1932

Abstract

Behavioral public policy came to the fore with the introduction of nudging, which aims to steer behavior while maintaining freedom of choice. Responding to critiques of nudging (e.g., that it does not promote agency and relies on benevolent choice architects), other behavioral policy approaches focus on empowering citizens. Here we review boosting, a behavioral policy approach that aims to foster people's agency, self-control, and ability to make informed decisions. It is grounded in evidence from behavioral science showing that human decision making is not as notoriously flawed as the nudging approach assumes. We argue that addressing the challenges of our time—such as climate change, pandemics, and the threats to liberal democracies and human autonomy posed by digital technologies and choice architectures—calls for fostering capable and engaged citizens as a first line of response to complement slower, systemic approaches.

Loading

Article metrics loading...

/content/journals/10.1146/annurev-psych-020924-124753
2025-01-17
2025-04-24
Loading full text...

Full text loading...

/deliver/fulltext/psych/76/1/annurev-psych-020924-124753.html?itemId=/content/journals/10.1146/annurev-psych-020924-124753&mimeType=html&fmt=ahah

Literature Cited

  1. Aikman D, Galesic M, Gigerenzer G, Kapadia S, Katsikopoulos K, et al. 2021.. Taking uncertainty seriously: simplicity versus complexity in financial regulation. . Ind. Corp. Change 30:(2):31745
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  2. Ainslie G. 2021.. Willpower with and without effort. . Behav. Brain Sci. 44::e30
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  3. Auer S, Stocker M, Karras O, Oelen A, D'Souza J, Lorenz AL. 2023.. Organizing scholarly knowledge in the Open Research Knowledge Graph: an open-science platform for FAIR scholarly knowledge. . In Proceedings of the Conference on Research Data Infrastructure, Vol. 1, ed. Y Sure-Vetter, C Goble . Hannover, Ger.:: TIB Open Publ. https://doi.org/10.52825/cordi.v1i.272
    [Google Scholar]
  4. Baltes PB, Staudinger UM, Lindenberger U. 1999.. Lifespan psychology: theory and application to intellectual functioning. . Annu. Rev. Psychol. 50::471507
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  5. Banerjee S, Galizzi MM, John P, Mourato S. 2023.. Sustainable dietary choices improved by reflection before a nudge in an online experiment. . Nat. Sustain. 6:(12):163242
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  6. Banerjee S, Grüne-Yanoff T, John P, Moseley A. 2024.. It's time we put agency into behavioural public policy. . Behav. Public Policy. In press . https://doi.org/10.1017/bpp.2024.6
    [Google Scholar]
  7. Banerjee S, John P. 2024.. Nudge plus: incorporating reflection into behavioral public policy. . Behav. Public Policy 8:(1):6984
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  8. Bauer MS, Damschroder L, Hagedorn H, Smith J, Kilbourne AM. 2015.. An introduction to implementation science for the non-specialist. . BMC Psychol. 3:(1):32
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  9. Berkowitz T, Schaeffer MW, Maloney EA, Peterson L, Gregor C, et al. 2015.. Math at home adds up to achievement in school. . Science 350:(6257):19698
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  10. Binmore K. 2011.. Rational Decisions. Princeton, NJ:: Princeton Univ. Press
    [Google Scholar]
  11. Blagys MD, Hilsenroth MJ. 2002.. Distinctive activities of cognitive-behavioral therapy: a review of the comparative psychotherapy process literature. . Clin. Psychol. Rev. 22:(5):671706
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  12. Braveman P, Egerter S, Williams DR. 2011.. The social determinants of health: coming of age. . Annu. Rev. Public Health 32::38198
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  13. Brighton H, Gigerenzer G. 2015.. The bias bias. . J. Bus. Res. 68:(8):177284
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  14. Brown S, Lent R. 2008.. Handbook of Counseling Psychology. Hoboken, NJ:: Wiley. , 4th ed..
