012
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  30. Concoran A, Perrykkad K, Feuerriegel D, Robinson J. 2023.. Body as first teacher: the role of rhythmic visceral dynamics in early cognitive development. . Perspect. Psychol. Sci. https://doi.org/10.1177/17456916231185343
    [Google Scholar]
  31. Conty L, George N, Hietanen JK. 2016.. Watching Eyes effects: when others meet the self. . Conscious. Cogn. 45::18497. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2016.08.016
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  32. Crucianelli L, Krahé C, Jenkinson PM, Fotopoulou AK. 2018.. Interoceptive ingredients of body ownership: affective touch and cardiac awareness in the rubber hand illusion. . Cortex 104::18092. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2017.04.018
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  33. Damasio A. 2000.. The Feeling of What Happens: Body, Emotion and the Making of Consciousness. London:: Vintage
    [Google Scholar]
  34. Damasio A. 2012.. Self Comes to Mind: Constructing the Conscious Brain. New York:: Vintage
    [Google Scholar]
  35. de Klerk CCJM, Bulgarelli C, Hamilton A, Southgate V. 2019a.. Selective facial mimicry of native over foreign speakers in preverbal infants. . J. Exp. Child Psychol. 183::3347. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2019.01.015
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  36. de Klerk CCJM, Johnson MH, Heyes CM, Southgate V. 2015.. Baby steps: investigating the development of perceptual–motor couplings in infancy. . Dev. Sci. 18:(2):27080. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12226
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  37. de Klerk CCJM, Lamy-Yang I, Southgate V. 2019b.. The role of sensorimotor experience in the development of mimicry in infancy. . Dev. Sci. 22:(3):e12771. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12771
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  38. Dégeilh F, Bernier A, Leblanc É, Daneault V, Beauchamp MH. 2018.. Quality of maternal behaviour during infancy predicts functional connectivity between default mode network and salience network 9 years later. . Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 34::5362. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2018.06.003
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  39. Dehaene S, Kerszberg M, Changeux J-P. 1998.. A neuronal model of a global workspace in effortful cognitive tasks. . PNAS 95:(24):1452934. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.95.24.14529
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  40. Dewey JA, Knoblich G. 2014.. Do implicit and explicit measures of the sense of agency measure the same thing?. PLOS ONE 9:(10):e110118. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0110118
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  41. Dienes Z, Perner J. 1999.. A theory of implicit and explicit knowledge. . Behav. Brain Sci. 22:(5):735808. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X99002186
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  42. Dondi M, Simion F, Caltran G. 1999.. Can newborns discriminate between their own cry and the cry of another newborn infant?. Dev. Psychol. 35:(2):41826. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.35.2.418
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  43. Duval S, Wicklund RA. 1972.. A Theory of Objective Self Awareness. New York:: Academic
    [Google Scholar]
  44. Fair DA, Cohen AL, Dosenbach NUF, Church JA, Miezin FM, et al. 2008.. The maturing architecture of the brain's default network. . PNAS 105:(10):402832. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0800376105
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  45. Fairhurst MT, Löken L, Grossmann T. 2014.. Physiological and behavioral responses reveal 9-month-old infants’ sensitivity to pleasant touch. . Psychol. Sci. 25:(5):112431. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797614527114
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  46. Feldman R, Singer M, Zagoory O. 2010.. Touch attenuates infants’ physiological reactivity to stress. . Dev. Sci. 13:(2):27178. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00890.x
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  47. Field T, Diego M, Hernandez-Reif M, Fernandez M. 2007.. Depressed mothers’ newborns show less discrimination of other newborns’ cry sounds. . Infant Behav. Dev. 30:(3):43135. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2006.12.011
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  48. Filippetti ML, Johnson MH, Lloyd-Fox S, Dragovic D, Farroni T. 2013.. Body perception in newborns. . Curr. Biol. 23:(23):241316. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2013.10.017
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  49. Filippetti ML, Orioli G, Johnson MH, Farroni T. 2015.. Newborn body perception: sensitivity to spatial congruency. . Infancy 20:(4):45565. https://doi.org/10.1111/infa.12083
