1932

Abstract

Moral learning and decision-making are crucial throughout our lives, from infancy to old age. Emerging evidence suggests that there are important differences in learning and decision-making in moral situations across the lifespan, and these are underpinned by co-occurring changes in the use of model-based values and theory of mind. Here, we review the decision neuroscience literature on moral choices and moral learning considering four key concepts. We show how in the earliest years, a sense of self/other distinction is foundational. Sensitivity to intention versus outcome is crucial for several moral concepts and is most similar in our earliest and oldest years. Across all ages, basic shifts in the influence of theory of mind and model-free and model-based learning support moral decision-making. Moving forward, a computational approach to key concepts of morality can help provide a mechanistic account and generate new hypotheses to test across the whole lifespan.

Loading

Article metrics loading...

/content/journals/10.1146/annurev-psych-021324-060611
2025-01-17
2025-02-13
Loading full text...

Full text loading...

/deliver/fulltext/psych/76/1/annurev-psych-021324-060611.html?itemId=/content/journals/10.1146/annurev-psych-021324-060611&mimeType=html&fmt=ahah

Literature Cited

  1. Achterberg M, van Duijvenvoorde ACK, van der Meulen M, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, Crone EA. 2018.. Heritability of aggression following social evaluation in middle childhood: an fMRI study. . Hum. Brain Mapp. 39:(7):282841
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  2. Almås I, Cappelen AW, Sørensen , Tungodden B. 2010.. Fairness and the development of inequality acceptance. . Science 328:(5982):117678
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  3. Almeling L, Hammerschmidt K, Sennhenn-Reulen H, Freund AM, Fischer J. 2016.. Motivational shifts in aging monkeys and the origins of social selectivity. . Curr. Biol. 26:(13):174449
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  4. Amsterdam B. 1972.. Mirror self-image reactions before age two. . Dev. Psychobiol. 5:(4):297305
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  5. Armon C, Dawson TL. 1997.. Developmental trajectories in moral reasoning across the life span. . J. Moral Educ. 26:(4):43353
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  6. Bailey PE, Ruffman T, Rendell PG. 2013.. Age-related differences in social economic decision making: the Ultimatum Game. . J. Gerontol. Ser. B 68:(3):35663
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  7. Baltes PB. 2009.. Longitudinal and cross-sectional sequences in the study of age and generation effects. . Hum. Dev. 11:(3):14571
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  8. Bandura A. 1965.. Influence of models’ reinforcement contingencies on the acquisition of imitative responses. . J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1:(6):58995
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  9. Bastin C, Harrison BJ, Davey CG, Moll J, Whittle S. 2016.. Feelings of shame, embarrassment and guilt and their neural correlates: a systematic review. . Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 71::45571
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  10. Baumard N, Mascaro O, Chevallier C. 2012.. Preschoolers are able to take merit into account when distributing goods. . Dev. Psychol. 48:(2):49298
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  11. Beadle JN, de la Vega CE. 2019.. Impact of aging on empathy: review of psychological and neural mechanisms. . Front. Psychiatry 10::331
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  12. Beier JS, Over H, Carpenter M. 2014.. Young children help others to achieve their social goals. . Dev. Psychol. 50:(3):93440
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  13. Bellucci G, Camilleri JA, Iyengar V, Eickhoff SB, Krueger F. 2020.. The emerging neuroscience of social punishment: meta-analytic evidence. . Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 113::42639
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  14. Bellucci G, Dreher J-C. 2022.. Trust and learning: neurocomputational signatures of learning to trust. . In The Neurobiology of Trust, ed. F Krueger , pp. 185220. Cambridge, UK:: Cambridge Univ. Press
    [Google Scholar]
  15. Bellucci G, Feng C, Camilleri J, Eickhoff SB, Krueger F. 2018.. The role of the anterior insula in social norm compliance and enforcement: evidence from coordinate-based and functional connectivity meta-analyses. . Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 92::37889
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  16. Bellucci G, Molter F, Park SQ. 2019.. Neural representations of honesty predict future trust behavior. . Nat. Commun. 10:(1):112
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  17. Benistant J, Guigon V, Nicolas A, Derrington E, Dreher J-C. 2024.. Dynamic valuation bias explains social influence on cheating behavior. . bioRxiv 594859:. https://doi.org/10.1101/2024.05.21.594859
    [Google Scholar]
  18. Bingham CR, Simons-Morton BG, Pradhan AK, Li K, Almani F, et al. 2016.. Peer passenger norms and pressure: experimental effects on simulated driving among teenage males. . Transp. Res. F 41::12437
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  19. Bird G, Viding E. 2014.. The self to other model of empathy: providing a new framework for understanding empathy impairments in psychopathy, autism, and alexithymia. . Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 47::52032
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  20. Blake P, McAuliffe K, Corbit J, Callaghan T, Barry O, et al. 2015.. The ontogeny of fairness in seven societies. . Nature 528:(7581):25861