    [Google Scholar]
  15. Brownell KD. 2005.. Does a “toxic” environment make obesity inevitable?. Obes. Manag. 1:(2):5255
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  16. Bryan CJ, Tipton E, Yeager DS. 2021a.. Behavioural science is unlikely to change the world without a heterogeneity revolution. . Nat. Hum. Behav. 5:(8):98089
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  17. Bryan G, Karlan D, Nelson S. 2010.. Commitment devices. . Annu. Rev. Econ. 2::67198
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  18. Bryan ML, Bryce AM, Roberts J. 2021b.. The effect of mental and physical health problems on sickness absence. . Eur. J. Health Econ. 22:(9):151933
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  19. Cadario R, Chandon P. 2020.. Which healthy eating nudges work best? A meta-analysis of field experiments. . Mark. Sci. 39:(3):46586
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  20. Chang W, Chen E, Mellers B, Tetlock P. 2016.. Developing expert political judgment: the impact of training and practice on judgmental accuracy in geopolitical forecasting tournaments. . Judgm. Decis. Mak. 11:(5):50926
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  21. Chater N, Loewenstein G. 2022.. The i-frame and the s-frame: how focusing on individual-level solutions has led behavioral public policy astray. . Behav. Brain Sci. 46::e147
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  22. Conger JA, Kanungo RN. 1988.. The empowerment process: integrating theory and practice. . Acad. Manag. Rev. 13:(3):47182
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  23. Crandall B, Klein GA, Hoffman RR. 2006.. Working Minds: A Practitioner's Guide to Cognitive Task Analysis. Cambridge, MA:: Bradford Books
    [Google Scholar]
  24. Cross A, Sheffield D. 2019.. Mental contrasting for health behaviour change: a systematic review and meta-analysis of effects and moderator variables. . Health Psychol. Rev. 13:(2):20925
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  25. Czubaroff J. 1988.. Criticism and response in the Skinner controversies. . J. Exp. Anal. Behav. 49:(2):32129
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  26. Dallacker M, Hertwig R, Mata J. 2018.. The frequency of family meals and nutritional health in children: a meta-analysis. . Obes. Rev. 19:(5):63853
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  27. Dallacker M, Hertwig R, Mata J. 2019a.. Quality matters: a meta-analysis on components of healthy family meals. . Health Psychol. 38:(12):113749
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  28. Dallacker M, Knobl V, Hertwig R, Mata J. 2023.. Effect of longer family meals on children's fruit and vegetable intake: a randomized clinical trial. . JAMA Netw. Open 6:(4):e236331
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  29. Dallacker M, Mata J, Hertwig R. 2019b.. Toward simple eating rules for the land of plenty. . In Taming Uncertainty, ed. R Hertwig, TJ Pleskac, T Pachur , Center for Adaptive Rationality, pp. 11128. Cambridge, MA:: MIT Press
    [Google Scholar]
  30. DellaVigna S, Linos E. 2022.. RCTs to scale: comprehensive evidence from two nudge units. . Econometrica 90:(1):81116
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  31. Drexler A, Fischer G, Schoar A. 2014.. Keeping it simple: financial literacy and rules of thumb. . Am. Econ. J. Appl. Econ. 6:(2):131
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  32. Duckworth AL, Milkman KL. 2022.. A guide to megastudies. . PNAS Nexus 1:(5):pgac214
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  33. Duckworth AL, Milkman KL, Laibson D. 2018.. Beyond willpower: strategies for reducing failures of self-control. . Psychol. Sci. Public Interest 19:(3):10229
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  34. Edwin Thanarajah S, DiFeliceantonio AG, Albus K, Kuzmanovic B, Rigoux L, et al. 2023.. Habitual daily intake of a sweet and fatty snack modulates reward processing in humans. . Cell Metab. 35::57184
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  35. Eisenstadt M, Liverpool S, Infanti E, Ciuvat RM, Carlsson C. 2021.. Mobile apps that promote emotion regulation, positive mental health, and well-being in the general population: systematic review and meta-analysis. . JMIR Ment. Health 8:(11):e31170
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  36. Ennis G. 2023.. Dark PR: How Corporate Disinformation Undermines Our Health and the Environment. Wakefield, Can:.: Daraja Press
    [Google Scholar]
  37. Ewert B. 2020.. Moving beyond the obsession with nudging individual behaviour: towards a broader understanding of Behavioural Public Policy. . Public Policy Adm. 35:(3):33760
    [Google Scholar]
  38. Fabian M, Pykett J. 2022.. Be happy: navigating normative issues in behavioral and well-being public policy. . Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 17:(1):16982
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  39. Fishbach A, Shen L. 2014.. The explicit and implicit ways of overcoming temptation. . In Dual-Process Theories of the Social Mind, ed. JW Sherman, B Gawronsk, Y Trope , pp. 45467. New York:: Guilford