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  50. Fiske A, de Klerk C, Lui KYK, Collins-Jones L, Hendry A, et al. 2022.. The neural correlates of inhibitory control in 10-month-old infants: a functional near-infrared spectroscopy study. . NeuroImage 257::119241. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2022.119241
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  51. Fivush R, Nelson K. 2004.. Culture and language in the emergence of autobiographical memory. . Psychol. Sci. 15:(9):57377. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00722.x
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  52. Fleming SM. 2021.. Know Thyself: The Science of Self-Awareness. New York:: Basic Books
    [Google Scholar]
  53. Fogiel AZ, Hermes J, Rakoczy H, Diesendruck G. 2023.. Infants’ biased individuation of in-group members. . Cognition 239::105561. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2023.105561
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  54. Fotopoulou A, Tsakiris M. 2017.. Mentalizing homeostasis: the social origins of interoceptive inference. . Neuropsychoanalysis 19:(1):328. https://doi.org/10.1080/15294145.2017.1294031
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  55. Fox MD, Snyder AZ, Vincent JL, Corbetta M, Van Essen DC, Raichle ME. 2005.. The human brain is intrinsically organized into dynamic, anticorrelated functional networks. . PNAS 102:(27):967378. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0504136102
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  56. Gallagher S, Meltzoff AN. 1996.. The earliest sense of self and others: Merleau-Ponty and recent developmental studies. . Philos. Psychol. 9:(2):21133. https://doi.org/10.1080/09515089608573181
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  57. Gallup GG. 1970.. Chimpanzees: self-recognition. . Science 167:(3914):8687. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.167.3914.86
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  58. Gao W, Alcauter S, Elton A, Hernandez-Castillo CR, Smith JK, et al. 2015.. Functional network development during the first year: relative sequence and socioeconomic correlations. . Cereb. Cortex 25:(9):291928. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhu088
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  59. Gao W, Lin W, Grewen K, Gilmore JH. 2017.. Functional connectivity of the infant human brain: plastic and modifiable. . Neuroscientist 23:(2):16984. https://doi.org/10.1177/1073858416635986
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  60. Gao W, Zhu H, Giovanello KS, Smith JK, Shen D, et al. 2009.. Evidence on the emergence of the brain's default network from 2-week-old to 2-year-old healthy pediatric subjects. . PNAS 106:(16):679095. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0811221106
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  61. Gergely G, Watson JS. 1999.. Early socio–emotional development: contingency perception and the social–biofeedback model. . In Social Cognition: Understanding Others in the First Months of Life, ed. P Rochat, pp. 10136. Mahwah, NJ:: Lawrence Erlbaum
    [Google Scholar]
  62. Gliga T, Southgate V. 2016.. Metacognition: pre-verbal infants adapt their behaviour to their knowledge states. . Curr. Biol. 26:(22):R119193. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2016.09.065
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  63. Goulden N, Khusnulina A, Davis NJ, Bracewell RM, Bokde AL, et al. 2014.. The salience network is responsible for switching between the default mode network and the central executive network: replication from DCM. . NeuroImage 99::18090. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.05.052
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  64. Goupil L, Kouider S. 2016.. Behavioral and neural indices of metacognitive sensitivity in preverbal infants. . Curr. Biol. 26:(22):303845. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2016.09.004
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  65. Grosse Wiesmann C, Rothmaler K, Habdank K, Hasan E, Yang C, et al. 2024.. From other-reference to a self-reference effect – Reversal of the classic memory bias in human infants. . PsyArXiv. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/my4jg
  66. Grossmann T, Johnson MH, Lloyd-Fox S, Blasi A, Deligianni F, et al. 2008.. Early cortical specialization for face-to-face communication in human infants. . Proc. R. Soc. B 275:( 1653.):280311. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2008.0986
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  67. Gusnard DA, Akbudak E, Shulman GL, Raichle ME. 2001.. Medial prefrontal cortex and self-referential mental activity: relation to a default mode of brain function. . PNAS 98:(7):425964. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.071043098
