1932

Abstract

Because of personal motives and the architecture of the mind, it may be difficult for people to know themselves. People often attempt to block out unwanted thoughts and feelings through conscious suppression and perhaps through unconscious repression, though whether such attempts are successful is controversial. A more common source of self-knowledge failure is the inaccessibility of much of the mind to consciousness, including mental processes involved in perception, motor learning, personality, attitudes, and self-esteem. Introspection cannot provide a direct pipeline to these mental processes, though some types of introspection may help people construct beneficial personal narratives. Other ways of increasing self-knowledge include looking at ourselves through the eyes of others and observing our own behavior. These approaches can potentially promote self-knowledge, although major obstacles exist. It is not always advantageous to hold self-perceptions that correspond perfectly with reality, but increasing awareness of nonconscious motives and personality is generally beneficial.

Loading

Article metrics loading...

/content/journals/10.1146/annurev.psych.55.090902.141954
2004-02-04
2025-05-12
Loading full text...

Full text loading...

/deliver/fulltext/ps/55/1/annurev.psych.55.090902.141954.html?itemId=/content/journals/10.1146/annurev.psych.55.090902.141954&mimeType=html&fmt=ahah

Literature Cited

  1. Allport GW. 1961. Pattern and Growth in Personality New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston [Google Scholar]
  2. Anderson MC, Green C. 2001. Suppressing unwanted memories by executive control. Nature 410:366–69 [Google Scholar]
  3. Armor DA, Taylor SE. 1998. Situated optimism: specific outcome expectancies and self-regulation. In Advances in Experimental Social Psychology ed. MP Zanna 30309–79 San Diego: Academic [Google Scholar]
  4. Asendorpf JB, Banse R, Muecke D. 2002. Double dissociation between implicit and explicit personality self-concept: the case of shy behavior. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 83:380–93 [Google Scholar]
  5. Bargh JA. 1994. The four horsemen of automaticity: awareness, intention, efficiency, and control in social cognition. In Handbook of Social Cognition Vol. 1 Basic Processes Vol. 2Applications ed. RS Wyer Jr, TK Srull pp. 1–40 Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, 2nd ed.. [Google Scholar]
  6. Bartholomew K, Shaver PR. 1998. Methods of assessing adult attachment: Do they converge?. In Attachment Theory and Close Relationships ed. JA Simpson, WS Rholes pp. 25–45 New York: Guilford [Google Scholar]
  7. Basden BH, Basden DR, Gargano GJ. 1993. Directed forgetting in implicit and explicit memory tests: a comparison of methods. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 19:603–16 [Google Scholar]
  8. Baumeister RF. 1984. Choking under pressure: self-consciousness and paradoxical effects of incentives on skillful performance. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 46:610–20 [Google Scholar]
  9. Baumeister RF. 1989. The optimal margin of illusion. J. Soc. Clin. Psychol. 8:176–89 [Google Scholar]
  10. Baumeister RF, Showers CJ. 1986. A review of paradoxical performance effects: choking under pressure in sports and mental tests. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 16:361–83 [Google Scholar]
  11. Bem DJ. 1972. Self-perception theory. In Advances in Experimental Social Psychology ed. L Berkowitz 61–62 New York: Academic [Google Scholar]
  12. Bhalla M, Proffitt DR. 2000. Geographical slant perception: dissociation and coordination between explicit awareness and visually guided actions. In Beyond Dissociation: Interaction Between Dissociated Implicit and Explicit Processing ed. Y Rossetti, A Revonsuo pp. 99–128 Amsterdam: Benjamin [Google Scholar]