    [Google Scholar]
  40. Folke T, Bertoldo G, D'Souza D, Alì S, Stablum F, Ruggeri K. 2021.. Boosting promotes advantageous risk-taking. . Humanit. Soc. Sci. Commun. 8:(1):270
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  41. Foltice B. 2017.. How to decrease the amortization bias: experience versus rules. . J. Financ. Educ. 43:(2):27393
    [Google Scholar]
  42. Forscher PS, Wagenmakers EJ, Coles NA, Silan MA, Dutra N, et al. 2023.. The benefits, barriers, and risks of big-team science. . Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 18:(3):60723
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  43. Franklin M, Folke T, Ruggeri K. 2019.. Optimising nudges and boosts for financial decisions under uncertainty. . Palgrave Commun. 5::113
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  44. Fundel VJ, Fleischhut N, Herzog SM, Göber M, Hagedorn R. 2019.. Promoting the use of probabilistic weather forecasts through a dialogue between scientists, developers and end-users. . Q. J. R. Meteorol. Soc. 145:(S1):21031
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  45. Furedi A. 1999.. Social consequences: the public health implications of the 1995 “pill scare. .” Hum. Reprod. Update 5:(6):62126
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  46. Garcia-Retamero R, Cokely ET. 2017.. Designing visual aids that promote risk literacy: a systematic review of health research and evidence-based design heuristics. . Hum. Factors 59:(4):582627
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  47. Geers M, Swire-Thompson B, Lorenz-Spreen P, Herzog SM, Kozyreva A, Hertwig R. 2024.. The Online Misinformation Engagement Framework. . Curr. Opin. Psychol. 55::101739
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  48. Gigerenzer G, Gaissmaier W. 2011.. Heuristic decision making. . Annu. Rev. Psychol. 62::45182
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  49. Gigerenzer G, Gaissmaier W, Kurz-Milcke E, Schwartz LM, Woloshin S. 2007.. Helping doctors and patients make sense of health statistics. . Psychol. Sci. Public Interest 8:(2):5396
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  50. Gigerenzer G, Hertwig R, Pachur T. 2011.. Heuristics: The Foundations of Adaptive Behavior. Oxford, UK:: Oxford Univ. Press
    [Google Scholar]
  51. Gigerenzer G, Reb J, Luan S. 2022.. Smart heuristics for individuals, teams, and organizations. . Annu. Rev. Organ. Psychol. Organ. Behav. 9::17198
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  52. Gilmore AB, Fabbri A, Baum F, Bertscher A, Bondy K, et al. 2023.. Defining and conceptualising the commercial determinants of health. . Lancet 401:(10383):1194213
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  53. Gollwitzer PM, Sheeran P. 2006.. Implementation intentions and goal achievement: a meta-analysis of effects and processes. . Adv. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 38::69119
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  54. Greenberg AE, Hershfield HE. 2019.. Financial decision making. . Consum. Psychol. Rev. 2:(1):1729
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  55. Grüne-Yanoff T. 2016.. Why behavioural policy needs mechanistic evidence. . Econ. Philos. 32:(3):46383
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  56. Grüne-Yanoff T, Hertwig R. 2016.. Nudge versus boost: How coherent are policy and theory?. Minds Mach. 26:(1):14983
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  57. Grüning DJ, Riedel F, Lorenz-Spreen P. 2023.. Directing smartphone use through the self-nudge app one sec. . PNAS 120:(8):e2213114120
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  58. Hallsworth M. 2023.. A manifesto for applying behavioural science. . Nat. Hum. Behav. 7:(3):31022
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  59. Hameiri B, Moore-Berg SL. 2022.. Intervention tournaments: an overview of concept, design, and implementation. . Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 17:(6):152540
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  60. Heath C, Larrick RP, Klayman J. 1998.. Cognitive repairs: how organizational practices can compensate for individual shortcomings. . Res. Organ. Behav. 20::137
    [Google Scholar]
  61. Hershfield HE. 2019.. The self over time. . Curr. Opin. Psychol. 26::7275
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  62. Hertwig R. 2017.. When to consider boosting: some rules for policy-makers. . Behav. Public Policy 1:(2):14361
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  63. Hertwig R. 2023.. The citizen choice architect in an ultra-processed world. . Behav. Public Policy 7:(4):90316
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  64. Hertwig R, Grüne-Yanoff T. 2017.. Nudging and boosting: steering or empowering good decisions. . Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 12:(6):97386
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  65. Hertwig R, Pleskac TJ, Pachur T, Center for Adaptive Rationality. 2019.. Taming Uncertainty. Cambridge, MA:: MIT Press