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  68. Haggard P. 2005.. Conscious intention and motor cognition. . Trends Cogn. Sci. 9:(6):29095. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2005.04.012
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  69. Hamlin JK, Mahajan N, Liberman Z, Wynn K. 2013.. Not like me = bad: Infants prefer those who harm dissimilar others. . Psychol. Sci. 24:(4):58994. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797612457785
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  70. Heyes CM. 1994.. Reflections on self-recognition in primates. . Anim. Behav. 47:(4):90919. https://doi.org/10.1006/anbe.1994.1123
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  71. Hood B. 2012.. The Self Illusion: Why There is No ‘You’ Inside Your Head. London:: Hachette
    [Google Scholar]
  72. Imafuku M, Yoshimoto H, Hiraki K. 2023.. Infants’ interoception is associated with eye contact in dyadic social interactions. . Sci. Rep. 13:(1):9520. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-023-35851-9
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  73. Ishikawa M, Itakura S. 2019.. Physiological arousal predicts gaze following in infants. . Proc. R. Soc. B 286:(1896):20182746. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2018.2746
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  74. Isomura T, Watanabe K. 2020.. Direct gaze enhances interoceptive accuracy. . Cognition 195::104113. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2019.104113
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  75. James W. 1890.. The Principles of Psychology, Vol. I. New York:: Henry Holt. https://doi.org/10.1037/10538-000
    [Google Scholar]
  76. Kampe KKW, Frith CD, Frith U. 2003.. “Hey John”: Signals conveying communicative intention toward the self activate brain regions associated with “mentalizing,” regardless of modality. . J. Neurosci. 23:(12):525863. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-12-05258.2003
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  77. Kampis D, Askitis D, Southgate V. 2024.. Altercentric bias in infants’ encoding of object kind. . PsyArXiv. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/5fpa4
    [Google Scholar]
  78. Kampis D, Grosse Wiesmann C, Koop S, Southgate V. 2022.. Understanding the self in relation to others: Infants spontaneously map another's face to their own at 16–26 months. . Dev. Sci. 25:(3):e13197. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.13197
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  79. Karmiloff-Smith A. 1994.. Précis of Beyond Modularity: a Developmental Perspective on Cognitive Science. . Behav. Brain Sci. 17:(4):693707. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X00036621
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  80. Kärtner J, Keller H, Chaudhary N, Yovsi RD. 2012.. The development of mirror self-recognition in different sociocultural contexts. . Monogr. Soc. Res. Child Dev. 77:(4):1101. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-5834.2012.00688.x
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  81. Kärtner J, Keller H, Lamm B, Abels M, Yovsi RD, et al. 2008.. Similarities and differences in contingency experiences of 3-month-olds across sociocultural contexts. . Infant Behav. Dev. 31:(3):488500. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2008.01.001
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  82. Keller H, Kärtner J, Borke J, Yovsi R, Kleis A. 2005.. Parenting styles and the development of the categorical self: a longitudinal study on mirror self-recognition in Cameroonian Nso and German families. . Int. J. Behav. Dev. 29:(6):496504. https://doi.org/10.1177/01650250500147485
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  83. Keller H, Yovsi R, Borke J, Kärtner J, Jensen H, Papaligoura Z. 2004.. Developmental consequences of early parenting experiences: self-recognition and self-regulation in three cultural communities. . Child Dev. 75:(6):174560. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00814.x
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  84. Kinzler KD, Dupoux E, Spelke ES. 2007.. The native language of social cognition. . PNAS 104:(30):1257780. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0705345104
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  85. Koós O, Gergely G, Csibra G, Bíro S. 1997.. Why eating smarties makes you smart: understanding false belief at the age of 3. Paper presented at the Society for Research in Child Development Biennial Meeting, Washington, DC:, Mar. 30–Apr. 2
    [Google Scholar]
  86. Koreki A, Goeta D, Ricciardi L, Eilon T, Chen J, et al. 2022.. The relationship between interoception and agency and its modulation by heartbeats: an exploratory study. . Sci. Rep. 12:(1):13624. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-022-16569-6