  13. Bjork RA. 1989. Retrieval inhibition as an adaptive mechanism in human memory. In Varieties of Memory and Consciousness: Essays in Honour of Endel Tulving ed. HL Roediger III, FIM Craik pp. 303–30 Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum [Google Scholar]
  14. Blair IV. 2002. The malleability of automatic stereotypes and prejudice. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 6:242–61 [Google Scholar]
  15. Bornstein RF. 1995. Sex differences in objective and projective dependency tests: a meta-analytic review. Assessment 2:319–31 [Google Scholar]
  16. Bosson JK. 2003. Discrepant explicit and implicit self-esteem: relations to self- and friend-ratings of personality and physical health. Unpubl. manuscr., Univ. Texas, Austin
  17. Bosson JK, Swann WB, Pennebaker JW. 2000. Stalking the perfect measure of implicit self-esteem: the blind men and the elephant revisited?. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 79:631–43 [Google Scholar]
  18. Brauer M, Wasel W, Niedenthal P. 2000. Implicit and explicit components of prejudice. Rev. Gen. Psychol. 4:79–101 [Google Scholar]
  19. Brunstein JC, Schultheiss OC, Grässmann R. 1998. Personal goals and emotional well-being: the moderating role of motive dispositions. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 75:494–508 [Google Scholar]
  20. Buehler R, Griffin D, Ross M. 1994. Exploring the “planning fallacy”: why people underestimate their task completion times. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 67:366–81 [Google Scholar]
  21. Carver CS, Scheier MF. 1981. Attention and Self-Regulation New York: Springer [Google Scholar]
  22. Cooley CH. 1902. Human Nature and the Social Order New York: Scribner's [Google Scholar]
  23. Crandall CS, Eshleman A. 2003. A justification-suppression model of the expression and experience of prejudice. Psychol. Bull. 129:414–46 [Google Scholar]
  24. Creem SH, Proffitt DR. 1999. Separate memories for visual guidance and explicit awareness: the roles of time and place. In Stratification in Cognition and Consciousness ed. BH Challis, BM Velichkovsky pp. 73–94 Amsterdam: Benjamins [Google Scholar]
  25. Devine PG. 1989. Automatic controlled processes in prejudice: the role of stereotypes and personal beliefs. In Attitude Structure and Function ed. AR Pratkanis, SJ Breckler, AG Greenwald pp. 181–212 Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum [Google Scholar]
  26. Dovidio JF, Kawakami K, Johnson C, Johnson B, Howard A. 1997. On the nature of prejudice: automatic and controlled processes. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 33:510–40 [Google Scholar]
  27. Erdelyi MH. 1974. A new look at the new look: perceptual defense and vigilance. Psychol. Rev. 81:1–25 [Google Scholar]
  28. Erdelyi MH. 1985. Psychoanalysis: Freud's Cognitive Psychology New York: Freeman [Google Scholar]
  29. Erdelyi MH. 1993. Repression: the mechanism and the defense. In Handbook of Mental Control ed. DM Wegner, JW Pennebaker pp. 126–48 Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall [Google Scholar]
  30. Fazio RH, Chen J, McDonel E, Sherman SJ. 1982. Attitude accessibility, attitude-behavior consistency, and the strength of the object-evaluation association. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 18:339–57 [Google Scholar]
  31. Fazio RH, Jackson JR, Dunton BC, Williams CJ. 1995. Variability in automatic activation as an unobstrusive measure of racial attitudes: a bona fide pipeline?. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 69:1013–27 [Google Scholar]
  32. Fazio RH, Olson MA. 2003. Implicit measures in social cognition research: their meaning and uses. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 54:297–327 [Google Scholar]
  33. Felson RB. 1993. The (somewhat) social self: how others affect self-appraisals. In The Self in Social Perspective ed. JM Suls 41–27 Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum [Google Scholar]