    [Google Scholar]
  66. Hertwig R, Todd PM. 2003.. More is not always better: the benefits of cognitive limits. . In Thinking: Psychological Perspectives on Reasoning, Judgment and Decision Making, ed. D Hardman, L Macchi , pp. 21331. Chichester, UK:: John Wiley & Sons
    [Google Scholar]
  67. Hertwig R, Wulff DU. 2022.. A description–experience framework of the psychology of risk. . Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 17:(3):63151
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  68. Herzog SM, Franklin M. 2024.. Boosting human competences with interpretable and explainable artificial intelligence. . Decision. In press
    [Google Scholar]
  69. Hofmann W. 2024.. Going beyond the individual level in self-control research. . Nat. Rev. Psychol. 3:(1):5666
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  70. Holford D, Fasce A, Tapper K, Demko M, Lewandowsky S, et al. 2023.. Science communication as a collective intelligence endeavor: a manifesto and examples for implementation. . Sci. Commun. 45:(4):53954
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  71. Inzlicht M, Werner KM, Briskin JL, Roberts BW. 2021.. Integrating models of self-regulation. . Annu. Rev. Psychol. 72::31945
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  72. Jachimowicz JM, Duncan S, Weber EU, Johnson EJ. 2019.. When and why defaults influence decisions: a meta-analysis of default effects. . Behav. Public Policy 3:(2):15986
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  73. Kahneman D. 2003.. A perspective on judgment and choice: mapping bounded rationality. . Am. Psychol. 58:(9):697720
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  74. Kahneman D. 2011.. Thinking, Fast and Slow. New York:: Farrar, Straus and Giroux
    [Google Scholar]
  75. Kaiser T, Lusardi A, Menkhoff L, Urban C. 2022.. Financial education affects financial knowledge and downstream behaviors. . J. Financ. Econ. 145:(2):25572
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  76. Katsikopoulos KV, Şimşek Ö, Buckmann M, Gigerenzer G. 2020.. Classification in the Wild: The Science and Art of Transparent Decision Making. Cambridge, MA:: MIT Press
    [Google Scholar]
  77. Kaufmann C, Weber M, Haisley E. 2013.. The role of experience sampling and graphical displays on one's investment risk appetite. . Manag. Sci. 59:(2):32340
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  78. Keller N, Jenny MA, Spies CA, Herzog SM. 2020.. Augmenting decision competence in healthcare using AI-based cognitive models. . In 2020 IEEE International Conference on Healthcare Informatics (ICHI). New York:: IEEE. https://doi.org/10.1109/ICHI48887.2020.9374376
    [Google Scholar]
  79. Kirgios EL, Mandel GH, Park Y, Milkman KL, Gromet DM, et al. 2020.. Teaching temptation bundling to boost exercise: a field experiment. . Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 161::2035
    [Google Scholar]
  80. Kozyreva A, Lewandowsky S, Hertwig R. 2020.. Citizens versus the Internet: confronting digital challenges with cognitive tools. . Psychol. Sci. Public Interest 21:(3):10356
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  81. Kozyreva A, Lorenz-Spreen P, Herzog SM, Ecker UKH, Lewandowsky S, et al. 2024.. Toolbox of individual-level interventions against online misinformation. . Nat. Hum. Behav. 8::104452
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  82. Kozyreva A, Wineburg S, Lewandowsky S, Hertwig R. 2023.. Critical ignoring as a core competence for digital citizens. . Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 32:(1):8188
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  83. Lammers J, Crusius J, Gast A. 2020.. Correcting misperceptions of exponential coronavirus growth increases support for social distancing. . PNAS 117:(28):1626466
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  84. Large DR, Choo MC, Houghton R. 2018.. Validating “Dutch reach”: a preliminary evaluation of far-hand door opening and its impact on car drivers' head movements. Paper presented at the 7th International Cycling Safety Conference, ICSC2018 , Barcelona, Spain:, Oct. 10–11
    [Google Scholar]
  85. Lejarraga T, Hertwig R. 2021.. How experimental methods shaped views on human competence and rationality. . Psychol. Bull. 147:(6):53564
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  86. Lewandowsky S, Ecker UKH, Cook J, van der Linden S, Roozenbeek J, Oreskes N. 2023.. Misinformation and the epistemic integrity of democracy. . Curr. Opin. Psychol. 54::101711
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  87. Lewandowsky S, Smilie L, Garcia D, Hertwig R, Weatherall J, et al. 2020.. Technology and democracy: understanding the influence of online technologies on political behaviour and decision making. JRC Sci. Policy Rep., Publ. Off. Eur. Union, Luxembourg:
    [Google Scholar]
  88. Lewandowsky S, van der Linden S. 2021.. Countering misinformation and fake news through inoculation and prebunking. . Eur. Rev. Soc. Psychol. 32:(2):34884
