1932

Abstract

Misinformation occurs when people hold incorrect factual beliefs and do so confidently. The problem, first conceptualized by Kuklinski and colleagues in 2000, plagues political systems and is exceedingly difficult to correct. In this review, we assess the empirical literature on political misinformation in the United States and consider what scholars have learned since the publication of that early study. We conclude that research on this topic has developed unevenly. Over time, scholars have elaborated on the psychological origins of political misinformation, and this work has cumulated in a productive way. By contrast, although there is an extensive body of research on how to correct misinformation, this literature is less coherent in its recommendations. Finally, a nascent line of research asks whether people's reports of their factual beliefs are genuine or are instead a form of partisan cheerleading. Overall, scholarly research on political misinformation illustrates the many challenges inherent in representative democracy.

Loading

Article metrics loading...

/content/journals/10.1146/annurev-polisci-050718-032814
2020-05-11
2024-03-29
Loading full text...

Full text loading...

/deliver/fulltext/polisci/23/1/annurev-polisci-050718-032814.html?itemId=/content/journals/10.1146/annurev-polisci-050718-032814&mimeType=html&fmt=ahah

Literature Cited

  1. Ahler DJ, Sood G. 2018. The parties in our heads: misperceptions about party composition and their consequences. J. Politics 80:964–81
    [Google Scholar]
  2. Althaus SL. 2003. Collective Preferences in Democratic Politics: Opinion Surveys and the Will of the People Cambridge, UK: Cambridge Univ. Press
  3. Arceneaux K, Vander Wielen RJ 2017. Taming Intuition: How Reflection Minimizes Partisan Reasoning and Promotes Democratic Accountability Cambridge, UK: Cambridge Univ. Press
  4. Benegal SD, Scruggs LA. 2018. Correcting misinformation about climate change: the impact of partisanship in an experimental setting. Climatic Change 148:61–80
    [Google Scholar]
  5. Berinsky AJ. 2017. Rumors and health care reform: experiments in political misinformation. Br. J. Political Sci. 47:241–62
    [Google Scholar]
  6. Berinsky AJ. 2018. Telling the truth about believing the lies? Evidence for the limited prevalence of expressive survey responding. J. Politics 80:211–24
    [Google Scholar]
  7. Bisgaard M. 2015. Bias will find a way: economic perceptions, attributions of blame, and partisan-motivated reasoning during crisis. J. Politics 77:849–60
    [Google Scholar]
  8. Bode L, Vraga EK. 2015. In related news, that was wrong: the correction of misinformation through related stories functionality in social media. J. Commun. 65:619–38
    [Google Scholar]
  9. Bolsen T, Druckman JN, Cook FL 2014. The influence of partisan motivated reasoning on public opinion. Political Behav 36:235–62
    [Google Scholar]
  10. Brotherton R, French CC, Pickering AD 2013. Measuring belief in conspiracy theories: the generic conspiracist beliefs scale. Front. Psychol. 4:279
    [Google Scholar]
  11. Bullock JG. 2007. Experiments on partisanship and public opinion: party cues, false beliefs, and Bayesian updating PhD Thesis, Stanford University Stanford, CA:
  12. Bullock JG. 2009. Partisan bias and the Bayesian ideal in the study of public opinion. J. Politics 71:1109–24
    [Google Scholar]
  13. Bullock JG. 2011. Elite influence on public opinion in an informed electorate. Am. Political Sci. Rev. 105:496–515
    [Google Scholar]
  14. Bullock JG, Gerber AS, Hill SJ, Huber GA 2015. Partisan bias in factual beliefs about politics. Q. J. Political Sci. 10:519–78
    [Google Scholar]
  15. Bullock JG, Lenz G. 2019. Partisan bias in surveys. Annu. Rev. Political Sci. 22:325–42
    [Google Scholar]
  16. Cacioppo JT, Petty RE. 1982. The need for cognition. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 42:116–31
    [Google Scholar]