  34. Fitts PM. 1964. Perceptual-motor skill learning. In Categories of Human Learning ed. AW Melton pp. 243–85 New York: Academic [Google Scholar]
  35. Gabrieli JDE. 1998. Cognitive neuroscience of human memory. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 49:87–115 [Google Scholar]
  36. Gaertner SL, Dovidio JF. 1986. The aversive form of racism. In Prejudice, Discrimination, and Racism ed. JF Dovidio, SL Gaertner pp. 61–89 Orlando: Academic [Google Scholar]
  37. Greenwald AG, Banaji MR. 1995. Implicit social cognition: attitudes, self-esteem, and stereotypes. Psychol. Rev. 102:4–27 [Google Scholar]
  38. Greenwald AG, Farnham SD. 2000. Using the Implicit Association Test to measure self-esteem and self-concept. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 79:1022–38 [Google Scholar]
  39. Greenwald AG, McGhee DE, Schwartz JLK. 1998. Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: the implicit association test. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 74:1464–80 [Google Scholar]
  40. Halberstadt JB, Levine GM. 1999. Effects of reasons analysis on the accuracy of predicting basketball games. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 29:517–30 [Google Scholar]
  41. Jenkins IH, Brooks DJ, Nixon PD, Frackowiak RSJ, Passingham RE. et al. 1994. Motor sequence learning: a study with positron emission tomography. J. Neurosci. 14:3775–90 [Google Scholar]
  42. Jones EE. 1990. Interpersonal Perception New York: Freeman [Google Scholar]
  43. Jordan CH, Spencer SJ, Zanna MP. 2003. “I love me… I love me not:” implicit self-esteem, explicit self-esteem, and defensiveness. See Spencer et al. 2003 pp. 117–46 [Google Scholar]
  44. Kenny DA. 1994. Interpersonal Perception: A Social Relations Analysis New York: Guilford [Google Scholar]
  45. Kenny DA, DePaulo BM. 1993. Do people know how others view them? An empirical and theoretical account. Psychol. Bull. 114:145–61 [Google Scholar]
  46. Kiesler CA, Nisbett RE, Zanna MP. 1969. On inferring one's beliefs from one's behavior. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 11:321–27 [Google Scholar]
  47. Kihlstrom JF. 1987. The cognitive unconscious. Science 237:1445–52 [Google Scholar]
  48. Kihlstrom JF. 2002. No need for repression. Trends Cogn. Sci. 6:502 [Google Scholar]
  49. Kimble CE, Rezabek JS. 1992. Playing games before an audience: social facilitation or choking. Soc. Behav. Personal. 20:115–20 [Google Scholar]
  50. Klonsky ED, Oltmanns TF, Turkheimer E. 2002. Informant-reports of personality disorder: relation to self-reports and future research directions. Clin. Psychol.-Sci. Pract. 9:300–11 [Google Scholar]
  51. Kolar DW, Funder DC, Colvin CR. 1996. Comparing the accuracy of personality judgments by the self and knowledgeable others. J. Personal. 64:311–37 [Google Scholar]
  52. Koole SL, Dijksterhuis A, van Knippenberg A. 2001. What's in a name: implicit self-esteem and the automatic self. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 80:669–85 [Google Scholar]
  53. Koole SL, Pelham BW. 2003. On the nature of implicit self-esteem: the case of the name letter effect. See Spencer et al. 2003 pp. 93–116 [Google Scholar]
  54. Loftus EF. 1997. Memories for a past that never was. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 6:60–65 [Google Scholar]
  55. Logan GD. 1985. Skill automaticity: relations, implications, and future directions. Can. J. Psychol. 39:367–86 [Google Scholar]
  56. Lyubomirsky S, Caldwell ND, Nolen-Hoeksema S. 1998. Effects of ruminative distracting responses to depressed mood on retrieval of autobiographical memories. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 75:166–77 [Google Scholar]
  57. Macrae CN, Bodenhausen GV, Milne AB, Jetten J. 1994. Out of mind but back in sight: stereotypes on the rebound. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 67:808–17 [Google Scholar]