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  89. Lieder F, Griffiths TL. 2020.. Resource-rational analysis: understanding human cognition as the optimal use of limited computational resources. . Behav. Brain Sci. 43::e1
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  90. Lorenz-Spreen P, Geers M, Pachur T, Hertwig R, Lewandowsky S, Herzog SM. 2021.. Boosting people's ability to detect microtargeted advertising. . Sci. Rep. 11:(1):15541
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  91. Loy LS, Wieber F, Gollwitzer PM, Oettingen G. 2016.. Supporting sustainable food consumption: Mental contrasting with implementation intentions (MCII) aligns intentions and behavior. . Front. Psychol. 7::607
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  92. Lusardi A, Samek A, Kapteyn A, Glinert L, Hung A, Heinberg A. 2017.. Visual tools and narratives: new ways to improve financial literacy. . J. Pension Econ. Finance 16:(3):297323
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  93. Lyon AR, Brewer SK, Areán PA. 2020.. Leveraging human-centered design to implement modern psychological science: return on an early investment. . Am. Psychol. 75:(8):106779
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  94. Mach KJ, Lemos MC, Meadow AM, Wyborn C, Klenk N, et al. 2020.. Actionable knowledge and the art of engagement. . Curr. Opin. Environ. Sustain. 42::3037
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  95. Maier M, Bartoš F, Stanley TD, Shanks DR, Harris AJL, Wagenmakers EJ. 2022.. No evidence for nudging after adjusting for publication bias. . PNAS 119:(31):e2200300119
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  96. Marques MM, Wright AJ, Corker E, Johnston M, West R, et al. 2023.. The Behaviour Change Technique Ontology: transforming the Behaviour Change Technique Taxonomy v1. . Wellcome Open Res. 8::308
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  97. Mata J, Frank R, Gigerenzer G. 2014.. Symptom recognition of heart attack and stroke in nine European countries: a representative survey. . Health Expect. 17:(3):37687
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  98. McDowell M, Gigerenzer G, Wegwarth O, Rebitschek FG. 2019.. Effect of tabular and icon fact box formats on comprehension of benefits and harms of prostate cancer screening: a randomized trial. . Med. Decis. Mak. 39:(1):4156
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  99. McDowell M, Jacobs P. 2017.. Meta-analysis of the effect of natural frequencies on Bayesian reasoning. . Psychol. Bull. 143:(12):1273312
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  100. McDowell M, Rebitschek FG, Gigerenzer G, Wegwarth O. 2016.. A simple tool for communicating the benefits and harms of health interventions: a guide for creating a fact box. . MDM Policy Pract. 1:(1):238146831666536
    [Google Scholar]
  101. McGrew S. 2024.. Teaching lateral reading: interventions to help people read like fact checkers. . Curr. Opin. Psychol. 55::101737
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  102. McKenzie CRM, Liersch MJ. 2011.. Misunderstanding savings growth: implications for retirement savings behavior. . J. Mark. Res. 48:(SPL):S1S13
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  103. Michie S, van Stralen MM, West R. 2011.. The behaviour change wheel: a new method for characterising and designing behaviour change interventions. . Implement. Sci. 6:(1):42
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  104. Milkman KL, Chugh D, Bazerman MH. 2009.. How can decision making be improved?. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 4:(4):37983
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  105. Morewedge CK, Yoon H, Scopelliti I, Symborski CW, Korris JH, Kassam KS. 2015.. Debiasing decisions: improved decision making with a single training intervention. . Policy Insights Behav. Brain Sci. 2:(1):12940
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  106. Narayanan A, Mathur A, Chetty M, Kshirsagar M. 2020.. Dark patterns: past, present, and future: the evolution of tricky user interfaces. . Queue 18:(2):6792
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  107. Nosek BA, Hardwicke TE, Moshontz H, Allard A, Corker KS, et al. 2022.. Replicability, robustness, and reproducibility in psychological science. . Annu. Rev. Psychol. 73::71948
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  108. Nussbaum MC. 2011.. Creating Capabilities: The Human Development Approach. Cambridge, MA:: Harvard Univ. Press
    [Google Scholar]
  109. Oettingen G. 2012.. Future thought and behaviour change. . Eur. Rev. Soc. Psychol. 23:(1):163
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  110. Oettingen G, Gollwitzer PM. 2010.. Strategies of setting and implementing goals: mental contrasting and implementation intentions. . In Social Psychological Foundations of Clinical Psychology, ed. JP Maddux, JE Tangney , pp. 11435. New York:: Guilford
    [Google Scholar]
  111. Oettingen G, Gollwitzer PM. 2015.. Self-regulation: principles and tools. . In Self-Regulation in Adolescence, ed. G Oettingen, PM Gollwitzer , pp. 329. Cambridge, UK:: Cambridge Univ. Press. , 1st ed..