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  21. Blake PR, McAuliffe K. 2011.. “ I had so much it didn't seem fair”: Eight-year-olds reject two forms of inequity. . Cognition 120:(2):21524
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  22. Blakemore S-J, Mills KL. 2014.. Is adolescence a sensitive period for sociocultural processing?. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 65::187207
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  23. Bloom P, Wynn K. 2016.. What develops in moral development?. In Core Knowledge and Conceptual Change, ed. D Barner, AS Baron , pp. 34764. Oxford, UK:: Oxford Univ. Press
    [Google Scholar]
  24. Bolenz F, Kool W, Reiter AM, Eppinger B. 2019.. Metacontrol of decision-making strategies in human aging. . eLife 8::e49154
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  25. Bolenz F, Reiter AMF, Eppinger B. 2017.. Developmental changes in learning: computational mechanisms and social influences. . Front Psychol. 8::2048
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  26. Bonnefon J-F, Rahwan I, Shariff A. 2024.. The moral psychology of artificial intelligence. . Annu. Rev. Psychol. 75::65375
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  27. Braams BR, Peters S, Peper JS, Güroğlu B, Crone EA, et al. 2014.. Gambling for self, friends, and antagonists: differential contributions of affective and social brain regions on adolescent reward processing. . NeuroImage 100::28189
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  28. Buckholtz JW. 2015.. Social norms, self-control, and the value of antisocial behavior. . Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 3::12229
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  29. Cameron S, Wilks M, Redshaw J, Nielsen M. 2022.. The effect of moral character on children's judgements of transgressions. . Cogn. Dev. 63::101221
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  30. Carlson RW, Crockett MJ. 2018.. The lateral prefrontal cortex and moral goal pursuit. . Curr. Opin. Psychol. 24::7782
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  31. Carpendale JIM, Kettner VA, Audet KN. 2015.. On the nature of toddlers’ helping: helping or interest in others’ activity?. Soc. Dev. 24:(2):35766
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  32. Castrellon JJ, Zald DH, Samanez-Larkin GR, Seaman KL. 2024.. Adult age-related differences in susceptibility to social conformity pressures in self-control over daily desires. . Psychol. Aging 39:(1):10212
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  33. Chang LJ, Koban L. 2013.. Modeling emotion and learning of norms in social interactions. . J. Neurosci. 33:(18):761517
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  34. Chang LJ, Smith A, Dufwenberg M, Sanfey AG. 2011.. Triangulating the neural, psychological, and economic bases of guilt aversion. . Neuron 70:(3):56072
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  35. Charles S, Carstensen LL. 2010.. Social and emotional aging. . Annu. Rev. Psychol. 61::383409
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  36. Chein JM, Albert D, O'Brien L, Uckert K, Steinberg L. 2011.. Peers increase adolescent risk taking by enhancing activity in the brain's reward circuitry. . Dev. Sci. 14:(2):F110
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  37. Chen Y-C, Chen C-C, Decety J, Cheng Y. 2014.. Aging is associated with changes in the neural circuits underlying empathy. . Neurobiol. Aging 35:(4):82736
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  38. Christopoulos GI, Liu X-X, Hong Y. 2017.. Toward an understanding of dynamic moral decision making: model-free and model-based learning. . J. Bus. Ethics 144:(4):699715
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  39. Ciranka S, van den Bos W. 2019.. Social influence in adolescent decision-making: a formal framework. . Front. Psychol. 10::1915
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  40. Cole PM, Barrett KC, Zahn-Waxler C. 1992.. Emotion displays in two-year-olds during mishaps. . Child Dev. 63:(2):31424
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  41. Contreras-Huerta LS, Lockwood PL, Bird G, Apps MAJ, Crockett MJ. 2022.. Prosocial behavior is associated with transdiagnostic markers of affective sensitivity in multiple domains. . Emotion 22:(5):82035
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  42. Crockett MJ. 2013.. Models of morality. . Trends Cogn. Sci. 17:(8):36366
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  43. Crockett MJ, Kurth-Nelson Z, Siegel JZ, Dayan P, Dolan RJ. 2015.. Harm to others outweighs harm to self in moral decision making. . PNAS 111:(4):E381
    [Google Scholar]
  44. Crockett MJ, Siegel JZ, Kurth-Nelson Z, Dayan P, Dolan RJ. 2017.. Moral transgressions corrupt neural representations of value. . Nat. Neurosci. 20::87985
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  45. Cushman F, Kumar V, Railton P. 2017.. Moral learning: psychological and philosophical perspectives. . Cognition 167::110
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  46. Cushman F, Sheketoff R, Wharton S, Carey S. 2013.. The development of intent-based moral judgment. . Cognition 127:(1):621
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  47. Cutler J, Nitschke JP, Lamm C, Lockwood PL. 2021a.. Older adults across the globe exhibit increased prosocial behavior but also greater in-group preferences. . Nat. Aging 1:(10):88088
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  48. Cutler J, Wittmann MK, Abdurahman A, Hargitai LD, Drew D, et al. 2021b.. Ageing is associated with disrupted reinforcement learning whilst learning to help others is preserved. . Nat. Commun. 12:(1): 4440.