  17. Chan MS, Jones CR, Hall Jamieson K, Albarracin D 2017. Debunking: a meta-analysis of the psychological efficacy of messages countering misinformation. Psychol. Sci. 28:1531–46
    [Google Scholar]
  18. Clifford S, Thorson E. 2017. Encouraging information search reduces factual misperceptions Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the Midwest Political Science Association Chicago, IL:
  19. Cobb M, Nyhan B, Reifler J 2013. Beliefs don't always preserve: how political figures are punished when positive information about them is discredited. Political Psychol 34:307–26
    [Google Scholar]
  20. Cook J, Ecker U, Lewandowsky S 2015. Misinformation and how to correct it. Emerging Trends in the Social and Behavioral Sciences RA Scott, MC Buchmann, SM Kosslyn New York: Wiley https://doi.org/10.1002/9781118900772.etrds0222
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  21. Delli Carpini MX, Keeter S 1993. Measuring political knowledge: putting first things first. Am. J. Political Sci. 37:1179–206
    [Google Scholar]
  22. Delli Carpini MX, Keeter S 1996. What Americans Know About Politics and Why It Matters New Haven, CT: Yale Univ. Press
  23. Ditto PH, Lopez DF. 1992. Motivated skepticism: use of differential decision criteria for preferred and nonpreferred conclusions. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 63:568–84
    [Google Scholar]
  24. Druckman JN 2012. The politics of motivation. Crit. Rev. J. Politics Soc. 24:199–216
    [Google Scholar]
  25. Druckman JN, McGrath MC. 2019. The evidence for motivated reasoning in climate change preference formation. Nat. Climate Change 9:111–19
    [Google Scholar]
  26. Druckman JN, Peterson E, Slothuus R 2013. How elite partisan polarization affects public opinion formation. Am. Political Sci. Rev. 107:57–79
    [Google Scholar]
  27. Duran ND, Nicholson SP, Dale R 2017. The hidden appeal and aversion to political conspiracies as revealed in the response dynamics of partisans. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 73:268–78
    [Google Scholar]
  28. Ecker UK, Ang LC. 2019. Political attitudes and the processing of misinformation corrections. Political Psychol 40:241–60
    [Google Scholar]
  29. Ecker UK, Lewandowsky S, Apai J 2011. Terrorists brought down the plane!—No, actually it was a technical fault: processing corrections of emotive information. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 64:283–310
    [Google Scholar]
  30. Ecker UK, Lewandowsky S, Chang EP, Pillai R 2014a. The effects of subtle misinformation in news headlines. J. Exp. Psychol. Appl. 20:323–35
    [Google Scholar]
  31. Ecker UK, Lewandowsky S, Fenton O, Martin K 2014b. Do people keep believing because they want to? Preexisting attitudes and the continued influence of misinformation. Mem. Cogn. 42:292–304
    [Google Scholar]
  32. Ecker UK, Lewandowsky S, Tang DT 2010. Explicit warnings reduce but do not eliminate the continued influence of misinformation. Mem. Cogn. 38:1087–100
    [Google Scholar]
  33. Feldman S. 1995. Answering survey questions: the measurement and meaning of public opinion. Political Judgment: Structure and Process M Lodge, KM McGraw 249–70 Ann Arbor: Univ. Mich. Press
    [Google Scholar]
  34. Feldman S, Huddy L, Marcus GE 2015. Going to War in Iraq: When Citizens and the Press Matter Chicago: Univ. Chicago Press
  35. Flynn D. 2016. The scope and correlates of political misperceptions in the mass public Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the American Political Science Association Philadelphia, PA:
  36. Flynn D, Nyhan B, Reifler J 2017. The nature and origins of misperceptions: understanding false and unsupported beliefs about politics. Adv. Political Psychol 38:127–50
    [Google Scholar]
  37. Gaines BJ, Kuklinski JH, Quirk PJ, Peyton B, Verkuilen J 2007. Same facts, different interpretations: partisan motivation and opinion on Iraq. J. Politics 69:957–74
    [Google Scholar]
  38. Gal D, Rucker DD. 2010. When in doubt, shout! Paradoxical influences of doubt on proselytizing. Psychol. Sci. 21:1701–7