    [Google Scholar]
  112. Okan O, Bollweg TM, Berens EM, Hurrelmann K, Bauer U, Schaeffer D. 2020.. Coronavirus-related health literacy: a cross-sectional study in adults during the COVID-19 infodemic in Germany. . Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 17:(15):5503
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  113. Olen H. 2013.. Pound Foolish: Exposing the Dark Side of the Personal Finance Industry. New York:: Penguin
    [Google Scholar]
  114. Oliver A. 2019.. Towards a new political economy of behavioral public policy. . Public Adm. Rev. 79:(6):91724
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  115. Osborne J, Pimentel D. 2022.. Science, misinformation, and the role of education. . Science 378:(6617):24648
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  116. Pagliai G, Dinu M, Madarena MP, Bonaccio M, Iacoviello L, Sofi F. 2021.. Consumption of ultra-processed foods and health status: a systematic review and meta-analysis. . Br. J. Nutr. 125:(3):30818
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  117. Paunov Y, Grüne-Yanoff T. 2023.. Boosting versus nudging sustainable energy consumption: a long-term comparative field test in a residential context. . Behav. Public Policy. In press . https://doi.org/10.1017/bpp.2023.30
    [Google Scholar]
  118. Pennebaker JW. 2018.. Expressive writing in psychological science. . Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 13:(2):22629
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  119. Peterson CR, Beach LR. 1967.. Man as an intuitive statistician. . Psychol. Bull. 68:(1):2946
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  120. Poti JM, Popkin BM. 2011.. Trends in energy intake among US children by eating location and food source, 1977–2006. . J. Am. Diet. Assoc. 111:(8):115664
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  121. Reijula S, Hertwig R. 2022.. Self-nudging and the citizen choice architect. . Behav. Public Policy 6:(1):11949
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  122. Roozenbeek J, van der Linden S, Goldberg B, Rathje S, Lewandowsky S. 2022.. Psychological inoculation improves resilience against misinformation on social media. . Sci. Adv. 8:(34):eabo6254
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  123. Rouyard T, Engelen B, Papanikitas A, Nakamura R. 2022.. Boosting healthier choices. . BMJ 376::e064225
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  124. Rudin C, Chen C, Chen Z, Huang H, Semenova L, Zhong C. 2022.. Interpretable machine learning: fundamental principles and 10 grand challenges. . Stat. Surv. 16::185
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  125. Ryan RM, Duineveld JJ, Di Domenico SI, Ryan WS, Steward BA, Bradshaw EL. 2022.. We know this much is (meta-analytically) true: a meta-review of meta-analytic findings evaluating self-determination theory. . Psychol. Bull. 148:(11–12):81342
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  126. Ryle G. 1945–1946.. Knowing how and knowing that: the presidential address. . Proc. Aristot. Soc. 46::116
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  127. Savage LJ. 1954.. The Foundations of Statistics. New York:: John Wiley & Sons. , 1st ed..
