1932

Abstract

There is increasing interest in the way that the size, composition, and environment of populations influence the way that their languages evolve. There are two reasons why an exploration of population and language change from the perspective of evolutionary biology might be useful. First, some of the relevant hypotheses rest explicitly or implicitly on theories developed in evolutionary biology, so it is important to critically evaluate the fit of these theories to language change. Second, methods developed in evolutionary biology have been applied to evaluating these hypotheses. Instead of aiming for a comprehensive review of the interaction between population size and language change, the focus of this review is on analogies drawn to processes in biological evolution (e.g., founder effects), processes that may have interesting parallels in both species and languages (e.g., evolution of complexity in small populations), and techniques from evolutionary biology that have been applied to language data (e.g., Wright-Fisher models).

Loading

Article metrics loading...

/content/journals/10.1146/annurev-linguistics-031422-123959
2025-02-03
2025-06-17
Loading full text...

Full text loading...

/deliver/fulltext/linguistics/11/1/annurev-linguistics-031422-123959.html?itemId=/content/journals/10.1146/annurev-linguistics-031422-123959&mimeType=html&fmt=ahah

Literature Cited

  1. Atkinson QD. 2011.. Phonemic diversity supports a serial founder effect model of language expansion from Africa. . Science 332::34649
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  2. Atkinson QD. 2012.. Response to comments on “Phonemic diversity supports a serial founder effect model of language expansion from Africa. .” Science 335::657
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  3. Atkinson QD, Meade A, Venditti C, Greenhill SJ, Pagel M. 2008.. Languages evolve in punctuational bursts. . Science 319::588
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  4. Barbieri C, Blasi DE, Arango-Isaza E, Sotiropoulos AG, Hammarström H, et al. 2022.. A global analysis of matches and mismatches between human genetic and linguistic histories. . PNAS 119::e2122084119
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  5. Baxter GJ, Blythe RA, Croft W, McKane AJ. 2006.. Utterance selection model of language change. . Phys. Rev. E 73::046118
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  6. Bentz C, Dediu D, Verkerk A, Jäger G. 2018.. The evolution of language families is shaped by the environment beyond neutral drift. . Nat. Hum. Behav. 2::81621
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  7. Bentz C, Verkerk A, Kiela D, Hill F, Buttery P. 2015.. Adaptive communication: Languages with more non-native speakers tend to have fewer word forms. . PLOS ONE 10::e0128254
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  8. Bentz C, Winter B. 2013.. Languages with more second language learners tend to lose nominal case. . Lang. Dyn. Change 3::127
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  9. Bickel B. 2013.. Distributional biases in language families. . In Language Typology and Historical Contingency: In Honor of Johanna Nichols, ed. B Bickel, LA Grenoble, DA Peterson, A Timberlake , pp. 41544. Amsterdam:: John Benjamins
    [Google Scholar]
  10. Blythe RA, Croft W. 2012.. S-curves and the mechanisms of propagation in language change. . Language 88::269304
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  11. Bowern C. 2010.. Correlates of language change in hunter-gatherer and other ‘small’ languages. . Lang. Linguist. Compass 4::66579
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  12. Bowern C. 2011.. Out of Africa? The logic of phoneme inventories and founder effects. . Linguist. Typology 15::20716
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  13. Brentari D, Ergin R, Senghas A, Cho PW, Owens E, Coppola M. 2021.. Community interactions and phonemic inventories in emerging sign languages. . Phonology 38::571609
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  14. Bromham L. 2016.. Testing hypotheses in macroevolution. . Stud. Hist. Philos. Sci. Part A 55::4759
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  15. Bromham L. 2017.. Curiously the same: swapping tools between linguistics and evolutionary biology. . Biol. Philos. 32::85586
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  16. Bromham L. 2023a.. Language endangerment: using analytical methods from conservation biology to illuminate loss of linguistic diversity. . Cambridge Prisms: Extinction 1::e3
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  17. Bromham L. 2023b.. Meaning and purpose: using phylogenies to investigate human history and cultural evolution. . Biol. Theory 18::284302
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  18. Bromham L. 2024a.. Combining molecular, macroevolutionary, and macroecological perspectives on the generation of diversity. . Cold Spring Harb. Perspect. Biol. 16::a041453
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  19. Bromham L. 2024b.. Solving Galton's problem: practical solutions for analysing language diversity and evolution. . In Historical Linguistics 2019: Selected Papers from the 24th International Conference on Historical Linguistics, Canberra, 1–5 July 2019, ed. B Evans, MK Gallego, L Miceli , pp. 74108. Amsterdam:: John Benjamins