    [Google Scholar]
  39. Garrett RK, Nisbet EC, Lynch EK 2013. Undermining the corrective effects of media-based political fact checking? The role of contextual cues and naïve theory. J. Commun. 63:617–37
    [Google Scholar]
  40. Gershkoff A, Kushner S. 2005. Shaping public opinion: the 9/11-Iraq connection in the Bush administration's rhetoric. Perspect. Politics 3:525–37
    [Google Scholar]
  41. Gilens M. 2001. Political ignorance and collective policy preferences. Am. Political Sci. Rev. 95:379–96
    [Google Scholar]
  42. Graham MH. 2018. Self-awareness of political knowledge. Political Behav https://doi.org/10.1007/s11109-018-9499-8
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  43. Guillory JJ, Geraci L. 2013. Correcting erroneous inferences in memory: the role of source credibility. J. Appl. Res. Mem. Cogn. 2:201–9
    [Google Scholar]
  44. Haglin K. 2017. The limitations of the backfire effect. Res. Politics 4:31–5
    [Google Scholar]
  45. Hart PS, Nisbet EC. 2012. Boomerang effects in science communication: how motivated reasoning and identity cues amplify opinion polarization about climate mitigation policies. Commun. Res. 39:701–23
    [Google Scholar]
  46. Hill SJ. 2017. Learning together slowly: Bayesian learning about political facts. J. Politics 79:1403–18
    [Google Scholar]
  47. Hochschild JL. 2001. Where you stand depends on what you see: connections among values, perceptions of fact, and political prescriptions. Citizens and Politics: Perspectives from Political Psychology JH Kuklinski 313–40 Cambridge, UK: Cambridge Univ. Press
    [Google Scholar]
  48. Hochschild JL, Einstein KL. 2015. Do Facts Matter? Information and Misinformation in American Politics Norman: Univ. Okla. Press
  49. Hopkins D, Sides J, Citrin J 2018. The muted consequences of correct information about immigration. J. Politics 81:315–20
    [Google Scholar]
  50. Jerit J, Barabas J. 2006. Bankrupt rhetoric: how misleading information affects knowledge about social security. Public Opin. Q. 70:278–303
    [Google Scholar]
  51. Jerit J, Barabas J. 2012. Partisan perceptual bias and the information environment. J. Politics 74:672–84
    [Google Scholar]
  52. Johnson HM, Seifert CM. 1994. Sources of the continued influence effect: when misinformation in memory affects later inferences. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 20:1420–36
    [Google Scholar]
  53. Jost JT, Glaser J, Kruglanski AW, Sulloway FJ 2003. Political conservatism as motivated social cognition. Psychol. Bull. 129:3339–75
    [Google Scholar]
  54. Kahan DM. 2016. The politically motivated reasoning paradigm, part 1: what politically motivated reasoning is and how to measure it. Emerging Trends in the Social and Behavioral Sciences RA Scott, SM Kosslyn 1–16 New York: Wiley https://doi.org/10.1002/9781118900772.etrds0417
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  55. Kahan DM, Landrum A, Carpenter K, Helft L, Hall Jamieson K 2017a. Science curiosity and political information processing. Political Psychol 38:179–99
    [Google Scholar]
  56. Kahan DM, Peters E, Dawson EC, Slovic P 2017b. Motivated numeracy and enlightened self-government. Behav. Public Policy 1:54–86
    [Google Scholar]
  57. Khanna K, Sood G. 2018. Motivated responding in studies of factual learning. Political Behav 40:79–101
    [Google Scholar]
  58. Kuklinski JH, Quirk PJ, Jerit J, Schwieder D, Rich RF 2000. Misinformation and the currency of democratic citizenship. J. Politics 62:790–816
    [Google Scholar]
  59. Kunda Z. 1990. The case for motivated reasoning. Psychol. Bull. 108:480–98
    [Google Scholar]
  60. Lavine HG, Johnston CD, Steenbergen MR 2012. The Ambivalent Partisan: How Critical Loyalty Promotes Democracy Oxford, UK: Oxford Univ. Press
  61. Lee S, Matsuo A. 2018. Decomposing political knowledge: what is confidence in knowledge and why it matters. Electoral Stud 51:1–13
    [Google Scholar]
  62. Leeper TJ, Slothuus R. 2014. Political parties, motivated reasoning, and public opinion formation. Political Psychol 35:129–56