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  87. Krol KM, Moulder RG, Lillard TS, Grossmann T, Connelly JJ. 2019.. Epigenetic dynamics in infancy and the impact of maternal engagement. . Sci. Adv. 5:(10):eaay0680. https://doi.org/10.1126/sciadv.aay0680
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  88. Lewis M, Brooks-Gunn J. 1979.. Toward a theory of social cognition: the development of self. . New Dir. Child Adolesc. Dev. 1979:(4):120. https://doi.org/10.1002/cd.23219790403
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  89. Lewis M, Ramsay D. 2004.. Development of self-recognition, personal pronoun use, and pretend play during the 2nd year. . Child Dev. 75:(6):182131. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00819.x
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  90. Lewis M, Sullivan MW, Stanger C, Weiss M. 1989.. Self development and self-conscious emotions. . Child Dev. 60:(1):14656. https://doi.org/10.2307/1131080
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  91. Li X, Chen H, Hu Y, Larsen RJ, Sutton BP, et al. 2023.. Functional neural network connectivity at 3 months predicts infant-mother dyadic flexibility during play at 6 months. . Cereb. Cortex 33:(13):832132. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhad117
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  92. Lloyd-Fox S, Széplaki-Köllőd B, Yin J, Csibra G. 2015.. Are you talking to me? Neural activations in 6-month-old infants in response to being addressed during natural interactions. . Cortex 70::3548. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2015.02.005
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  93. Mahajan N, Wynn K. 2012.. Origins of “us” versus “them”: prelinguistic infants prefer similar others. . Cognition 124:(2):22733. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2012.05.003
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  94. Mahler MS, Pine F, Bergman A. 1975.. The Psychological Birth of the Infant. New York:: Basic Books
    [Google Scholar]
  95. Maister L, Hodossy L, Tsakiris M, Shinskey JL. 2020.. Self or (m)other? Infants’ sensitivity to bodily overlap with their mother reflects their dyadic coordination. . Child Dev. 91:(5):163149. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.13361
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  96. Maister L, Sebanz N, Knoblich G, Tsakiris M. 2013.. Experiencing ownership over a dark-skinned body reduces implicit racial bias. . Cognition 128:(2):17078. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2013.04.002
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  97. Maister L, Tang T, Tsakiris M. 2017.. Neurobehavioral evidence of interoceptive sensitivity in early infancy. . eLife 6:e25318. https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.25318
    [Google Scholar]
  98. Manea V, Kampis D, Grosse Wiesmann C, Revencu B, Southgate V. 2023.. An initial but receding altercentric bias in preverbal infants’ memory. . Proc. R. Soc. B 290::20230738. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2023.0738
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  99. Margulies DS, Ghosh SS, Goulas A, Falkiewicz M, Huntenburg JM, et al. 2016.. Situating the default-mode network along a principal gradient of macroscale cortical organization. . PNAS 113:(44):1257479. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1608282113
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  100. Markus HR, Kitayama S. 1991.. Culture and the self: implications for cognition, emotion, and motivation. . Psychol. Rev. 98:(2):22453. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.98.2.224
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  101. Mead GH. 1934.. Mind, Self and Society. Chicago:: Univ. Chicago Press
    [Google Scholar]
  102. Meltzoff AN. 1990.. Foundations for developing a concept of self: the role of imitation in relating self to other and the value of social mirroring, social modeling, and self practice in infancy. . In The Self in Transition: Infancy to Childhood, ed. D Cicchetti, M Beeghly , pp. 13964. Chicago:: Univ. Chicago Press
    [Google Scholar]
  103. Meltzoff AN. 2007.. ‘ Like me’: a foundation for social cognition. . Dev. Sci. 10:(1):12634. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00574.x
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  104. Meyer M, Hunnius S. 2021.. Neural processing of self-produced and externally generated events in 3-month-old infants. . J. Exp. Child Psychol. 204::105039. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2020.105039
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  105. Miles LK, Nind LK, Henderson Z, Macrae CN. 2010.. Moving memories: behavioral synchrony and memory for self and others. . J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 46:(2):45760. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2009.12.006