    [Google Scholar]
  20. Bromham L, Dinnage R, Skirgård H, Ritchie A, Cardillo M, et al. 2022.. Global predictors of language endangerment and the future of linguistic diversity. . Nat. Ecol. Evol. 6::16373
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  21. Bromham L, Duchêne S, Hua X, Ritchie AM, Duchêne DA, Ho SY. 2018a.. Bayesian molecular dating: opening up the black box. . Biol. Rev. 93::116591
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  22. Bromham L, Hua X, Algy C, Meakins F. 2020.. Language endangerment: a multidimensional analysis of risk factors. . J. Lang. Evol. 5::7591
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  23. Bromham L, Hua X, Cardillo M, Schneemann H, Greenhill SJ. 2018b.. Parasites and politics: why cross-cultural studies must control for relatedness, proximity and covariation. . R. Soc. Open Sci. 5::181100
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  24. Bromham L, Hua X, Fitzpatrick TG, Greenhill SJ. 2015.. Rate of language evolution is affected by population size. . PNAS 112::2097102
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  25. Bromham L, Yaxley KJ. 2023.. Neighbours and relatives: accounting for spatial distribution when testing causal hypotheses in cultural evolution. . Evol. Hum. Sci. 5::e27
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  26. Bromham L, Yaxley KJ, Cardillo M. 2024.. Islands are engines of language diversity. . Nat. Ecol. Evol. 8::19912002
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  27. Brunet TDP, Doolittle WF. 2018.. The generality of Constructive Neutral Evolution. . Biol. Philos. 33::2
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  28. Butcher AR. 2018.. The special nature of Australian phonologies: why auditory constraints on human language sound systems are not universal. . Proc. Meet. Acoust. 35::060004
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  29. Bybee J. 2011.. How plausible is the hypothesis that population size and dispersal are related to phoneme inventory size? Introducing and commenting on a debate. . Linguist. Typology 15::14753
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  30. Charlesworth B. 2009.. Effective population size and patterns of molecular evolution and variation. . Nat. Rev. Genet. 10::195205
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  31. Dale R, Lupyan G. 2012.. Understanding the origins of morphological diversity: the linguistic niche hypothesis. . Adv. Complex Syst. 15::1150017
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  32. Dediu D. 2021.. Tone and genes: New cross-linguistic data and methods support the weak negative effect of the “derived” allele of ASPM on tone, but not of Microcephalin. . PLOS ONE 16::e0253546
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  33. Dediu D, Janssen R, Moisik SR. 2017.. Language is not isolated from its wider environment: vocal tract influences on the evolution of speech and language. . Lang. Commun. 54::920
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  34. Duchêne DA, Hua X, Bromham L. 2017.. Phylogenetic estimates of diversification rate are affected by molecular rate variation. . J. Evol. Biol. 30::188497
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  35. Ellison TM, Miceli L. 2017.. Language monitoring in bilinguals as a mechanism for rapid lexical divergence. . Language 93::25587
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  36. Everett C. 2013.. Evidence for direct geographic influences on linguistic sounds: the case of ejectives. . PLOS ONE 8::e65275
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  37. Everett C, Blasi DE, Roberts SG. 2016.. Language evolution and climate: the case of desiccation and tone. . J. Lang. Evol. 1::3346
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  38. Everett C, Chen S. 2021.. Speech adapts to differences in dentition within and across populations. . Sci. Rep. 11::1066
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  39. Eyre-Walker A, Keightley PD. 2007.. The distribution of fitness effects of new mutations. . Nat. Rev. Genet. 8::61018
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  40. Felsenstein J. 1985.. Phylogenies and the comparative method. . Am. Naturalist 125::115
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  41. Fernández A, Lynch M. 2011.. Non-adaptive origins of interactome complexity. . Nature 474::5025
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  42. Fisher RA. 1930.. The Genetical Theory of Natural Selection. Oxford, UK:: Clarendon
    [Google Scholar]
  43. Fitch WM, Ayala FJ. 1994.. Tempo and mode in evolution. . PNAS 91::671720
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  44. Fort J, Pérez-Losada J. 2016.. Can a linguistic serial founder effect originating in Africa explain the worldwide phonemic cline?. J. R. Soc. Interface 13::20160185
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  45. Galton F. 1889.. Comment on ‘On a Method of Investigating the Development of Institutions; Applied to Laws of Marriage and Descent’ by E. B. Tylor. . J. Anthropol. Inst. G.B. Irel. 18::24572
