1932

Abstract

A common view of learning in infancy emphasizes the role of incidental sensory experiences from which increasingly abstract statistical regularities are extracted. In this view, infant brains initially support basic sensory and motor functions, followed by maturation of higher-level association cortex. Here, we critique this view and posit that, by contrast and more like adults, infants are active, endogenously motivated learners who structure their own learning through flexible selection of attentional targets and active interventions on their environment. We further argue that the infant brain, and particularly the prefrontal cortex (PFC), is well equipped to support these learning behaviors. We review recent progress in characterizing the function of the infant PFC, which suggests that, as in adults, the PFC is functionally specialized and highly connected. Together, we present an integrative account of infant minds and brains, in which the infant PFC represents multiple intrinsic motivations, which are leveraged for active learning.

Loading

Article metrics loading...

/content/journals/10.1146/annurev-devpsych-121318-084841
2020-12-15
2024-04-20
Loading full text...

Full text loading...

/deliver/fulltext/devpsych/2/1/annurev-devpsych-121318-084841.html?itemId=/content/journals/10.1146/annurev-devpsych-121318-084841&mimeType=html&fmt=ahah

Literature Cited

  1. Adolph KE, Hoch JE, Cole WG 2018. Development (of walking): 15 suggestions. Trends Cogn. Sci. 22:699–711
    [Google Scholar]
  2. Alcauter S, Lin W, Smith J, Short S, Goldman B et al. 2014. Development of thalamocortical connectivity during infancy and its cognitive correlations. J. Neurosci. 34:9067–75
    [Google Scholar]
  3. Alexander WH, Brown JW. 2018. Frontal cortex function as derived from hierarchical predictive coding. Sci. Rep. 8:3843
    [Google Scholar]
  4. Arcaro MJ, Schade PF, Livingstone MS 2019. Universal mechanisms and the development of the face network: What you see is what you get. Annu. Rev. Vis. Sci. 5:341–72
    [Google Scholar]
  5. Atkinson J, Braddick O, Moar K 1977. Development of contrast sensitivity over the first 3 months of life in the human infant. Vis. Res. 17:1037–44
    [Google Scholar]
  6. Baillargeon R. 1987. Object permanence in 3.5- and 4.5-month-old infants. Dev. Psychol. 23:655–64
    [Google Scholar]
  7. Baillargeon R, Spelke ES, Wasserman S 1985. Object permanence in five-month-old infants. Cognition 20:191–208
    [Google Scholar]
  8. Ball G, Pazderova L, Chew A, Tusor N, Merchant N et al. 2015. Thalamocortical connectivity predicts cognition in children born preterm. Cereb. Cortex 25:4310–18
    [Google Scholar]
  9. Barbey AK, Koenigs M, Grafman J 2013. Dorsolateral prefrontal contributions to human working memory. Cortex 49:1195–205
    [Google Scholar]
  10. Bartra O, McGuire JT, Kable JW 2013. The valuation system: a coordinate-based meta-analysis of BOLD fMRI experiments examining neural correlates of subjective value. NeuroImage 76:412–27
    [Google Scholar]
  11. Batterink LJ, Paller KA, Reber PJ 2019. Understanding the neural bases of implicit and statistical learning. Top. Cogn. Sci. 11:482–503
    [Google Scholar]
  12. Bedny M. 2017. Evidence from blindness for a cognitively pluripotent cortex. Trends Cogn. Sci. 21:637–48
    [Google Scholar]
  13. Bengio Y, Louradour J, Collobert R, Weston J 2009. Curriculum learning. Proceedings of the 26th Annual International Conference on Machine Learning (ICML '09) L Bottou, M Littman 41–48 New York: ACM Press
    [Google Scholar]
  14. Bourvis N, Singer M, Saint Georges C, Bodeau N, Chetouani M et al. 2018. Pre-linguistic infants employ complex communicative loops to engage mothers in social exchanges and repair interaction ruptures. R. Soc. Open Sci. 5:170274
    [Google Scholar]
  15. Boyer TW, Harding SM, Bertenthal BI 2020. The temporal dynamics of infants’ joint attention: effects of others’ gaze cues and manual actions. Cognition 197:104151
    [Google Scholar]
  16. Browning M, Holmes EA, Murphy SE, Goodwin GM, Harmer CJ 2010. Lateral prefrontal cortex mediates the cognitive modification of attentional bias. Biol. Psychiatry 67:919–25