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  49. da Silva Pinho A, Molleman L, Braams BR, van den Bos W. 2021.. Majority and popularity effects on norm formation in adolescence. . Sci. Rep. 11::12884
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  50. Dahl A, Paulus M. 2019.. From interest to obligation: the gradual development of human altruism. . Child Dev. Perspect. 13:(1):1014
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  51. Dayan P, Berridge KC. 2014.. Model-based and model-free Pavlovian reward learning: revaluation, revision, and revelation. . Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 14:(2):47392
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  52. Decety J, Cowell JM. 2018.. Interpersonal harm aversion as a necessary foundation for morality: a developmental neuroscience perspective. . Dev. Psychopathol. 30:(1):15364
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  53. Decety J, Holvoet C. 2021.. The emergence of empathy: a developmental neuroscience perspective. . Dev. Rev. 62::100999
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  54. Decety J, Michalska KJ. 2010.. Neurodevelopmental changes in the circuits underlying empathy and sympathy from childhood to adulthood. . Dev. Sci. 13:(6):88699
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  55. Decety J, Michalska KJ, Kinzler KD. 2012.. The contribution of emotion and cognition to moral sensitivity: a neurodevelopmental study. . Cereb. Cortex 22:(1):20920
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  56. Decker JH, Otto AR, Daw ND, Hartley CA. 2016.. From creatures of habit to goal-directed learners. . Psychol. Sci. 27:(6):84858
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  57. DeScioli P, Kurzban R. 2009.. Mysteries of morality. . Cognition 112:(2):28199
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  58. Dimant E. 2019.. Contagion of pro- and anti-social behavior among peers and the role of social proximity. . J. Econ. Psychol. 73::6688
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  59. Dogan A, Morishima Y, Heise F, Tanner C, Gibson R, et al. 2016.. Prefrontal connections express individual differences in intrinsic resistance to trading off honesty values against economic benefits. . Sci. Rep. 6:(1):33263
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  60. Earp BD, McLoughlin KL, Monrad JT, Clark MS, Crockett MJ. 2021.. How social relationships shape moral wrongness judgments. . Nat. Commun. 12:(1):5776
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  61. Eisenberg N, Cumberland A, Guthrie IK, Murphy BC, Shepard SA. 2005.. Age changes in prosocial responding and moral reasoning in adolescence and early adulthood. . J. Res. Adolesc. 15:(3):23560
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  62. Engel C. 2011.. Dictator games: a meta study. . Exp. Econ. 14:(4):583610
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  63. Eppinger B, Bruckner R. 2015.. Towards a mechanistic understanding of age-related changes in learning and decision making: a neuro-computational approach. . In Aging and Decision Making, ed. TM Hess, J Strough, CE Löckenhoff , pp. 6177. San Diego, CA:: Academic
    [Google Scholar]
  64. Fagard J, Rat-Fischer L, Esseily R, Somogyi E, O'Regan JK. 2016.. What does it take for an infant to learn how to use a tool by observation?. Front. Psychol. 7::267
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  65. Fairchild G, Hawes DJ, Frick PJ, Copeland WE, Odgers CL, et al. 2019.. Conduct disorder. . Nat. Rev. Disease Primers 5:(1):125
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  66. Fehr E, Bernhard H, Rockenbach B. 2008.. Egalitarianism in young children. . Nature 454:(7208):107983
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  67. Fehr E, Fischbacher U. 2004a.. Social norms and human cooperation. . Trends Cogn. Sci. 8:(4):18590
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  68. Fehr E, Fischbacher U. 2004b.. Third party punishment and social norms. . Evol. Hum. Behav. 25:(2):6397
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  69. Fehr E, Schmidt KM. 1999.. A theory of fairness, competition, and cooperation. . Q. J. Econ. 114:(3):81768
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  70. FeldmanHall O, Dunsmoor J. 2019.. Viewing adaptive social choice through the lens of associative learning. . Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 14:(2):17596
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  71. Feng C, Luo Y-J, Krueger F. 2015.. Neural signatures of fairness-related normative decision making in the ultimatum game: a coordinate-based meta-analysis. . Hum. Brain Mapp. 36:(2):591602
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  72. Feng C, Zhu Z, Cui Z, Ushakov V, Dreher J-C, et al. 2021.. Prediction of trust propensity from intrinsic brain morphology and functional connectome. . Hum. Brain Mapp. 42:(1):17591
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  73. Fett A-KJ, Shergill SS, Gromann PM, Dumontheil I, Blakemore S-J, et al. 2014.. Trust and social reciprocity in adolescence—a matter of perspective-taking. . J. Adolesc. 37:(2):17584
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  74. Fields EC, Kensinger EA, Garcia SM, Ford JH, Cunningham TJ. 2022.. With age comes well-being: older age associated with lower stress, negative affect, and depression throughout the COVID-19 pandemic. . Aging Ment. Health 26:(10):207179
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  75. Foncelle A, Barat E, Dreher J-C, van der Henst J-B. 2022.. Rank reversal aversion and fairness in hierarchies. . Adapt. Hum. Behav. Physiol. 8:(4):52037
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  76. Fornari L, Ioumpa K, Nostro AD, Evans NJ, De Angelis L, et al. 2023.. Neuro-computational mechanisms and individual biases in action-outcome learning under moral conflict. . Nat. Commun. 14:(1):1218
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  77. Foti F, Martone D, Orrù S, Montuori S, Imperlini E, et al. 2018.. Are young children able to learn exploratory strategies by observation?. Psychol. Res. 82:(6):121223
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  78. Foulkes L, Blakemore S-J. 2016.. Is there heightened sensitivity to social reward in adolescence?. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 40::8185
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  79. Fouragnan E, Chierchia G, Greiner S, Neveu R, Avesani P, Coricelli G. 2013.. Reputational priors magnify striatal responses to violations of trust. . J. Neurosci. 33:(8):360211
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  80. Frick PJ, Kemp EC. 2021.. Conduct disorders and empathy development. . Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 17::391416
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  81. Gabay AS, Apps MAJ. 2021.. Foraging optimally in social neuroscience: computations and methodological considerations. . Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 16:(8):78294
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  82. Gao X, Yu H, Sáez I, Blue PR, Zhu L, et al. 2018.. Distinguishing neural correlates of context-dependent advantageous- and disadvantageous-inequity aversion. . PNAS 115:(33):E768089
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  83. Garrett N, Lazzaro SC, Ariely D, Sharot T. 2016.. The brain adapts to dishonesty. . Nat. Neurosci. 19:(12):172732
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  84. Gigerenzer G. 2008.. Moral intuition = fast and frugal heuristics?. In Moral Psychology, Vol. 2: The Cognitive Science of Morality: Intuition Diversity, ed. W Sinnott-Armstrong , pp. 126. Cambridge, MA:: Boston Rev.