  58. Mazzoni GAL, Loftus EF, Kirsch I. 2001. Changing beliefs about implausible autobiographical events: A little plausibility goes a long way. J. Exp. Psychol. Appl. 7:51–59 [Google Scholar]
  59. McAdams DP. 1993. The Stories We Live By: Personal Myths and the Making of the Self New York: Morrow [Google Scholar]
  60. McAdams DP. 2001. The psychology of life stories. Rev. Gen. Psychol. 5:100–22 [Google Scholar]
  61. McClelland DC, Koestner R, Weinberger J. 1989. How do self-attributed implicit motives differ?. Psychol. Rev. 96:690–702 [Google Scholar]
  62. McGinnies E. 1949. Emotionality and perceptual defense. Psychol. Rev. 56:244–51 [Google Scholar]
  63. Mead GH. 1934. Mind, Self, and Society from the Standpoint of a Social Behaviorist Chicago: Univ. Chicago Press [Google Scholar]
  64. Monteith MJ, Spicer CV, Tooman GD. 1998. Consequences of stereotype suppression: stereotypes on AND not on the rebound. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 34:355–77 [Google Scholar]
  65. Moskowitz GB, Gollwitzer PM, Wasel W, Schaal B. 1999. Preconscious control of stereotype activation through chronic egalitarian goals. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 77:167–84 [Google Scholar]
  66. Moskowitz GB, Salomon AR, Taylor CM. 2000. Preconsciously controlling stereotyping: Implicitly activated egalitarian goals prevent the activation of stereotypes. Soc. Cogn. 18:151–77 [Google Scholar]
  67. Nisbett RE, Wilson TD. 1977. Telling more than we can know: verbal reports on mental processes. Psychol. Rev. 84:231–59 [Google Scholar]
  68. Nissen MJ, Bullemer P. 1987. Attentional requirements of learning: evidence from performance measures. Cogn. Psychol. 19:1–32 [Google Scholar]
  69. Nolen-Hoeksema S. 2000. The role of rumination in depressive disorders and mixed anxiety/depressive symptoms. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 109:504–11 [Google Scholar]
  70. Nolen-Hoeksema S, McBride A, Larson J. 1997. Rumination and psychological distress among bereaved partners. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 72:855–62 [Google Scholar]
  71. Nosek BA. 2002.Moderators of the relationship between implicit and explicit attitudes. Unpubl. PhD diss., Yale Univ.
  72. Nosek BA, Banaji MR. 2002. (At least) two factors moderate the relationship between implicit and explicit attitudes. In Natura Automatyzmow ed. RK Ohme, M Jarymowicz pp. 49–56 Warszawa: WIP PAN & SWPS [Google Scholar]
  73. Nosek BA, Banaji MR, Greenwald AG. 2002. Harvesting implicit group attitudes and beliefs from a demonstration web site. Group Dyn.: Theory Res. Pract. 6:101–15 [Google Scholar]
  74. Nuttin JM. 1985. Narcissism beyond Gestalt and awareness: the name letter effect. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 15:353–61 [Google Scholar]
  75. Oettingen G. 1996. Positive fantasy and motivation. In The Psychology of Action: Linking Cognition and Motivation to Behavior ed. PM Gollwitzer, JA Bargh pp. 226–39 New York: Guilford [Google Scholar]
  76. Oettingen G, Wadden TA. 1991. Expectation, fantasy, and weight loss: Is the impact of positive thinking always positive?. Cogn. Ther. Res. 15:167–75 [Google Scholar]
  77. Paz-Caballero MD, Menor J. 1999. ERP correlates of directed forgetting effects in direct and indirect memory tests. Eur. J. Cogn. Psychol. 11:239–60 [Google Scholar]
  78. Pelham BW, Mirenberg MC, Jones JT. 2002. Why Susie sells seashells by the seashore: implicit egotism and major life decisions. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 82:469–87 [Google Scholar]
  79. Pennebaker JW. 1997. Opening Up: The Healing Power of Expressing Emotions New York: Guilford [Google Scholar]
  80. Pennebaker JW, Colder M, Sharp LK. 1990. Accelerating the coping process. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 58:528–37 [Google Scholar]