    [Google Scholar]
  46. Garamszegi LZ. 2014.. Modern Phylogenetic Comparative Methods and Their Application in Evolutionary Biology: Concepts and Practice. Berlin:: Springer
    [Google Scholar]
  47. Gavin MC, Botero CA, Bowern C, Colwell RK, Dunn M, et al. 2013.. Toward a mechanistic understanding of linguistic diversity. . BioScience 63::52435
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  48. Gray RD, Atkinson QD, Greenhill SJ. 2011.. Language evolution and human history: what a difference a date makes. . Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B 366::1090100
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  49. Gray RD, Drummond AJ, Greenhill SJ. 2009.. Language phylogenies reveal expansion pulses and pauses in Pacific settlement. . Science 323::47983
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  50. Greenhill SJ. 2015.. Demographic correlates of language diversity. . In The Routledge Handbook of Historical Linguistics, ed. C Bowern, B Evans , pp. 55778. Abingdon, UK:: Routledge
    [Google Scholar]
  51. Greenhill SJ, Hua X, Welsh CF, Schneemann H, Bromham L. 2018.. Population size and the rate of language evolution: a test across Indo-European, Austronesian, and Bantu languages. . Front. Psychol. 9::00576
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  52. Greenhill SJ, Wu C-H, Hua X, Dunn M, Levinson SC, Gray RD. 2017.. Evolutionary dynamics of language systems. . PNAS 114::E882229
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  53. Hamilton MJ, Walker RS. 2019.. Nonlinear diversification rates of linguistic phylogenies over the Holocene. . PLOS ONE 14::e0213126
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  54. Hammarström H. 2016.. Commentary: There is no demonstrable effect of desiccation. . J. Lang. Evol. 1::6569
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  55. Hartmann F, Roberts SG, Valdes P, Grollemund R. 2024.. Investigating environmental effects on phonology using diachronic models. . Evol. Hum. Sci. 6::e8
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  56. Harvey PH, Pagel MD. 1991.. The Comparative Method in Evolutionary Biology. Oxford, UK:: Oxford Univ. Press
    [Google Scholar]
  57. Hay J, Bauer L. 2007.. Phoneme inventory size and population size. . Language 83::388400
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  58. Hoffmann K, Bouckaert R, Greenhill SJ, Kühnert D. 2021.. Bayesian phylogenetic analysis of linguistic data using BEAST. . J. Lang. Evol. 6::11935
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  59. Holman EW, Wichmann S. 2017.. New evidence from linguistic phylogenetics identifies limits to punctuational change. . Syst. Biol. 66::60410
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  60. Hou L, de Vos C. 2022.. Classifications and typologies: labeling sign languages and signing communities. . J. Socioling. 26::11825
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  61. Hua X. 2022.. BayesVarbrul: a unified multidimensional analysis of language change in a speaker community. . J. Lang. Evol. 7::4052
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  62. Hua X, Bromham L. 2017.. Darwinism for the genomic age: connecting mutation to diversification. . Front. Genet. 8::12
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  63. Hua X, Bromham L. 2018.. Fitness. . In Encyclopedia of Ecology, , 1029. Amsterdam:: Elsevier. 2nd ed.
    [Google Scholar]
  64. Hua X, Greenhill SJ, Cardillo M, Schneemann H, Bromham L. 2019.. The ecological drivers of variation in global language diversity. . Nat. Commun. 10::2047
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  65. Hunley K, Bowern C, Healy M. 2012.. Rejection of a serial founder effects model of genetic and linguistic coevolution. . Proc. R. Soc. B 279::228188
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  66. Ishida Y, Rosales A. 2020.. The origins of the stochastic theory of population genetics: the Wright-Fisher model. . Stud. Hist. Philos. Sci. C 79::101226
    [Google Scholar]
  67. Kandler A. 2009.. Demography and language competition. . Hum. Biol. 81::181210
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  68. Karjus A, Blythe R, Kirby S, Smith K. 2020.. Challenges in detecting evolutionary forces in language change using diachronic corpora. . Glossa 5:(1):45
    [Google Scholar]
  69. Kauhanen H, Einhaus S, Walkden G. 2023.. Language structure is influenced by the proportion of non-native speakers: a reply to Koplenig (2019). . J. Lang. Evol. 8::90101
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  70. Kimura M. 1979.. The neutral theory of molecular evolution. . Sci. Am. 241::98129
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  71. Kimura M. 1983.. The Neutral Theory of Molecular Evolution. Cambridge, UK:: Cambridge Univ. Press
    [Google Scholar]
  72. Kirby S. 1998.. Fitness and the selective adaptation of language. . In Approaches to the Evolution of Language: Social and Cognitive Bases, ed. JR Hurford, M Studdert-Kennedy, C Knight , pp. 35983. Cambridge, UK:: Cambridge Univ. Press
    [Google Scholar]