    [Google Scholar]
  85. Gong X, Brazil IA, Chang LJ, Sanfey AG. 2019.. Psychopathic traits are related to diminished guilt aversion and reduced trustworthiness during social decision-making. . Sci. Rep. 9::7307
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  86. Greene JD, Paxton JM. 2009.. Patterns of neural activity associated with honest and dishonest moral decisions. . PNAS 106:(30):1250611
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  87. Gummerum M, Chu MT. 2014.. Outcomes and intentions in children's, adolescents’, and adults’ second- and third-party punishment behavior. . Cognition 133:(1):97103
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  88. Gummerum M, López-Pérez B, van Dijk E, van Dillen LF. 2020.. When punishment is emotion-driven: children's, adolescents’, and adults’ costly punishment of unfair allocations. . Soc. Dev. 29:(1):12642
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  89. Güroğlu B, van den Bos W, Crone EA. 2014.. Sharing and giving across adolescence: an experimental study examining the development of prosocial behavior. . Front. Psychol. 5::291
    [Google Scholar]
  90. Güroǧlu B, van den Bos W, van Dijk E, Rombouts SARB, Crone EA. 2011.. Dissociable brain networks involved in development of fairness considerations: understanding intentionality behind unfairness. . NeuroImage 57:(2):63441
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  91. Guyer AE, Choate VR, Pine DS, Nelson EE. 2012.. Neural circuitry underlying affective response to peer feedback in adolescence. . Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 7:(1):8192
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  92. Hamlin JK. 2013.. Moral judgment and action in preverbal infants and toddlers: evidence for an innate moral core. . Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 22:(3):18693
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  93. Hamlin JK, Wynn K, Bloom P. 2007.. Social evaluation by preverbal infants. . Nature 450:(7169):55759
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  94. Hämmerer D, Schwartenbeck P, Gallagher M, FitzGerald THB, Düzel E, Dolan RJ. 2019.. Older adults fail to form stable task representations during model-based reversal inference. . Neurobiol. Aging 74::90100
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  95. Happé FG, Winner E, Brownell H. 1998.. The getting of wisdom: theory of mind in old age. . Dev. Psychol. 34:(2):35862
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  96. Henry JD, Phillips LH, Ruffman T, Bailey PE. 2013.. A meta-analytic review of age differences in theory of mind. . Psychol. Aging 28:(3):82639
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  97. Heyes C. 2018.. Enquire within: cultural evolution and cognitive science. . Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B 373:( 1743.):20170051
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  98. Hill CA, Suzuki S, Polania R, Moisa M, O'Doherty JP, Ruff CC. 2017.. A causal account of the brain network computations underlying strategic social behavior. . Nat. Neurosci. 20:(8):114249
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  99. Hofmans L, van den Bos W. 2022.. Social learning across adolescence: a Bayesian neurocognitive perspective. . Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 58::101151
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  100. Holvoet C, Scola C, Arciszewski T, Picard D. 2016.. Infants’ preference for prosocial behaviors: a literature review. . Infant Behav. Dev. 45::12539
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  101. Hopp FR, Amir O, Fisher JT, Grafton S, Sinnott-Armstrong W, Weber R. 2023.. Moral foundations elicit shared and dissociable cortical activation modulated by political ideology. . Nat. Hum. Behav. 7:(12):218298
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  102. House BR, Henrich J, Brosnan SF, Silk JB. 2012.. The ontogeny of human prosociality: behavioral experiments with children aged 3 to 8. . Evol. Hum. Behav. 33:(4):291308
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  103. Hu Y, Hu C, Derrington E, Corgnet B, Qu C, Dreher J-C. 2021a.. Neural basis of corruption in power-holders. . eLife 10::e63922
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  104. Hu Y, Pereira AM, Gao X, Campos BM, Derrington E, et al. 2021b.. Right temporoparietal junction underlies avoidance of moral transgression in autism spectrum disorder. . J. Neurosci. 41:(8):1699715
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  105. Hubbard J, Harbaugh WT, Srivastava S, Degras D, Mayr U. 2016.. A general benevolence dimension that links neural, psychological, economic, and life-span data on altruistic tendencies. . J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 145:(10):135158
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  106. Izuma K. 2012.. The social neuroscience of reputation. . Neurosci. Res. 72:(4):28388
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  107. Jara-Ettinger J, Gweon H, Schulz LE, Tenenbaum JB. 2016.. The naïve utility calculus: computational principles underlying commonsense psychology. . Trends Cogn. Sci. 20:(8):589604
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  108. Kanngiesser P, Warneken F. 2012.. Young children consider merit when sharing resources with others. . PLOS ONE 7:(8):e43979
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  109. Kemp J, Després O, Sellal F, Dufour A. 2012.. Theory of Mind in normal ageing and neurodegenerative pathologies. . Ageing Res. Rev. 11:(2):199219
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  110. Kendal RL, Boogert NJ, Rendell L, Laland KN, Webster M, Jones PL. 2018.. Social learning strategies: bridge-building between fields. . Trends Cogn. Sci. 22:(7):65165
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  111. Kettner SE, Waichman I. 2016.. Old age and prosocial behavior: social preferences or experimental confounds?. J. Econ. Psychol. 53::11830
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  112. Kienbaum J, Wilkening F. 2009.. Children's and adolescents’ intuitive judgements about distributive justice: integrating need, effort, and luck. . Eur. J. Dev. Psychol. 6:(4):48198