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  106. Miyazaki M, Hiraki K. 2006.. Delayed intermodal contingency affects young children's recognition of their current self. . Child Dev. 77:(3):73650. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2006.00900.x
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  107. Molnar-Szakacs I, Uddin LQ. 2013.. Self-processing and the default mode network: interactions with the mirror neuron system. . Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7::571. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2013.00571
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  108. Musholt K. 2015.. Thinking about Onself: From Nonconceptual Content to the Concept of Self. Cambridge, MA:: MIT Press
    [Google Scholar]
  109. Nabokov V. 2012.. Strong Opinions. . London:: Penguin
  110. Neisser U. 1991.. Two perceptually given aspects of the self and their development. . Dev. Rev. 11:(3):197209. https://doi.org/10.1016/0273-2297(91)90009-D
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  111. Nishizato M, Fujisawa TX, Kosaka H, Tomoda A. 2017.. Developmental changes in social attention and oxytocin levels in infants and children. . Sci. Rep. 7:(1):2540. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-017-02368-x
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  112. Paladino M-P, Mazzurega M, Pavani F, Schubert TW. 2010.. Synchronous multisensory stimulation blurs self-other boundaries. . Psychol. Sci. 21:(9):12027. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797610379234
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  113. Park H-D, Bernasconi F, Bello-Ruiz J, Pfeiffer C, Salomon R, Blanke O. 2016.. Transient modulations of neural responses to heartbeats covary with bodily self-consciousness. . J. Neurosci. 36:(32):845360. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0311-16.2016
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  114. Park H-D, Tallon-Baudry C. 2014.. The neural subjective frame: from bodily signals to perceptual consciousness. . Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B 369:(1641):20130208. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2013.0208
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  115. Petrović L, Cebioğlu S. 2023.. Mirror self-recognition in Ni-Vanuatu toddlers. Paper presented at the Budapest CEU Conference on Cognitive Development, Budapest, Hungary:, Jan. 5–7
    [Google Scholar]
  116. Piaget J. 1999.. Play, Dreams, and Imitation in Childhood. London:: Routledge
    [Google Scholar]
  117. Povinelli DJ, Landau KR, Perilloux HK. 1996.. Self-recognition in young children using delayed versus live feedback: evidence of a developmental asynchrony. . Child Dev. 67:(4):154054. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.1996.tb01813.x
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  118. Qin P, Northoff G. 2011.. How is our self related to midline regions and the default-mode network?. NeuroImage 57:(3):122133. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.05.028
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  119. Ray E, Heyes C. 2011.. Imitation in infancy: the wealth of the stimulus. . Dev. Sci. 14:(1):92105. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2010.00961.x
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  120. Reddy V. 2003.. On being the object of attention: implications for self–other consciousness. . Trends Cogn. Sci. 7:(9):397402. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(03)00191-8
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  121. Richman AL, Miller PM, LeVine RA. 1992.. Cultural and educational variations in maternal responsiveness. . Dev. Psychol. 28:(4):61421. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.28.4.614
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  122. Riebner R, Carton A, Wollock J, eds. 1998.. The Collected Works of L.S. Vygotsky, Vol. 5: Child Psychology . New York:: Plenum Press
    [Google Scholar]
  123. Rochat P. 2003.. Five levels of self-awareness as they unfold early in life. . Conscious. Cogn. 12:(4):71731. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1053-8100(03)00081-3
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  124. Rochat P. 2009.. Others in Mind: Social Origins of Self-Consciousness. New York:: Cambridge Univ. Press
    [Google Scholar]
  125. Rochat P, Hespos SJ. 1997.. Differential rooting response by neonates: evidence for an early sense of self. . Early Dev. Parent. 6:(3–4):10512. https://doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1099-0917(199709/12)6:3/4<105::AID-EDP150>3.0.CO;2-U
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  126. Rochat P, Morgan R. 1995.. Spatial determinants in the perception of self-produced leg movements in 3- to 5-month-old infants. . Dev. Psychol. 31:(4):62636. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.31.4.626