    [Google Scholar]
  17. Buckner RL, DiNicola LM. 2019. The brain's default network: updated anatomy, physiology and evolving insights. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 20:593–608
    [Google Scholar]
  18. Collin G, van den Heuvel MP 2013. The ontogeny of the human connectome. Neuroscientist 19:616–28
    [Google Scholar]
  19. Colombo J, Mitchell DW. 2009. Infant visual habituation. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 92:225–34
    [Google Scholar]
  20. Cristia A, Dupoux E, Gurven M, Stieglitz J 2017. Child-directed speech is infrequent in a forager-farmer population: a time allocation study. Child Dev 90:759–73
    [Google Scholar]
  21. Crone EA, Steinbeis N. 2017. Neural perspectives on cognitive control development during childhood and adolescence. Trends Cogn. Sci. 21:205–15
    [Google Scholar]
  22. Csibra G, Hernik M, Mascaro O, Tatone D, Lengyel M 2016. Statistical treatment of looking-time data. Dev. Psychol. 52:521–36
    [Google Scholar]
  23. Cusack R, Wild CJ, Zubiaurre-Elorza L, Linke AC 2018. Why does language not emerge until the second year. ? Hear. Res. 366:75–81
    [Google Scholar]
  24. de Lange FP, Heilbron M, Kok P 2018. How do expectations shape perception. ? Trends Cogn. Sci. 22:764–79
    [Google Scholar]
  25. Deen B, Richardson H, Dilks DD, Takahashi A, Keil B et al. 2017. Organization of high-level visual cortex in human infants. Nat. Commun. 8:13995
    [Google Scholar]
  26. Dehaene-Lambertz G. 2017. The human infant brain: a neural architecture able to learn language. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 24:48–55
    [Google Scholar]
  27. Dehaene-Lambertz G, Spelke E. 2015. The infancy of the human brain. Neuron 88:93–109
    [Google Scholar]
  28. Della Longa L, Gliga T, Farroni T 2019. Tune to touch: Affective touch enhances learning of face identity in 4-month-old infants. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 35:42–46
    [Google Scholar]
  29. Deoni SC, Dean DC, O'Muircheartaigh J, Dirks H, Jerskey BA 2012. Investigating white matter development in infancy and early childhood using myelin water faction and relaxation time mapping. NeuroImage 63:1038–53
    [Google Scholar]
  30. Dettmer AM, Kaburu SS, Simpson EA, Paukner A, Sclafani V et al. 2016. Neonatal face-to-face interactions promote later social behaviour in infant rhesus monkeys. Nat. Commun. 7:11940
    [Google Scholar]
  31. Diamond A. 2002. Normal development of prefrontal cortex from birth to young adulthood: cognitive functions, anatomy, and biochemistry. Principles of Frontal Lobe Function D Stuss, R Knight 466–503 New York: Oxford Univ. Press
    [Google Scholar]
  32. DiNicola LM, Braga RM, Buckner RL 2020. Parallel distributed networks dissociate episodic and social functions within the individual. J. Neurophysiol. 123:1144–79
    [Google Scholar]
  33. Dixon ML, De La Vega A, Mills C, Andrews-Hanna J, Spreng RN et al. 2018. Heterogeneity within the frontoparietal control network and its relationship to the default and dorsal attention networks. PNAS 115:E1598–607
    [Google Scholar]
  34. Dubois J, Dehaene-Lambertz G, Kulikova S, Poupon C, Hüppi P, Hertz-Pannier L 2014. The early development of brain white matter: a review of imaging studies in fetuses, newborns and infants. Neuroscience 276:48–71
    [Google Scholar]
  35. Dweck CS. 2017. From needs to goals and representations: foundations for a unified theory of motivation, personality, and development. Psychol. Rev. 124:689–719
    [Google Scholar]
  36. Ecoffet A, Huizinga J, Lehman J, Stanley KO, Clune J 2019. Go-Explore: a new approach for hard-exploration problems. arXiv:1901.10995 [cs.LG]
  37. Emberson LL, Cannon G, Palmeri H, Richards JE, Aslin RN 2017. Using fNIRS to examine occipital and temporal responses to stimulus repetition in young infants: evidence of selective frontal cortex involvement. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 23:26–38
    [Google Scholar]
  38. Emberson LL, Richards JE, Aslin RN 2015. Top-down modulation in the infant brain: Learning-induced expectations rapidly affect the sensory cortex at 6 months. PNAS 112:9585–90