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  113. Köbis N, Starke C, Rahwan I. 2022.. The promise and perils of using artificial intelligence to fight corruption. . Nat. Mach. Intell. 4:(5):41824
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  114. Koster-Hale J, Saxe R. 2013.. Theory of mind: a neural prediction problem. . Neuron 79:(5):83648
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  115. Krettenauer T, Malti T, Sokol BW. 2008.. The development of moral emotion expectancies and the happy victimizer phenomenon: a critical review of theory and application. . Int. J. Dev. Sci. 2:(3):22135
    [Google Scholar]
  116. Kwak Y, Huettel SA. 2016.. Prosocial reward learning in children and adolescents. . Front. Psychol. 7::1539
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  117. Lee KJJ, Setoh P. 2023.. Early prosociality is conditional on opportunity cost and familiarity with the target. . Evol. Hum. Behav. 44:(1):3949
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  118. Lee NC, Jolles J, Krabbendam L. 2016.. Social information influences trust behaviour in adolescents. . J. Adolesc. 46::6675
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  119. Li V, Spitzer B, Olson KR. 2014.. Preschoolers reduce inequality while favoring individuals with more. . Child Dev. 85:(3):112333
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  120. Lind J, Ghirlanda S, Enquist M. 2019.. Social learning through associative processes: a computational theory. . R. Soc. Open Sci. 6:(3):181777
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  121. Lindström B, Jangard S, Selbing I, Olsson A. 2018.. The role of a “common is moral” heuristic in the stability and change of moral norms. . J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 147:(2):22842
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  122. LoBue V, Nishida T, Chiong C, DeLoache JS, Haidt J. 2011.. When getting something good is bad: Even three-year-olds react to inequality. . Soc. Dev. 20:(1):15470
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  123. Lockwood PL. 2016.. The anatomy of empathy: vicarious experience and disorders of social cognition. . Behav. Brain Res. 311::25566
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  124. Lockwood PL, Abdurahman A, Gabay AS, Drew D, Tamm M, et al. 2021.. Aging increases prosocial motivation for effort. . Psychol. Sci. 32:(5):66881
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  125. Lockwood PL, Apps MAJ, Chang SWC. 2020a.. Is there a “social” brain? Implementations and algorithms. . Trends Cogn. Sci. 24:(10):80213
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  126. Lockwood PL, Apps MAJ, Valton V, Viding E, Roiser JP. 2016.. Neurocomputational mechanisms of prosocial learning and links to empathy. . PNAS 113:(35):976368
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  127. Lockwood PL, Klein-Flügge MC. 2021.. Computational modelling of social cognition and behaviour—a reinforcement learning primer. . Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 16:(8):76171
    [Google Scholar]
  128. Lockwood PL, Klein-Flügge MC, Abdurahman A, Crockett MJ. 2020b.. Model-free decision making is prioritized when learning to avoid harming others. . PNAS 117:(44):27719
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  129. Lockwood PL, Wittmann MK, Apps MAJ, Klein-Flügge MC, Crockett MJ, et al. 2018.. Neural mechanisms for learning self and other ownership. . Nat. Commun. 9:(1):4747
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  130. Ma F, Heyman GD, Jing C, Fu Y, Compton BJ, et al. 2018.. Promoting honesty in young children through observational learning. . J. Exp. Child Psychol. 167::23445
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  131. Margoni F, Geipel J, Hadjichristidis C, Bakiaj R, Surian L. 2023.. Age-related differences in moral judgment: the role of probability judgments. . Cogn. Sci. 47:(9):e13345
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  132. Margoni F, Geipel J, Hadjichristidis C, Surian L. 2018.. Moral judgment in old age: evidence for an intent-to-outcome shift. . Exp. Psychol. 65:(2):10514
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  133. Margoni F, Geipel J, Hadjichristidis C, Surian L. 2021.. Inequity aversion in old age: an outcome bias in older adults’ socio-economic decisions. . Cogn. Dev. 58::101016
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  134. Mastroianni AM, Gilbert DT. 2023.. The illusion of moral decline. . Nature 618:(7966):78289
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  135. Matheson H, Moore C, Akhtar N. 2013.. The development of social learning in interactive and observational contexts. . J. Exp. Child Psychol. 114:(2):16172
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  136. Mayr U, Freund A. 2020.. Do we become more prosocial as we age, and if so, why?. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 29:(3):24854
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  137. Mayr U, Rohovit T, Freund AM. 2024.. Increases in prosociality across adulthood: the pure-altruism hypothesis. . Curr. Opin. Psychol. 55::101782
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  138. McAuliffe K, Blake PR, Kim G, Wrangham RW, Warneken F. 2013.. Social influences on inequity aversion in children. . PLOS ONE 8:(12):e80966
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  139. McAuliffe K, Jordan JJ, Warneken F. 2015.. Costly third-party punishment in young children. . Cognition 134::110
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  140. Mills KL, Tamnes CK. 2020.. Longitudinal structural and functional brain development in childhood and adolescence. . In The Oxford Handbook of Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, ed. K Cohen Kadosh , pp. 7598. Oxford, UK:: Oxford Univ. Press
    [Google Scholar]
  141. Molleman L, Ciranka S, van den Bos W. 2022.. Social influence in adolescence as a double-edged sword. . Proc. R. Soc. B 289:( 1977.):20220045
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  142. Molleman L, Kanngiesser P, van den Bos W. 2019.. Social information use in adolescents: the impact of adults, peers and household composition. . PLOS ONE 14:(11):e0225498