  81. Pennebaker JW, Kiecolt-Glaser JK, Glaser R. 1988. Disclosure of traumas and immune function: health implications for psychotherapy. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 56:239–45 [Google Scholar]
  82. Pennebaker JW, Mayne TJ, Francis ME. 1997. Linguistic predictors of adaptive bereavement. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 72:863–71 [Google Scholar]
  83. Peterson C, Ulrey LM. 1994. Can explanatory style be scored from TAT protocols?. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 20:102–6 [Google Scholar]
  84. Proffitt DR, Bhalla M, Gossweiler R, Midgett J. 1995. Perceiving geographical slant. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 2:409–28 [Google Scholar]
  85. Proffitt DR, Stefanucci J, Banton T, Epstein W. 2003. The role of effort in perceiving distance. Psychol. Sci. 14:106–12 [Google Scholar]
  86. Robinson MD, Vargas PT, Crawford EG. 2003. Putting process into personality, appraisal, and emotion: evaluative processing as a missing link. In The Psychology of Evaluation: Affective Processes in Cognition and Emotion ed. J Musch, KC Klauer pp. 275–306 Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum [Google Scholar]
  87. Ross L, Nisbett RE. 1991. The Person and the Situation: Perspectives of Social Psychology New York: McGraw-Hill [Google Scholar]
  88. Schachter S, Singer J. 1962. Cognitive, social, and physiological determinants of emotional state. Psychol. Rev. 69:379–99 [Google Scholar]
  89. Schooler JW. 2001. Discovering memories of abuse in the light of meta-awareness. J. Aggress. Maltreat. Trauma 4:105–36 [Google Scholar]
  90. Schultheiss OC. 2001. An information processing account of implicit motive arousal. In Advances in Motivation and Achievement ed. ML Maehr, P Pintrich 121–41 Greenwich: JAI [Google Scholar]
  91. Schultheiss OC, Brunstein JC. 1999. Goal imagery: bridging the gap between implicit motives and explicit goals. J. Personal. 67:1–38 [Google Scholar]
  92. Shanks DR, St. John MF. 1994. Characteristics of dissociable human learning systems. Behav. Brain Sci. 17:367–447 [Google Scholar]
  93. Shrauger JS, Schoeneman TJ. 1979. Symbolic interactionist view of self-concept: through the looking glass darkly. Psychol. Bull. 86:549–73 [Google Scholar]
  94. Silvia PJ, Gendolla GHE. 2001. On introspection and self-perception: Does self-focused attention enable accurate self-knowledge?. Rev. Gen. Psychol 5:241–69 [Google Scholar]
  95. Snyder M. 1982. When believing means doing: creating links between attitudes and behavior. See Zanna et al. 1982 pp. 105–30 [Google Scholar]
  96. Spain JS, Eaton LG, Funder DC. 2000. Perspectives on personality: the relative accuracy of self versus others for the prediction of emotion behavior. J. Personal. 68:837–67 [Google Scholar]
  97. Spalding LR, Hardin CD. 1999. Unconscious unease and self-handicapping: behavioral consequences of individual differences in implicit and explicit self-esteem. Psychol. Sci. 10:535–39 [Google Scholar]
  98. Spangler WD. 1992. Validity of questionnaire and TAT measures of need for achievement: two meta-analyses. Psychol. Bull. 112:140–54 [Google Scholar]
  99. Spencer SJ, Fein S, Zanna MP, Olson JM. eds 2003. Motivated Social Perception: The Ontario Symposium Vol. 9 Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum [Google Scholar]
  100. Spera SP, Buhrfeind ED, Pennebaker JW. 1994. Expressive writing and coping with job loss. Acad. Manag. J. 37:722–33 [Google Scholar]
  101. Taylor SE, Brown JD. 1988. Illusion and well being: a social-psychological perspective on mental health. Psychol. Bull. 116:21–27 [Google Scholar]
  102. Taylor SE, Kemeny ME, Aspinwall LG, Schneider SG, Rodriguez R. et al. 1992. Optimism, coping, psychological distress, and high-risk sexual behavior among men at risk for acquired immunodeficiency syndrome (AIDS). J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 63:460–73 [Google Scholar]