    [Google Scholar]
  39. Esposito R, Cieri F, Chiacchiaretta P, Cera N, Lauriola M et al. 2017. Modifications in resting state functional anticorrelation between default mode network and dorsal attention network: comparison among young adults, healthy elders and mild cognitive impairment patients. Brain Imaging Behav 12:127–41
    [Google Scholar]
  40. Eyre M, Fitzgibbon SP, Ciarrusta J, Cordero-Grande L, Price AN et al. 2020. The Developing Human Connectome Project: typical and disrupted perinatal functional connectivity. bioRxiv 912881. https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.01.20.912881
    [Crossref]
  41. Falk S, Kello CT. 2017. Hierarchical organization in the temporal structure of infant-direct speech and song. Cognition 163:80–86
    [Google Scholar]
  42. Farroni T, Csibra G, Simion F, Johnson M 2002. Eye contact detection in humans from birth. PNAS 99:9602–5
    [Google Scholar]
  43. Farroni T, Johnson M, Menon E, Zulian L, Faraguna D, Csibra G 2005. Newborns’ preference for face-relevant stimuli: effects of contrast polarity. PNAS 102:17245–50
    [Google Scholar]
  44. Ferradal SL, Gagoski B, Jaimes C, Yi F, Carruthers C et al. 2018. System-specific patterns of thalamocortical connectivity in early brain development as revealed by structural and functional MRI. Cereb. Cortex 29:1218–29
    [Google Scholar]
  45. Fletcher P, Anderson J, Shanks D, Honey R, Carpenter T et al. 2001. Responses of human frontal cortex to surprising events are predicted by formal associative learning theory. Nat. Neurosci. 4:1043–48
    [Google Scholar]
  46. Fló A, Brusini P, Macagno F, Nespor M, Mehler J, Ferry AL 2019. Newborns are sensitive to multiple cues for word segmentation in continuous speech. Dev. Sci. 22:e12802
    [Google Scholar]
  47. Foerde K, Shohamy D. 2011. The role of the basal ganglia in learning and memory: insight from Parkinson's disease. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 96:624–36
    [Google Scholar]
  48. Frank MC, Vul E, Saxe R 2011. Measuring the development of social attention using free-viewing. Infancy 17:355–75
    [Google Scholar]
  49. Fransson P, Åden U, Blennow M, Lagercrantz H 2010. The functional architecture of the infant brain as revealed by resting-state fMRI. Cereb. Cortex 21:145–54
    [Google Scholar]
  50. Frith U, Frith C. 2010. The social brain: allowing humans to boldly go where no other species has been. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B 365:165–76
    [Google Scholar]
  51. Fu X, Pérez-Edgar K. 2019. Threat-related attention bias in socioemotional development: a critical review and methodological considerations. Dev. Rev. 51:31–57
    [Google Scholar]
  52. Gabrieli JD, Corkin S, Mickel SF, Growdon JH 1993. Intact acquisition and long-term retention of mirror-tracing skill in Alzheimer's disease and in global amnesia. Behav. Neurosci. 107:899–910
    [Google Scholar]
  53. Gamond L, Cattaneo Z. 2016. The dorsomedial prefrontal cortex plays a causal role in mediating in-group advantage in emotion recognition: a TMS study. Neuropsychologia 93:312–17
    [Google Scholar]
  54. Gao W, Alcauter S, Elton A, Hernandez-Castillo CR, Smith JK et al. 2014. Functional network development during the first year: relative sequence and socioeconomic correlations. Cereb. Cortex 25:2919–28
    [Google Scholar]
  55. Gogtay N, Giedd J, Lusk L, Hayashi K, Greenstein D et al. 2004. Dynamic mapping of human cortical development during childhood through early adulthood. PNAS 101:8174–79
    [Google Scholar]
  56. Gopnik A, Griffiths TL, Lucas CG 2015. When younger learners can be better (or at least more open-minded) than older ones. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 24:87–92
    [Google Scholar]
  57. Gottlieb G. 1991. Experiential canalization of behavioral development: theory. Dev. Psychol. 27:4–13
    [Google Scholar]
  58. Haber N, Mrowca D, Wang S, Fei-Fei LF, Yamins DL 2018. Learning to play with intrinsically-motivated, self-aware agents. Advances in Neural Information Processing Systems 31 S Bengio, H Wallach, H Larochelle, K Grauman, N Cesa-Bianchi, R Garnett 8388–99 Red Hook, NY: Curran Assoc.