    [Google Scholar]
  128. Schelling TC. 1978.. Egonomics, or the art of self-management. . Am. Econ. Rev. 68:(2):29094
    [Google Scholar]
  129. Schendler A. 2021.. Worrying about your carbon footprint is exactly what Big Oil wants you to do. . New York Times, Aug. 31 . https://www.nytimes.com/2021/08/31/opinion/climate-change-carbon-neutral.html
    [Google Scholar]
  130. Schmidt AT, Engelen B. 2020.. The ethics of nudging: an overview. . Philos. Compass 15:(4):e12658
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  131. Schunk D, Berger EM, Hermes H, Winkel K, Fehr E. 2022.. Teaching self-regulation. . Nat. Hum. Behav. 6:(12):168090
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  132. Schurz G, Hertwig R. 2019.. Cognitive success: a consequentialist account of rationality in cognition. . Topics Cogn. Sci. 11:(1):736
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  133. Schüz B, Meyerhof H, Hilz LK, Mata J. 2021.. Equity effects of dietary nudging field experiments: systematic review. . Front. Public Health 9::668998
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  134. Sedlmeier P, Gigerenzer G. 2001.. Teaching Bayesian reasoning in less than two hours. . J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 130:(3):380400
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  135. Semenova L, Rudin C, Parr R. 2022.. On the existence of simpler machine learning models. . In 2022 ACM Conference on Fairness, Accountability, and Transparency, pp. 182758. New York:: ACM
    [Google Scholar]
  136. Sen A. 2002.. Rationality and Freedom. Cambridge, MA:: Harvard Univ. Press
    [Google Scholar]
  137. Sen A. 2006.. Reason, freedom and well-being. . Utilitas 18:(1):8096
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  138. Shafir E. 2017.. Decisions in poverty contexts. . Curr. Opin. Psychol. 18::13136
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  139. Sharp C, Kaplan RM, Strauman TJ. 2023.. The use of ontologies to accelerate the behavioral sciences: promises and challenges. . Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 32:(5):41826
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  140. Simchon A, Edwards M, Lewandowsky S. 2024.. The persuasive effects of political microtargeting in the age of generative artificial intelligence. . PNAS Nexus 3:(2):pgae035
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  141. Simon HA. 1955.. A behavioral model of rational choice. . Q. J. Econ. 69:(1):99118
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  142. Simon HA. 1990.. Invariants of human behavior. . Annu. Rev. Psychol. 41::119
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  143. Soll JB, Keeney RL, Larrick RP. 2013.. Consumer misunderstanding of credit card use, payments, and debt: causes and solutions. . J. Public Policy Mark. 32:(1):6681
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  144. Soll JB, Milkman KL, Payne JW. 2015.. A user's guide to debiasing. . In The Wiley Blackwell Handbook of Judgment and Decision Making, ed. G Keren, G Wu , pp. 92451. New York:: John Wiley & Sons
    [Google Scholar]
  145. Sørensen K, Van den Broucke S, Fullam J, Doyle G, Pelikan J, et al. 2012.. Health literacy and public health: a systematic review and integration of definitions and models. . BMC Public Health 12:(1):80
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  146. Spadaro G, Tiddi I, Columbus S, Jin S, ten Teije A, et al. 2022.. The Cooperation Databank: Machine-readable science accelerates research synthesis. . Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 17:(5):147289
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  147. Spiegelhalter D, Pearson M, Short I. 2011.. Visualizing uncertainty about the future. . Science 333:(6048):1393400
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  148. Spiliopoulos L, Hertwig R. 2020.. A map of ecologically rational heuristics for uncertain strategic worlds. . Psychol. Rev. 127:(2):24580
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  149. Sunstein CR. 2014.. Nudging: a very short guide. . J. Consum. Policy 37:(4):58388
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  150. Sunstein CR. 2016.. The council of psychological advisers. . Annu. Rev. Psychol. 67::71337
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  151. Sunstein CR. 2022.. The distributional effects of nudges. . Nat. Hum. Behav. 6:(1):910
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  152. Sunstein CR, Thaler RH. 2003.. Libertarian paternalism is not an oxymoron. . Univ. Chicago Law Rev. 70:(4):1159202
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  153. Swets JA, Dawes RM, Monahan J. 2000.. Psychological science can improve diagnostic decisions. . Psychol. Sci. Public Interest 1:(1):126
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  154. Szaszi B, Higney A, Charlton A, Gelman A, Ziano I, et al. 2022.. No reason to expect large and consistent effects of nudge interventions. . PNAS 119:(31):e2200732119
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  155. Tang YY, Tang R, Posner MI, Gross JJ. 2022.. Effortless training of attention and self-control: mechanisms and applications. . Trends Cogn. Sci. 26:(7):56777
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  156. Thaler RH. 1994.. Quasi Rational Economics. New York:: Russell Sage Found. , 1st ed..