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  127. Rochat P, Striano T. 1999.. Emerging self-exploration by 2-month-old infants. . Dev. Sci. 2:(2):20618. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-7687.00069
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  128. Rondeel E, Van Steenbergen H, Holland R, van Knippenberg A. 2015.. A closer look at cognitive control: differences in resource allocation during updating, inhibition and switching as revealed by pupillometry. . Front. Hum. Neurosci. 9::494. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2015.00494
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  129. Ross J, Anderson JR, Campbell RN, Collins WA. 2011.. I remember me: mnemonic self-reference effects in preschool children. . Monogr. Soc. Res. Child Dev. 76:(3):1102
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  130. Ross J, Yilmaz M, Dale R, Cassidy R, Yildirim I, Zeedyk MS. 2017.. Cultural differences in self-recognition: the early development of autonomous and related selves?. Dev. Sci. 20:(3):e12387. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12387
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  131. Rothbaum F, Pott M, Azuma H, Miyake K, Weisz J. 2000.. The development of close relationships in Japan and the United States: paths of symbiotic harmony and generative tension. . Child Dev. 71:(5):112142. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00214
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  132. Rovee CK, Rovee DT. 1969.. Conjugate reinforcement of infant exploratory behavior. . J. Exp. Child Psychol. 8:(1):3339. https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-0965(69)90025-3
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  133. Samson D, Apperly IA, Kathirgamanathan U, Humphreys GW. 2005.. Seeing it my way: a case of a selective deficit in inhibiting self-perspective. . Brain 128:(5):110211. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awh464
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  134. Santesso DL, Schmidt LA, Trainor LJ. 2007.. Frontal brain electrical activity (EEG) and heart rate in response to affective infant-directed (ID) speech in 9-month-old infants. . Brain Cogn. 65:(1):1421. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2007.02.008
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  135. Schilbach L, Bzdok D, Timmermans B, Fox PT, Laird AR, et al. 2012.. Introspective minds: using ALE meta-analyses to study commonalities in the neural correlates of emotional processing, social & unconstrained cognition. . PLOS ONE 7:(2):e30920. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0030920
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  136. Schmuckler MA. 1996.. Visual-proprioceptive intermodal perception in infancy. . Infant Behav. Dev. 19:(2):22132. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0163-6383(96)90021-1
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  137. Sel A, Azevedo RT, Tsakiris M. 2017.. Heartfelt self: cardio-visual integration affects self-face recognition and interoceptive cortical processing. . Cereb. Cortex 27:(11):514455. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhw296
    [Google Scholar]
  138. Sforza A, Bufalari I, Haggard P, Aglioti SM. 2010.. My face in yours: visuo-tactile facial stimulation influences sense of identity. . Soc. Neurosci. 5:(2):14862. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470910903205503
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  139. Simner ML. 1971.. Newborn's response to the cry of another infant. . Dev. Psychol. 5:(1):13650. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0031066
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  140. Smallwood J, Karapanagiotidis T, Ruby F, Medea B, de Caso I, et al. 2016.. Representing representation: integration between the temporal lobe and the posterior cingulate influences the content and form of spontaneous thought. . PLOS ONE 11:(4):e0152272. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0152272
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  141. Soley G, Sebastián-Gallés N. 2015.. Infants prefer tunes previously introduced by speakers of their native language. . Child Dev. 86:(6):168592. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12408
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  142. Southgate V. 2020.. Are infants altercentric? The other and the self in early social cognition. . Psychol. Rev. 127:(4):50523. https://doi.org/10.1037/rev0000182
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  143. Suddendorf T, Butler DL. 2013.. The nature of visual self-recognition. . Trends Cogn. Sci. 17:(3):12127. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2013.01.004
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  144. Sueyoshi T, Sugimoto F, Katayama J, Fukushima H. 2014.. Neural correlates of error processing reflect individual differences in interoceptive sensitivity. . Int. J. Psychophysiol. 94:(3):27886. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2014.10.001