    [Google Scholar]
  59. Hacohen G, Weinshall D. 2019. On the power of curriculum learning in training deep networks. arXiv:1904.03626 [cs.LG]
  60. Hamlin J, Wynn K, Bloom P, Mahajan N 2011. How infants and toddlers react to antisocial others. PNAS 108:19931–36
    [Google Scholar]
  61. Hasegawa M, Houdou S, Mito T, Takashima S, Asanuma K, Ohno T 1992. Development of myelination in the human fetal and infant cerebrum: a myelin basic protein immunohistochemical study. Brain Dev 14:1–6
    [Google Scholar]
  62. Hasson U, Frith CD. 2016. Mirroring and beyond: coupled dynamics as a generalized framework for modelling social interactions. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B 371:20150366
    [Google Scholar]
  63. Helfrich RF, Huang M, Wilson G, Knight RT 2017. Prefrontal cortex modulates posterior alpha oscillations during top-down guided visual perception. PNAS 114:9457–62
    [Google Scholar]
  64. Henry JD, von Hippel W, Molenberghs P, Lee T, Sachdev PS 2015. Clinical assessment of social cognitive function in neurological disorders. Nat. Rev. Neurol. 12:28–39
    [Google Scholar]
  65. Hinton G. 2006. Reducing the dimensionality of data with neural networks. Science 313:504–7
    [Google Scholar]
  66. Hunter MA, Ames EW. 1988. A multifactor model of infant preferences for novel and familiar stimuli. Adv. Infancy Res. 5:69–95
    [Google Scholar]
  67. Huttenlocher PR, Dabholkar AS. 1997. Regional differences in synaptogenesis in human cerebral cortex. J. Comp. Neurol. 387:167–78
    [Google Scholar]
  68. Hwang K, Bertolero MA, Liu WB, D'Esposito M 2017. The human thalamus is an integrative hub for functional brain networks. J. Neurosci. 37:5594–607
    [Google Scholar]
  69. Imafuku M, Hakuno Y, Uchida-Ota M, Yamamoto JI, Minagawa Y 2014. “Mom called me!” Behavioral and prefrontal responses of infants to self-names spoken by their mothers. NeuroImage 103:476–84
    [Google Scholar]
  70. Jakab A, Natalucci G, Koller B, Tuura R, Ruegger C, Hagmann C 2020. Mental development is associated with cortical connectivity of the ventral and nonspecific thalamus of preterm newborns. bioRxiv 078196. https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.05.05.078196
    [Crossref]
  71. Jayaraman S, Fausey CM, Smith LB 2017. Why are faces denser in the visual experiences of younger than older infants. ? Dev. Psychol. 53:38–49
    [Google Scholar]
  72. Jenkins LM, Andrewes DG, Nicholas CL, Drummond KJ, Moffat BA et al. 2014. Social cognition in patients following surgery to the prefrontal cortex. Psychiatry Res. Neuroimaging 224:192–203
    [Google Scholar]
  73. Jernigan TL, Gamst AC. 2005. Changes in volume with age-consistency and interpretation of observed effects. Neurobiol. Aging 26:1271–74
    [Google Scholar]
  74. Johnson MH, Dziurawiec S, Ellis H, Morton J 1991. Newborns’ preferential tracking of face-like stimuli and its subsequent decline. Cognition 40:1–19
    [Google Scholar]
  75. Johnson MH, Senju A, Tomalski P 2015. The two-process theory of face processing: modifications based on two decades of data from infants and adults. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 50:169–79
    [Google Scholar]
  76. Kalashnikova M, Carignan C, Burnham D 2017. The origins of babytalk: smiling, teaching or social convergence. ? R. Soc. Open Sci. 4:170306
    [Google Scholar]
  77. Kam JW, Solbakk AK, Endestad T, Meling TR, Knight RT 2018. Lateral prefrontal cortex lesion impairs regulation of internally and externally directed attention. NeuroImage 175:91–99
    [Google Scholar]
  78. Kanjlia S, Bedny M. 2018. Regional specialization of visual cortex in congenital blindness reveals takeover by multiple distinct top-down fronto-parietal inputs. J. Vis. 18:31