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  143. Moran JM. 2013.. Lifespan development: the effects of typical aging on theory of mind. . Behav. Brain Res. 237::3240
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  144. Murman DL. 2015.. The impact of age on cognition. . Semin. Hear. 36:(3):11121
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  145. Nihonsugi T, Ihara A, Haruno M. 2015.. Selective increase of intention-based economic decisions by noninvasive brain stimulation to the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. . J. Neurosci. 35:(8):341219
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  146. Nong T-TA, Qu C, Li Y, Woo JH, Yuqian W, et al. 2023.. Computational mechanisms underlying the emergence of theory of mind in children. . PsyArXiv, Sept. 5
    [Google Scholar]
  147. Nunner-Winkler G, Sodian B. 1988.. Children's understanding of moral emotions. . Child Dev. 59:(5):132338
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  148. Nussenbaum K, Cohen AO, Davis ZJ, Halpern DJ, Gureckis TM, Hartley CA. 2020.. Causal information-seeking strategies change across childhood and adolescence. . Cogn. Sci. 44:(9):e12888
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  149. Nussenbaum K, Hartley CA. 2019.. Reinforcement learning across development: What insights can we draw from a decade of research?. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 40::100733
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  150. Obeso I, Moisa M, Ruff CC, Dreher J-C. 2018.. A causal role for right temporo-parietal junction in signaling moral conflict. . eLife 7::e40671
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  151. Over H, Eggleston A, Bell J, Dunham Y. 2018.. Young children seek out biased information about social groups. . Dev. Sci. 21:(3):e12580
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  152. Park B, Fareri D, Delgado M, Young L. 2021.. The role of right temporo-parietal junction in processing social prediction error across relationship contexts. . Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 16:(8):77281
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  153. Park SA, Goïame S, O'Connor DA, Dreher J-C. 2017.. Integration of individual and social information for decision-making in groups of different sizes. . PLOS Biol. 15:(6):e2001958
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  154. Pauli R, Brazil IA, Kohls G, Klein-Flügge MC, Rogers JC, et al. 2023.. Action initiation and punishment learning differ from childhood to adolescence while reward learning remains stable. . Nat. Commun. 14:(1):5689
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  155. Pauli R, Lockwood PL. 2023.. The computational psychiatry of antisocial behaviour and psychopathy. . Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 145::104995
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  156. Paulus M. 2016.. Friendship trumps neediness: the impact of social relations and others’ wealth on preschool children's sharing. . J. Exp. Child Psychol. 146::10620
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  157. Paulus M. 2020.. Is young children's helping affected by helpees’ need? Preschoolers, but not infants selectively help needy others. . Psychol. Res. 84:(5):144050
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  158. Philippe R, Janet R, Khalvati K, Rao R, Lee D, Dreher J-C. 2024.. Neurocomputational mechanisms engaged in detecting cooperative and competitive intentions of others. . Nat. Commun. 15::3189
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  159. Piaget J. 2013.. The Moral Judgment of the Child. London:: Routledge
    [Google Scholar]
  160. Potter TCS, Bryce NV, Hartley CA. 2017.. Cognitive components underpinning the development of model-based learning. . Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 25::27280
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  161. Qu C, Bénistant J, Dreher J-C. 2022.. Neurocomputational mechanisms engaged in moral choices and moral learning. . Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 132::5060
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  162. Qu C, Hu Y, Tang Z, Derrington E, Dreher J-C. 2020.. Neurocomputational mechanisms underlying immoral decisions benefiting self or others. . Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 15:(2):13549
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  163. Qu C, Météreau E, Butera L, Villeval MC, Dreher J-C. 2019.. Neurocomputational mechanisms at play when weighing concerns for extrinsic rewards, moral values, and social image. . PLOS Biol. 17:(6):e3000283
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  164. Raihani NJ, McAuliffe K. 2012.. Human punishment is motivated by inequity aversion, not a desire for reciprocity. . Biol. Lett. 8:(5):8024
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  165. Reyna VF, Farley F. 2006.. Risk and rationality in adolescent decision making: implications for theory, practice, and public policy. . Psychol. Sci. Public Interest Suppl. 7:(1):144
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  166. Rheingold HL. 1982.. Little children's participation in the work of adults, a nascent prosocial behavior. . Child Dev. 53:(1):11425
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  167. Roos S, Hodges E, Salmivalli C. 2013.. Do guilt- and shame-proneness differentially predict prosocial, aggressive, and withdrawn behaviors during early adolescence?. Dev. Psychol. 50::94146
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  168. Rovee CK, Rovee DT. 1969.. Conjugate reinforcement of infant exploratory behavior. . J. Exp. Child Psychol. 8:(1):3339
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  169. Ruff CC, Ugazio G, Fehr E. 2013.. Changing social norm compliance with noninvasive brain stimulation. . Science 342:(6157):48284
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  170. Rutledge RB, De Berker AO, Espenhahn S, Dayan P, Dolan RJ. 2016.. The social contingency of momentary subjective well-being. . Nat. Commun. 7:(1):11825