    [Google Scholar]
  157. Thaler RH, Sunstein CR. 2003.. Libertarian paternalism. . Am. Econ. Rev. 93:(2):17579
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  158. Thaler RH, Sunstein CR. 2008.. Nudge: Improving Decisions About Health, Wealth and Happiness. New York:: Penguin
    [Google Scholar]
  159. Topp L, Mair D, Smillie L, Cairney P. 2018.. Knowledge management for policy impact: the case of the European Commission's Joint Research Centre. . Palgrave Commun. 4:(1):87
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  160. van der Bles AM, van der Linden S, Freeman ALJ, Mitchell J, Galvao AB, et al. 2019.. Communicating uncertainty about facts, numbers and science. . R. Soc. Open Sci. 6:(5):181870
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  161. van Roekel H, Reinhard J, Grimmelikhuijsen S. 2022.. Improving hand hygiene in hospitals: comparing the effect of a nudge and a boost on protocol compliance. . Behav. Public Policy 6:(1):5274
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  162. Wang G, Wang Y, Gai X. 2021.. A meta-analysis of the effects of mental contrasting with implementation intentions on goal attainment. . Front. Psychol. 12::565202
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  163. Wang ZJ, Zhong C, Xin R, Takagi T, Chen Z, et al. 2022.. TimberTrek: exploring and curating sparse decision trees with interactive visualization. . In 2022 IEEE Visualization and Visual Analytics (VIS), pp. 6064. New York:: IEEE
    [Google Scholar]
  164. Wegwarth O, Ludwig WD, Spies C, Schulte E, Hertwig R. 2022.. The role of simulated-experience and descriptive formats on perceiving risks of strong opioids: a randomized controlled trial with chronic noncancer pain patients. . Patient Educ. Couns. 105:(6):157180
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  165. Weinert FE. 2001.. Concept of competence: a conceptual clarification. . In Defining and Selecting Key Competencies, ed. DS Rychen, LH Salganik , pp. 4565. Ashland, OH:: Hogrefe & Huber Publ.
    [Google Scholar]
  166. Welzel C, Inglehart R. 2010.. Agency, values, and well-being: a human development model. . Soc. Indic. Res. 97:(1):4363
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  167. Wheeler G. 2020.. Bounded rationality. . In The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy, ed. EN Zalta . Stanford, CA:: Stanford Univ. https://plato.stanford.edu/archives/fall2020/entries/bounded-rationality/
    [Google Scholar]
  168. Wickens CD, Helton WS, Hollands JG, Banbury S. 2022.. Engineering Psychology and Human Performance. New York:: Routledge. , 5th ed..
    [Google Scholar]
  169. Wood W, Rünger D. 2016.. Psychology of habit. . Annu. Rev. Psychol. 67::289314
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  170. Xiong C, Sarvghad A, Goldstein DG, Hofman JM, Demiralp Ç. 2022.. Investigating perceptual biases in icon arrays. . In Proceedings of the 2022 CHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems, . New York:: ACM. https://doi.org/10.1145/3491102.3501874
    [Google Scholar]
  171. Zhang H, Morris Q, Ustun B, Ghassemi M. 2021.. Learning optimal predictive checklists. . Adv. Neural Inf. Process. Syst. 34::121529
    [Google Scholar]
  172. Zimmerman MA. 2000.. Empowerment theory. . In Handbook of Community Psychology, ed. J Rappaport, S Seidman , pp. 4363. Boston:: Springer
    [Google Scholar]
  173. Zimmerman S, Herzog SM, Elsweiler D, Chamberlain J, Kruschwitz U. 2020.. Towards a framework for harm prevention in web search. . In Proceedings of the First Workshop on Bridging the Gap Between Information Science, Information Retrieval and Data Science (BIRDS) 2020, ed. I Frommholz, H Liu, M Melucci , pp. 3046. CEUR Workshop Proc. https://ceur-ws.org/Vol-2741/paper-07.pdf
    [Google Scholar]
/content/journals/10.1146/annurev-psych-020924-124753
Loading
/content/journals/10.1146/annurev-psych-020924-124753
Loading

Data & Media loading...

  • Article Type: Review Article
This is a required field
Please enter a valid email address
Approval was a Success
Invalid data
An Error Occurred
Approval was partially successful, following selected items could not be processed due to error