    [Google Scholar]
  79. Kanjlia S, Pant R, Bedny M 2018. Sensitive period for cognitive repurposing of human visual cortex. Cereb. Cortex 29:3993–4005
    [Google Scholar]
  80. Kanske P, Böckler A, Trautwein FM, Singer T 2015. Dissecting the social brain: introducing the EmpaToM to reveal distinct neural networks and brain–behavior relations for empathy and theory of mind. NeuroImage 122:6–19
    [Google Scholar]
  81. Kennedy DP, Adolphs R. 2012. The social brain in psychiatric and neurological disorders. Trends Cogn. Sci. 16:559–72
    [Google Scholar]
  82. Keunen K, Counsell SJ, Benders MJ 2017. The emergence of functional architecture during early brain development. NeuroImage 160:2–14
    [Google Scholar]
  83. Kidd C, Piantadosi ST, Aslin RN 2012. The Goldilocks effect: Human infants allocate attention to visual sequences that are neither too simple nor too complex. PLOS ONE 7:e36399
    [Google Scholar]
  84. Kidd C, Piantadosi ST, Aslin RN 2014. The Goldilocks effect in infant auditory attention. Child Dev 85:1795–804
    [Google Scholar]
  85. Kostović I, Judaš M. 2010. The development of the subplate and thalamocortical connections in the human foetal brain. Acta Paediatr 99:1119–27
    [Google Scholar]
  86. Kouider S, Stahlhut C, Gelskov S, Barbosa L, Dutat M et al. 2013. A neural marker of perceptual consciousness in infants. Science 340:376–80
    [Google Scholar]
  87. Libertus K, Landa RJ, Haworth JL 2017. Development of attention to faces during the first 3 years: influences of stimulus type. Front. Psychol. 8:1976
    [Google Scholar]
  88. Lieberman MD, Straccia MA, Meyer ML, Du M, Tan KM 2019. Social, self, (situational), and affective processes in medial prefrontal cortex (MPFC): causal, multivariate, and reverse inference evidence. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 99:311–28
    [Google Scholar]
  89. Livingstone MS, Arcaro MJ, Schade PF 2019. Cortex is cortex: Ubiquitous principles drive face-domain development. Trends Cogn. Sci. 23:3–4
    [Google Scholar]
  90. Loonis RF, Brincat SL, Antzoulatos EG, Miller EK 2017. A meta-analysis suggests different neural correlates for implicit and explicit learning. Neuron 96:521–34.e7
    [Google Scholar]
  91. ManyBabies Consort 2020. Quantifying sources of variability in infancy research using the infant-directed-speech preference. Adv. Methods Pract. Psychol. Sci. 3:24–52
    [Google Scholar]
  92. Martin J, Rychlowska M, Wood A, Niedenthal P 2017. Smiles as multipurpose social signals. Trends Cogn. Sci. 21:864–77
    [Google Scholar]
  93. Martinelli P, Sperduti M, Piolino P 2012. Neural substrates of the self-memory system: new insights from a meta-analysis. Hum. Brain Mapp. 34:1515–29
    [Google Scholar]
  94. McDonald NM, Perdue KL. 2018. The infant brain in the social world: moving toward interactive social neuroscience with functional near-infrared spectroscopy. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 87:38–49
    [Google Scholar]
  95. Mesman J, van IJzendoorn MH, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ 2009. The many faces of the still-face paradigm: a review and meta-analysis. Dev. Rev. 29:120–62
    [Google Scholar]
  96. Meyer ML, Davachi L, Ochsner KN, Lieberman MD 2018. Evidence that default network connectivity during rest consolidates social information. Cereb. Cortex 29:1910–20
    [Google Scholar]
  97. Miller EK, Cohen JD. 2001. An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 24:167–202
    [Google Scholar]
  98. Mitra A, Snyder AZ, Tagliazucchi E, Laufs H, Elison J et al. 2017. Resting-state fMRI in sleeping infants more closely resembles adult sleep than adult wakefulness. PLOS ONE 12:e0188122
    [Google Scholar]