  103. Valins S. 1966. Cognitive effects of false heart-rate feedback. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 4:400–8 [Google Scholar]
  104. Ward A, Lyubomirsky S, Sousa L, Nolen-Hoeksema S. 2003. Can't quite commit: rumination and uncertainty. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 29:96–107 [Google Scholar]
  105. Wegner DM. 1994. Ironic processes of mental control. Psychol. Rev. 101:34–52 [Google Scholar]
  106. Wegner DM, Schneider DJ, Carter SR, White TL. 1987. Paradoxical effects of thought suppression. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 53:5–13 [Google Scholar]
  107. Wenzlaff RM, Wegner DM. 2000. Thought suppression. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 51:59–91 [Google Scholar]
  108. Wicklund RA. 1982. Self-focused attention and the validity of self-reports. See Zanna et al. 1982 pp. 149–72 [Google Scholar]
  109. Willingham DB. 1998. A neuropsychological theory of motor skill learning. Psychol. Rev. 105:(3)558–84 [Google Scholar]
  110. Willingham DB, Goedert-Eschmann K. 1999. The relation between implicit and explicit learning: Evidence for parallel development. Psychol. Sci. 10:531–34 [Google Scholar]
  111. Wilson TD. 2002. Strangers to Ourselves: Discovering the Adaptive Unconscious Cambridge, MA: Harvard Univ. Press [Google Scholar]
  112. Wilson TD, Dunn DS. 1986. Effects of introspection on attitude-behavior consistency: analyzing reasons versus focusing on feelings. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 22:249–63 [Google Scholar]
  113. Wilson TD, Dunn DS, Bybee JA, Hyman DB, Rotondo JA. 1984. Effects of analyzing reasons on attitude-behavior consistency. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 47:5–16 [Google Scholar]
  114. Wilson TD, Dunn DS, Kraft D, Lisle DJ. 1989. Introspection, attitude change, and attitude-behavior consistency: the disruptive effects of explaining why we feel the way we do. In Advances in Experimental Social Psychology ed. L Berkowitz 22287–343 New York: Academic [Google Scholar]
  115. Wilson TD, Hodges SD, LaFleur SJ. 1995. Effects of introspecting about reasons: inferring attitudes from accessible thoughts. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 69:16–28 [Google Scholar]
  116. Wilson TD, LaFleur SJ. 1995. Knowing what you'll do: effects of analyzing reasons on self-prediction. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 68:21–35 [Google Scholar]
  117. Wilson TD, Lindsey S, Schooler TY. 2000. A model of dual attitudes. Psychol. Rev. 107:101–26 [Google Scholar]
  118. Wilson TD, Lisle DJ, Schooler JW, Hodges SD, Klaaren KJ. et al. 1993. Introspecting about reasons can reduce post-choice satisfaction. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 19:331–39 [Google Scholar]
  119. Wilson TD, Schooler JW. 1991. Thinking too much: Introspection can reduce the quality of preferences and decisions. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 60:181–92 [Google Scholar]
  120. Zanna MP, Higgins ET, Herman CP. eds 1982. Consistency in Social Behavior: The Ontario Symposium Vol. 2 Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum [Google Scholar]
  121. Zillmann D. 1978. Attribution and misattribution of excitatory reaction. In New Directions in Attribution Research ed. JH Harvey, W Ickes, RF Kidd 2335–68 Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum [Google Scholar]
/content/journals/10.1146/annurev.psych.55.090902.141954
Loading
/content/journals/10.1146/annurev.psych.55.090902.141954
Loading

Data & Media loading...

  • Article Type: Review Article
This is a required field
Please enter a valid email address
Approval was a Success
Invalid data
An Error Occurred
Approval was partially successful, following selected items could not be processed due to error