  73. Koplenig A. 2019.. Language structure is influenced by the number of speakers but seemingly not by the proportion of non-native speakers. . R. Soc. Open Sci. 6::181274
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  74. Kortmann B, Szmrecsanyi B, eds. 2012.. Linguistic Complexity: Second Language Acquisition, Indigenization, Contact. Berlin:: De Gruyter
    [Google Scholar]
  75. Kummu M, Varis O. 2011.. The world by latitudes: a global analysis of human population, development level and environment across the north–south axis over the past half century. . Appl. Geogr. 31::495507
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  76. Labov W. 1965.. On the mechanism of linguistic change. . Georget. Monogr. Lang. Linguist. 18::91114
    [Google Scholar]
  77. Ladoukakis ED, Michelioudakis D, Anagnostopoulou E. 2022.. Toward an evolutionary framework for language variation and change. . BioEssays 44::2100216
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  78. Lanfear R, Kokko H, Eyre-Walker A. 2014.. Population size and the rate of evolution. . Trends Ecol. Evol. 29::3341
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  79. Levinson SC, Gray RD. 2012.. Tools from evolutionary biology shed new light on the diversification of languages. . Trends Cogn. Sci. 16::16773
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  80. Liang Y, Wang L, Wichmann S, Xia Q, Wang S, et al. 2023.. Languages in China link climate, voice quality, and tone in a causal chain. . Humanit. Soc. Sci. Commun. 10::453
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  81. Lupyan G, Dale R. 2010.. Language structure is partly determined by social structure. . PLOS ONE 5::e8559
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  82. Lupyan G, Dale R. 2016.. Why are there different languages? The role of adaptation in linguistic diversity. . Trends Cogn. Sci. 20::64960
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  83. Lynch M, Conery JS. 2003.. The origins of genome complexity. . Science 302::14014
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  84. Mace R, Pagel M. 1995.. A latitudinal gradient in the density of human languages in North America. . Proc. R. Soc. London B 261::11721
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  85. Maddieson I. 2013.. Tone. . In WALS Online, version 2020.3, ed. MS Dryer, M Haspelmath. http://wals.info/chapter/13 . Leipzig, Ger:.: Max Planck Inst. Evol. Anthropol.
    [Google Scholar]
  86. Maddieson I. 2018.. Language adapts to environment: sonority and temperature. . Front. Commun. 3::00028
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  87. Maddieson I, Benedict K. 2023.. Demonstrating environmental impacts on the sound structure of languages: challenges and solutions. . Front. Psychol. 14::1200463
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  88. Maddieson I, Bhattacharya T, Smith DE, Croft W. 2011.. Geographical distribution of phonological complexity. . Linguist. Typology 15::26779
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  89. Masel J. 2011.. Genetic drift. . Curr. Biol. 21::R83738
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  90. Matsumae H, Ranacher P, Savage PE, Blasi DE, Currie TE, et al. 2021.. Exploring correlations in genetic and cultural variation across language families in northeast Asia. . Sci. Adv. 7::eabd9223
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  91. Meakins F, Hua X, Algy C, Bromham L. 2019.. Birth of a contact language did not favor simplification. . Language 95::294332
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  92. Milroy J, Milroy L. 1985.. Linguistic change, social network and speaker innovation. . J. Linguist. 21::33984
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  93. Mithun M. 2015.. Morphological complexity and language contact in languages indigenous to North America. . Linguist. Discov. 13::3759
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  94. Mithun M. 2017.. Polysynthesis in North America. . In The Oxford Handbook of Polysynthesis, ed. M Fortescue, M Mithun, N Evans , pp. 23559. Oxford, UK:: Oxford Univ. Press
    [Google Scholar]
  95. Moisik SR, Dediu D. 2017.. Anatomical biasing and clicks: evidence from biomechanical modeling. . J. Lang. Evol. 2::3751
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  96. Moran S, McCloy D, Wright R. 2012.. Revisiting population size versus phoneme inventory size. . Language 88::87793
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  97. Nettle D. 1998.. Explaining global patterns of language diversity. . J. Anthropol. Archaeol. 17::35474
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  98. Nettle D. 1999.. Is the rate of linguistic change constant?. Lingua 108::11936
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  99. Nettle D. 2012.. Social scale and structural complexity in human languages. . Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B 367::182936
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  100. Newberry MG, Ahern CA, Clark R, Plotkin JB. 2017.. Detecting evolutionary forces in language change. . Nature 551::22326
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  101. Ohta T. 1972.. Population size and rate of evolution. . J. Mol. Evol. 1::30514