  99. Monroy CD, Meyer M, Schröer L, Gerson SA, Hunnius S 2019. The infant motor system predicts actions based on visual statistical learning. NeuroImage 185:947–54
    [Google Scholar]
  100. Murray L, De Pascalis L, Bozicevic L, Hawkins L, Sclafani V, Ferrari PF 2016. The functional architecture of mother-infant communication, and the development of infant social expressiveness in the first two months. Sci. Rep. 6:39019
    [Google Scholar]
  101. Nagy E. 2008. Innate intersubjectivity: newborns’ sensitivity to communication disturbance. Dev. Psychol. 44:1779–84
    [Google Scholar]
  102. Nakano T, Homae F, Watanabe H, Taga G 2008. Anticipatory cortical activation precedes auditory events in sleeping infants. PLOS ONE 3:e3912
    [Google Scholar]
  103. Nakano T, Watanabe H, Homae F, Taga G 2009. Prefrontal cortical involvement in young infants’ analysis of novelty. Cereb. Cortex 19:455–63
    [Google Scholar]
  104. Nguyen T, Schleihauf H, Kayhan E, Matthes D, Vrtička P, Hoehl S 2020. The effects of interaction quality on neural synchrony during mother-child problem solving. Cortex 124:235–49
    [Google Scholar]
  105. Pant R, Kanjlia S, Bedny M 2020. A sensitive period in the neural phenotype of language in blind individuals. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 41:100744
    [Google Scholar]
  106. Parnaudeau S, Bolkan SS, Kellendonk C 2018. The mediodorsal thalamus: an essential partner of the prefrontal cortex for cognition. Biol. Psychiatry 83:648–56
    [Google Scholar]
  107. Pathak D, Agrawal P, Efros AA, Darrell T 2017. Curiosity-driven exploration by self-supervised prediction. 2017 IEEE Conference on Computer Vision and Pattern Recognition Workshops (CVPRW) K Brkic, I Sikiric, T Hrkac, Z Kalafatic 16–17 Piscataway, NJ: IEEE
    [Google Scholar]
  108. Peltola MJ, Yrttiaho S, Leppänen JM 2018. Infants’ attention bias to faces as an early marker of social development. Dev. Sci. 21:e12687
    [Google Scholar]
  109. Perez J, Feigenson L. 2020. Violations of expectation trigger infants to search for explanations. PsyArXiv https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/eahjd
    [Crossref]
  110. Pezzulo G, Rigoli F, Friston KJ 2018. Hierarchical active inference: a theory of motivated control. Trends Cogn. Sci. 22:294–306
    [Google Scholar]
  111. Philippi CL, Duff MC, Denburg NL, Tranel D, Rudrauf D 2012. Medial PFC damage abolishes the self-reference effect. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 24:475–81
    [Google Scholar]
  112. Piaget J. 1955. The Child's Construction of Reality London: Routledge & Paul
  113. Piazza EA, Hasenfratz L, Hasson U, Lew-Williams C 2019. Infant and adult brains are coupled to the dynamics of natural communication. Psychol. Sci. 31:6–17
    [Google Scholar]
  114. Polich J. 2007. Updating p300: an integrative theory of p3a and p3b. Clin. Neurophysiol. 118:2128–48
    [Google Scholar]
  115. Powell LJ, Spelke ES. 2018. Third-party preferences for imitators in preverbal infants. Open Mind 2:61–71
    [Google Scholar]
  116. Quaresima V, Ferrari M. 2019. Functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) for assessing cerebral cortex function during human behavior in natural/social situations: a concise review. Organ. Res. Methods 22:46–68
    [Google Scholar]
  117. Rahnev D. 2017. Top-down control of perceptual decision making by the prefrontal cortex. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 26:464–69
    [Google Scholar]
  118. Raichle M, MacLeod A, Snyder A, Powers W, Gusnard D, Shulman G 2001. A default mode of brain function. PNAS 98:676–82
    [Google Scholar]
  119. Ratan Murty NA, Teng S, Beeler D, Mynick A, Oliva A, Kanwisher N 2020. Visual experience is not necessary for the development of face-selectivity in the lateral fusiform gyrus. PNAS 117:372301120