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  171. Sai L, Bellucci G, Wang C, Fu G, Camilleri JA, et al. 2021.. Neural mechanisms of deliberate dishonesty: dissociating deliberation from other control processes during dishonest behaviors. . PNAS 118:(43):e2109208118
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  172. Samanez-Larkin GR, Knutson B. 2015.. Decision making in the ageing brain: changes in affective and motivational circuits. . Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 16::27889
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  173. Schaaf JV, Weidinger L, Molleman L, van den Bos W. 2024.. Test-retest reliability of reinforcement learning parameters. . Behav. Res. Methods 56::458299
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  174. Scholl J, Klein-Flügge M. 2018.. Understanding psychiatric disorder by capturing ecologically relevant features of learning and decision-making. . Behav. Brain Res. 355::5675
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  175. Seara-Cardoso A, Sebastian CL, McCrory E, Foulkes L, Buon M, et al. 2016.. Anticipation of guilt for everyday moral transgressions: the role of the anterior insula and the influence of interpersonal psychopathic traits. . Sci. Rep. 6:(1):36273
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  176. Setton R, Mwilambwe-Tshilobo L, Girn M, Lockrow AW, Baracchini G, et al. 2022.. Age differences in the functional architecture of the human brain. . Cereb. Cortex 33:(1):11434
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  177. Sheskin M, Bloom P, Wynn K. 2014.. Anti-equality: social comparison in young children. . Cognition 130:(2):15256
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  178. Siegel JZ, Estrada S, Crockett MJ, Baskin-Sommers A. 2019.. Exposure to violence affects the development of moral impressions and trust behavior in incarcerated males. . Nat. Commun. 10:(1):1942
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  179. Siegel JZ, Mathys C, Rutledge RB, Crockett MJ. 2018.. Beliefs about bad people are volatile. . Nat. Hum. Behav. 2:(10):75056
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  180. Sijtsma H, Lee NC, Braams BR, Hollarek M, Walsh RJ, et al. 2023.. The development of adolescent trust behavior. . J. Exp. Child Psychol. 231::105653
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  181. Spaans J, Peters S, Becht A, van der Cruijsen R, van de Groep S, Crone EA. 2023.. Longitudinal neural and behavioral trajectories of charity contributions across adolescence. . J. Res. Adolesc. 33:(2):48095
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  182. Speer SPH, Smidts A, Boksem MAS. 2020.. Cognitive control increases honesty in cheaters but cheating in those who are honest. . PNAS 117:(32):1908091
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  183. Steinbeis N. 2016.. The role of self-other distinction in understanding others’ mental and emotional states: neurocognitive mechanisms in children and adults. . Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B 371:( 1686.):20150074
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  184. Su Z, Garvert M, Zhang L, Manohar SG, Vogel TA, et al. 2024.. Older adults are more susceptible to impulsive social influence. . PsyArXiv, Febr. 7:. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/zdr7q
    [Google Scholar]
  185. Sullivan NJ, Li R, Huettel SA. 2022.. Peer presence increases the prosocial behavior of adolescents by speeding the evaluation of outcomes for others. . Sci. Rep. 12:(1):6477
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  186. Tafreshi D, Thompson JJ, Racine TP. 2014.. An analysis of the conceptual foundations of the infant preferential looking paradigm. . Hum. Dev. 57:(4):22240
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  187. Tang Z, Qu C, Hu Y, Benistant J, Moisan F, et al. 2023.. Strengths of social ties modulate brain computations for third-party punishment. . Sci. Rep. 13:(1):10510
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  188. Tangney JP, Stuewig J, Mashek DJ. 2007.. Moral emotions and moral behavior. . Annu. Rev. Psychol. 58::34572
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  189. Tierney AL, Nelson CA. 2009.. Brain development and the role of experience in the early years. . Zero Three 30:(2):913
    [Google Scholar]
  190. Toelch U, Dolan RJ. 2015.. Informational and normative influences in conformity from a neurocomputational perspective. . Trends Cogn. Sci. 19:(10):57989
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  191. Tomasello M, Vaish A. 2013.. Origins of human cooperation and morality. . Annu. Rev. Psychol. 64::23155
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  192. Tousignant B, Eugène F, Sirois K, Jackson PL. 2018.. Difference in neural response to social exclusion observation and subsequent altruism between adolescents and adults. . Neuropsychologia 116::1525
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  193. Tummeltshammer KS, Mareschal D, Kirkham NZ. 2014.. Infants’ selective attention to reliable visual cues in the presence of salient distractors. . Child Dev. 85:(5):198194
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  194. Ugazio G, Grueschow M, Polania R, Lamm C, Tobler PN, Ruff CC. 2022.. Neuro-computational foundations of moral preferences. . Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 17:(3):25365
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  195. van Baar JM, Chang LJ, Sanfey AG. 2019.. The computational and neural substrates of moral strategies in social decision-making. . Nat. Commun. 10::1483
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  196. van de Groep S, Meuwese R, Zanolie K, Güroğlu B, Crone EA. 2020.. Developmental changes and individual differences in trust and reciprocity in adolescence. . J. Res. Adolesc. 30:(S1):192208
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  197. van den Bos W, Bruckner R, Nassar MR, Mata R, Eppinger B. 2018.. Computational neuroscience across the lifespan: promises and pitfalls. . Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 33::4253
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  198. van den Bos W, Cohen MX, Kahnt T, Crone EA. 2012a.. Striatum-medial prefrontal cortex connectivity predicts developmental changes in reinforcement learning. . Cereb. Cortex 22:(6):124755