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  145. Supekar K, Uddin LQ, Prater K, Amin H, Greicius MD, Menon V. 2010.. Development of functional and structural connectivity within the default mode network in young children. . NeuroImage 52:(1):290301. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.04.009
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  146. Symons CS, Johnson BT. 1997.. The self-reference effect in memory: a meta-analysis. . Psychol. Bull. 121:(3):37194. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.121.3.371
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  147. Tajadura-Jiménez A, Grehl S, Tsakiris M. 2012.. The other in me: interpersonal multisensory stimulation changes the mental representation of the self. . PLOS ONE 7:(7):e40682. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0040682
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  148. Tajfel H. 1981.. Human Groups and Social Categories: Studies in Social Psychology. Cambridge, UK:: Cambridge Univ. Press
    [Google Scholar]
  149. Timmermans B, Schilbach L, Pasquali A, Cleeremans A. 2012.. Higher order thoughts in action: consciousness as an unconscious re-description process. . Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B 367:(1594):141223. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2011.0421
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  150. Tomasello M. 1999.. The Cultural Origins of Human Cognition. Cambridge, MA:: Harvard Univ. Press
    [Google Scholar]
  151. Tomova L, Heinrichs M, Lamm C. 2019.. The other and me: effects of oxytocin on self-other distinction. . Int. J. Psychophysiol. 136::4953. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2018.03.008
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  152. Tsakiris M, Jiménez AT, Costantini M. 2011.. Just a heartbeat away from one's body: Interoceptive sensitivity predicts malleability of body-representations. . Proc. R. Soc. B 278:(1717):247076. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2010.2547
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  153. Wang L, Gu Y, Zhao G, Chen A. 2020.. Error-related negativity and error awareness in a Go/No-go task. . Sci. Rep. 10::4026. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-60693-0
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  154. Watanabe H, Taga G. 2006.. General to specific development of movement patterns and memory for contingency between actions and events in young infants. . Infant Behav. Dev. 29:(3):40222. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2006.02.001
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  155. Watson J. 1972.. Smiling, cooing, and “the game.”. Merrill-Palmer Q. Behav. Dev. 18:(4):32339
    [Google Scholar]
  156. Wellman HM, Cross D, Watson J. 2001.. Meta-analysis of theory-of-mind development: the truth about false belief. . Child Dev. 72:(3):65584. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00304
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  157. Wiltermuth SS, Heath C. 2009.. Synchrony and cooperation. . Psychol. Sci. 20:(1):15. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02253.x
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  158. Yeung E, Askitis D, Manea V, Southgate V. 2022.. Emerging self-representation presents a challenge when perspectives conflict. . Open Mind 6::23249. https://doi.org/10.1162/opmi_a_00065
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  159. Yeung E, Askitis D, Southgate V. 2024.. Developing self-awareness generates perspective conflict: Evidence in the infant brain. . PsyArXiv. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/6ytfw
  160. Yue T, Jiang Y, Yue C, Huang X. 2017.. Differential effects of oxytocin on visual perspective taking for men and women. . Front. Behav. Neurosci. 11::228. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2017.00228
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  161. Zhang Y, Wang F, Sui J. 2023.. Decoding individual differences in self-prioritization from the resting-state functional connectome. . NeuroImage 276::120205. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2023.120205
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  162. Zhao W, Yao S, Li Q, Geng Y, Ma X, et al. 2016.. Oxytocin blurs the self-other distinction during trait judgments and reduces medial prefrontal cortex responses. . Hum. Brain Mapp. 37:(7):251227. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.23190
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
/content/journals/10.1146/annurev-devpsych-120621-025747
Loading
This is a required field
Please enter a valid email address
Approval was a Success
Invalid data
An Error Occurred
Approval was partially successful, following selected items could not be processed due to error