    [Google Scholar]
  120. Reber J, Feinstein JS, O'Doherty JP, Liljeholm M, Adolphs R, Tranel D 2017. Selective impairment of goal-directed decision-making following lesions to the human ventromedial prefrontal cortex. Brain 140:1743–56
    [Google Scholar]
  121. Reber PJ. 2013. The neural basis of implicit learning and memory: a review of neuropsychological and neuroimaging research. Neuropsychologia 51:2026–42
    [Google Scholar]
  122. Reynolds GD, Romano AC. 2016. The development of attention systems and working memory in infancy. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 10:15
    [Google Scholar]
  123. Richardson H, Lisandrelli G, Riobueno-Naylor A, Saxe R 2018. Development of the social brain from age three to twelve years. Nat. Commun. 9:1027
    [Google Scholar]
  124. Rikhye RV, Gilra A, Halassa MM 2018. Thalamic regulation of switching between cortical representations enables cognitive flexibility. Nat. Neurosci. 21:1753–63
    [Google Scholar]
  125. Roder BJ, Bushneil EW, Sasseville AM 2000. Infants’ preferences for familiarity and novelty during the course of visual processing. Infancy 1:491–507
    [Google Scholar]
  126. Rossi A, Bichot N, Desimone R, Ungerleider L 2007. Top down attentional deficits in macaques with lesions of lateral prefrontal cortex. J. Neurosci. 27:11306–14
    [Google Scholar]
  127. Ruvolo P, Messinger D, Movellan J 2015. Infants time their smiles to make their moms smile. PLOS ONE 10:e0136492
    [Google Scholar]
  128. Saito Y, Aoyama S, Kondo T, Fukumoto R, Konishi N et al. 2007a. Frontal cerebral blood flow change associated with infant-directed speech. Arch. Dis. Child. Fetal Neonatal Ed. 92:F113–16
    [Google Scholar]
  129. Saito Y, Kondo T, Aoyama S, Fukumoto R, Konishi N et al. 2007b. The function of the frontal lobe in neonates for response to a prosodic voice. Early Hum. Dev. 83:225–30
    [Google Scholar]
  130. Sampaio RC, Truwit CL. 2001. Myelination in the developing human brain. Handbook of Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience CA Nelson, M Luciana 35–44 Cambridge, MA: MIT Press
    [Google Scholar]
  131. Saxe R, Kanwisher N. 2003. People thinking about thinking people: the role of the temporo-parietal junction in “theory of mind.”. NeuroImage 19:1835–42
    [Google Scholar]
  132. Schachner A, Hannon EE. 2011. Infant-directed speech drives social preferences in 5-month-old infants. Dev. Psychol. 47:19–25
    [Google Scholar]
  133. Schilbach L, Timmermans B, Reddy V, Costall A, Bente G et al. 2013. Toward a second-person neuroscience. Behav. Brain Sci. 36:393–414
    [Google Scholar]
  134. Shackman AJ, McMenamin BW, Maxwell JS, Greischar LL, Davidson RJ 2009. Right dorsolateral prefrontal cortical activity and behavioral inhibition. Psychol. Sci. 20:1500–6
    [Google Scholar]
  135. Shannon C. 1948. A mathematical theory of communication. Bell Syst. Tech. J. 27:379–423
    [Google Scholar]
  136. Sherman BE, Graves KN, Turk-Browne NB 2020. The prevalence and importance of statistical learning in human cognition and behavior. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 32:15–20
    [Google Scholar]
  137. Sim ZL, Xu F. 2018. Another look at looking time: surprise as rational statistical inference. Top. Cogn. Sci. 11:154–63
    [Google Scholar]
  138. Sisk CA, Remington RW, Jiang YV 2019. Mechanisms of contextual cueing: a tutorial review. Atten. Percept. Psychophys. 81:2571–89
    [Google Scholar]
  139. Skerry AE, Saxe R. 2015. Neural representations of emotion are organized around abstract event features. Curr. Biol. 25:1945–54
    [Google Scholar]