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  102. Ohta T. 1992.. The nearly neutral theory of molecular evolution. . Annu. Rev. Ecol. Syst. 23::26386
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  103. Otto SP, Whitlock MC. 1997.. The probability of fixation in populations of changing size. . Genetics 146::72333
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  104. Padilla-Iglesias C, Gjesfjeld E, Vinicius L. 2020.. Geographical and social isolation drive the evolution of Austronesian languages. . PLOS ONE 15::e0243171
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  105. Pagel M, Atkinson QD, Meade A. 2007.. Frequency of word-use predicts rates of lexical evolution throughout Indo-European history. . Nature 449::71720
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  106. Pagel M, Venditti C, Meade A. 2006.. Large punctuational contribution of speciation to evolutionary divergence at the molecular level. . Science 314::11921
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  107. Pakendorf B, Dobrushina N, Khanina O. 2021.. A typology of small-scale multilingualism. . Int. J. Bilingualism 25::83559
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  108. Reali F, Chater N, Christiansen MH. 2018.. Simpler grammar, larger vocabulary: how population size affects language. . Proc. R. Soc. B 285::20172586
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  109. Reali F, Griffiths TL. 2010.. Words as alleles: connecting language evolution with Bayesian learners to models of genetic drift. . Proc. R. Soc. B 277::42936
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  110. Ritchie AM, Hua X, Bromham L. 2022.. Investigating the reliability of molecular estimates of evolutionary time when substitution rates and speciation rates vary. . BMC Ecol. Evol. 22::61
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  111. Ritchie AM, Hua X, Cardillo M, Yaxley KJ, Dinnage R, Bromham L. 2021.. Phylogenetic diversity metrics from molecular phylogenies: modelling expected degree of error under realistic rate variation. . Divers. Distrib. 27::16478
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  112. Shcherbakova O, Michaelis SM, Haynie HJ, Passmore S, Gast V, et al. 2023.. Societies of strangers do not speak less complex languages. . Sci. Adv. 9::eadf7704
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  113. Simpson GG. 1984.. Tempo and Mode in Evolution. New York:: Columbia Univ. Press
    [Google Scholar]
  114. Skirgård H, Haynie HJ, Blasi DE, Hammarström H, Collins J, et al. 2023.. Grambank reveals the importance of genealogical constraints on linguistic diversity and highlights the impact of language loss. . Sci. Adv. 9::eadg6175
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  115. Solan Z, Ruppin E, Horn D, Edelman S. 2005.. Evolution of language diversity: why fitness counts. . Language Origins: Perspectives on Evolution, ed. M Tallerman , pp. 35771. Oxford, UK:: Oxford Univ. Press
    [Google Scholar]
  116. Trudgill P. 2004.. Linguistic and social typology: the Austronesian migrations and phoneme inventories. . Linguist. Typology 8::30520
    [Google Scholar]
  117. Trudgill P. 2011.. Sociolinguistic Typology: Social Determinants of Linguistic Complexity. Oxford, UK:: Oxford Univ. Press
    [Google Scholar]
  118. Trudgill P. 2020.. Sociolinguistic typology and the speed of linguistic change. . J. Hist. Socioling. 6::20190015
    [Google Scholar]
  119. Turney PD, Mohammad SM. 2019.. The natural selection of words: finding the features of fitness. . PLOS ONE 14::e0211512
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  120. Urban M, Moran S. 2021.. Altitude and the distributional typology of language structure: ejectives and beyond. . PLOS ONE 16::e0245522
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  121. Venditti C, Pagel M. 2010.. Speciation as an active force in promoting genetic evolution. . Trends Ecol. Evol. 25::1420
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  122. Wang T, Wichmann S, Xia Q, Ran Q. 2023.. Temperature shapes language sonority: revalidation from a large dataset. . PNAS Nexus 2::pgad384
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  123. Wichmann S, Holman EW. 2009.. Population size and rates of language change. . Hum. Biol. 81::25974
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  124. Woolfit M. 2009.. Effective population size and the rate and pattern of nucleotide substitutions. . Biol. Lett. 5::41720
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  125. Wray A, Grace GW. 2007.. The consequences of talking to strangers: evolutionary corollaries of socio-cultural influences on linguistic form. . Lingua 117::54378
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  126. Wright S. 1931.. Evolution in Mendelian populations. . Genetics 16::97159
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
/content/journals/10.1146/annurev-linguistics-031422-123959
Loading
/content/journals/10.1146/annurev-linguistics-031422-123959
Loading

Data & Media loading...

This is a required field
Please enter a valid email address
Approval was a Success
Invalid data
An Error Occurred
Approval was partially successful, following selected items could not be processed due to error