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  199. van den Bos W, Vahl P, Güroğlu B, van Nunspeet F, Colins O, et al. 2014.. Neural correlates of social decision-making in severely antisocial adolescents. . Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 9:(12):205966
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  200. van den Bos W, van Dijk E, Crone EA. 2012b.. Learning whom to trust in repeated social interactions: a developmental perspective. . Group Process. Intergroup Relatsh. 15:(2):24356
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  201. van den Bos W, van Dijk E, Westenberg M, Rombouts SARB, Crone EA. 2011.. Changing brains, changing perspectives: the neurocognitive development of reciprocity. . Psychol. Sci. 22:(1):6070
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  202. van den Bos W, Westenberg M, van Dijk E, Crone EA. 2010.. Development of trust and reciprocity in adolescence. . Cogn. Dev. 25:(1):90102
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  203. van Hoorn J, Fuligni AJ, Crone EA, Galván A. 2016a.. Peer influence effects on risk-taking and prosocial decision-making in adolescence: insights from neuroimaging studies. . Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 10::5964
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  204. van Hoorn J, van Dijk E, Güroğlu B, Crone EA. 2016b.. Neural correlates of prosocial peer influence on public goods game donations during adolescence. . Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 11:(6):92333
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  205. Vogel T, Lockwood PL. 2024.. Normative processing needs multiple levels of explanation: from algorithm to implementation. . Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 19:(1):5356
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  206. Waltmann M, Schlagenhauf F, Deserno L. 2022.. Sufficient reliability of the behavioral and computational readouts of a probabilistic reversal learning task. . Behav. Res. 54:(6):29933014
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  207. Warneken F, Tomasello M. 2009.. The roots of human altruism. . Br. J. Psychol. 100:(3):45571
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  208. Watkins CJCH, Dayan P. 1992.. Q-learning. . Mach. Learn. 8:(3):27992
    [Google Scholar]
  209. Westhoff B, Blankenstein NE, Schreuders E, Crone EA, van Duijvenvoorde ACK. 2021.. Increased ventromedial prefrontal cortex activity in adolescence benefits prosocial reinforcement learning. . Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 52::101018
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  210. Westhoff B, Molleman L, Viding E, van den Bos W, van Duijvenvoorde AC. 2020.. Developmental asymmetries in learning to adjust to cooperative and uncooperative environments. . Sci. Rep. 10:(1):21761
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  211. Will G-J, Crone EA, van den Bos W, Güroğlu B. 2013.. Acting on observed social exclusion: developmental perspectives on punishment of excluders and compensation of victims. . Dev. Psychol. 49:(12):223644
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  212. Will G-J, Rutledge RB, Moutoussis M, Dolan RJ. 2017.. Neural and computational processes underlying dynamic changes in self-esteem. . eLife 6::e28098
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  213. Wittmann MK, Lockwood PL, Rushworth MFS. 2018.. Neural mechanisms of social cognition in primates. . Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 41::99118
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  214. Wynn K, Bloom P, Jordan A, Marshall J, Sheskin M. 2018.. Not noble savages after all: limits to early altruism. . Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 27:(1):38
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  215. Xiang T, Lohrenz T, Montague PR. 2013.. Computational substrates of norms and their violations during social exchange. . J. Neurosci. 33:(3):1099108
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  216. Yu H, Siegel JZ, Clithero JA, Crockett MJ. 2021.. How peer influence shapes value computation in moral decision-making. . Cognition 211::104641
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  217. Yu H, Siegel JZ, Crockett MJ. 2019.. Modeling morality in 3-D: decision-making, judgment, and inference. . Topics Cogn. Sci. 11:(2):40932
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  218. Zahn R, de Oliveira-Souza R, Moll J. 2020.. Moral motivation and the basal forebrain. . Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 108::20717
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  219. Zeman J, Cassano M, Perry-Parrish C, Stegall S. 2006.. Emotion regulation in children and adolescents. . J. Dev. Behav. Pediatr. 27:(2):15568
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  220. Zhang L, Lengersdorff L, Mikus N, Gläscher J, Lamm C. 2020.. Using reinforcement learning models in social neuroscience: frameworks, pitfalls and suggestions of best practices. . Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 15:(6):695707
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  221. Zhong S, Chark R, Hsu M, Chew SH. 2016.. Computational substrates of social norm enforcement by unaffected third parties. . NeuroImage 129::95104
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  222. Zhu L, Jenkins AC, Set E, Scabini D, Knight RT, et al. 2014.. Damage to dorsolateral prefrontal cortex affects tradeoffs between honesty and self-interest. . Nat. Neurosci. 17:(10):131921
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  223. Ziv T, Sommerville JA. 2017.. Developmental differences in infants’ fairness expectations from 6 to 15 months of age. . Child Dev. 88:(6):193051
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
/content/journals/10.1146/annurev-psych-021324-060611
Loading
/content/journals/10.1146/annurev-psych-021324-060611
Loading

Data & Media loading...

  • Article Type: Review Article
This is a required field
Please enter a valid email address
Approval was a Success
Invalid data
An Error Occurred
Approval was partially successful, following selected items could not be processed due to error