  140. Smith LB, Jayaraman S, Clerkin E, Yu C 2018. The developing infant creates a curriculum for statistical learning. Trends Cogn. Sci. 22:325–36
    [Google Scholar]
  141. Spelke ES, Kinzler KD. 2007. Core knowledge. Dev. Sci. 10:89–96
    [Google Scholar]
  142. Stahl AE, Feigenson L. 2015. Observing the unexpected enhances infants’ learning and exploration. Science 348:91–94
    [Google Scholar]
  143. Stahl AE, Feigenson L. 2018. Violations of core knowledge shape early learning. Top. Cogn. Sci. 11:136–53
    [Google Scholar]
  144. Stevner A, Vidaurre D, Cabral J, Rapuano K, Nielsen S et al. 2019. Discovery of key whole-brain transitions and dynamics during human wakefulness and non-REM sleep. Nat. Commun. 10:1035
    [Google Scholar]
  145. Tamir DI, Thornton MA, Contreras JM, Mitchell JP 2015. Neural evidence that three dimensions organize mental state representation: rationality, social impact, and valence. PNAS 113:194–99
    [Google Scholar]
  146. Thompson-Schill SL, Ramscar M, Chrysikou EG 2009. Cognition without control. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 18:259–63
    [Google Scholar]
  147. Tomasello M. 2020. The adaptive origins of uniquely human sociality. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B 375:20190493
    [Google Scholar]
  148. Toulmin H, Beckmann CF, O'Muircheartaigh J, Ball G, Nongena P et al. 2015. Specialization and integration of functional thalamocortical connectivity in the human infant. PNAS 112:6485–90
    [Google Scholar]
  149. Turk E, van den Heuvel MI, Benders MJ, de Heus R, Franx A et al. 2019. Functional connectome of the fetal brain. J. Neurosci. 39:9716–24
    [Google Scholar]
  150. Urakawa S, Takamoto K, Ishikawa A, Ono T, Nishijo H 2014. Selective medial prefrontal cortex responses during live mutual gaze interactions in human infants: an fNIRS study. Brain Topogr 28:691–701
    [Google Scholar]
  151. van den Hurk J, Van Baelen M, Op de Beeck HP 2017. Development of visual category selectivity in ventral visual cortex does not require visual experience. PNAS 114:E4501–10
    [Google Scholar]
  152. Vossel S, Geng JJ, Fink GR 2013. Dorsal and ventral attention systems. Neuroscientist 20:150–59
    [Google Scholar]
  153. Watters N, Matthey L, Bosnjak M, Burgess CP, Lerchner A 2019. COBRA: data-efficient model-based RL through unsupervised object discovery and curiosity-driven exploration. arXiv:1905.09275 [cs.LG]
  154. Werchan DM, Amso D. 2017. A novel ecological account of prefrontal cortex functional development. Psychol. Rev. 124:720–39
    [Google Scholar]
  155. Werchan DM, Amso D. 2020. Top-down knowledge rapidly acquired through abstract rule learning biases subsequent visual attention in 9-month-old infants. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 42:100761
    [Google Scholar]
  156. Werchan DM, Collins AG, Frank MJ, Amso D 2015. 8-month-old infants spontaneously learn and generalize hierarchical rules. Psychol. Sci. 26:805–15
    [Google Scholar]
  157. Werchan DM, Collins AG, Frank MJ, Amso D 2016. Role of prefrontal cortex in learning and generalizing hierarchical rules in 8-month-old infants. J. Neurosci. 36:10314–22
    [Google Scholar]
  158. Wimmer RD, Schmitt LI, Davidson TJ, Nakajima M, Deisseroth K, Halassa MM 2015. Thalamic control of sensory selection in divided attention. Nature 526:705–9
    [Google Scholar]
/content/journals/10.1146/annurev-devpsych-121318-084841
Loading
/content/journals/10.1146/annurev-devpsych-121318-084841
Loading

Data & Media loading...

  • Article Type: Review Article
This is a required field
Please enter a valid email address
Approval was a Success
Invalid data
An Error Occurred
Approval was partially successful, following selected items could not be processed due to error