    [Google Scholar]
  63. Lewandowsky S, Ecker UK, Cook J 2017. Beyond misinformation: understanding and coping with the “post-truth” era. J. Appl. Res. Mem. Cogn. 6:353–69
    [Google Scholar]
  64. Lewandowsky S, Ecker UK, Seifert CM, Schwarz N, Cook J 2012. Misinformation and its correction: continued influence and successful debiasing. Psychol. Sci. Public Interest 13:106–31
    [Google Scholar]
  65. Lewandowsky S, Stritzke WGK, Oberauer K, Morales M 2005. Memory for fact, fiction, and misinformation: the Iraq War 2003. Psychol. Sci. 16:190–95
    [Google Scholar]
  66. Lodge M, Taber CS. 2013. The Rationalizing Voter Cambridge, UK: Cambridge Univ. Press
  67. Maio GR, Esses VM. 2001. The need for affect: individual differences in the motivation to approach or avoid emotions. J. Personal. 69:583–614
    [Google Scholar]
  68. Miller JM, Saunders KL, Farhart CE 2016. Conspiracy endorsement as motivated reasoning: the moderating roles of political knowledge and trust. Am. J. Political Sci. 60:824–44
    [Google Scholar]
  69. Muirhead R, Rosenblum NL. 2019. A Lot of People Are Saying: The New Conspiracism and the Assault on Democracy Princeton, NJ: Princeton Univ. Press
  70. Murphy ST, Zajonc RB. 1993. Affect, cognition, and awareness: affective priming with optimal and suboptimal stimulus exposures. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 64:723–39
    [Google Scholar]
  71. Nir L. 2011. Motivated reasoning and public opinion perception. Public Opin. Q. 75:504–32
    [Google Scholar]
  72. Nisbet EC, Cooper KE, Garrett RK 2015. The partisan brain: how dissonant science messages lead conservatives and liberals to (dis)trust science. Ann. Am. Acad. Political Soc. Sci. 658:36–66
    [Google Scholar]
  73. Nyhan B. 2010. Why the “death panel” myth wouldn't die: misinformation in the health care reform debate. Forum 8:15
    [Google Scholar]
  74. Nyhan B, Porter E, Reifler J, Wood TJ 2019. Taking fact-checks literally but not seriously? The effects of journalistic fact-checking on factual beliefs and candidate favorability. Political Behav https://doi.org/10.1007/s11109-019-09528-x
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  75. Nyhan B, Reifler J. 2010. When corrections fail: the persistence of political misperceptions. Political Behav 32:303–30
    [Google Scholar]
  76. Nyhan B, Reifler J. 2015a. Displacing misinformation about events: an experimental test of causal corrections. J. Exp. Political Sci. 2:81–93
    [Google Scholar]
  77. Nyhan B, Reifler J. 2015b. Does correcting myths about the flu vaccine work? An experimental evaluation of the effects of corrective information. Vaccine 33:459–64
    [Google Scholar]
  78. Oliver JE, Wood TJ. 2014. Conspiracy theories and the paranoid style(s) of mass opinion. Am. J. Political Sci. 58:952–66
    [Google Scholar]
  79. Page BI, Shapiro RY. 1992. The Rational Public: Fifty Years of Trends in Americans’ Policy Preferences Chicago: Univ. Chicago Press
  80. Parker-Stephen E. 2013. Tides of disagreement: how reality facilitates (and inhibits) partisan public opinion. J. Politics 75:1077–88
    [Google Scholar]
  81. Pasek J, Sood G, Krosnick JA 2015. Misinformed about the Affordable Care Act? Leveraging certainty to assess the prevalence of misperceptions. J. Commun. 65:660–73
    [Google Scholar]
  82. Pennycook G, Rand DG. 2019. Cognitive reflection and the 2016 US Presidential election. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 45:224–39
    [Google Scholar]
  83. Peter C, Koch T. 2016. When debunking scientific myths fails (and when it does not): the backfire effect in the context of journalistic coverage and immediate judgments as prevention strategy. Sci. Commun. 38:3–25
    [Google Scholar]
  84. Pornpitakpan C. 2004. The persuasiveness of source credibility: A critical review of five decades’ evidence. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 34:243–81
    [Google Scholar]
  85. Prasad M, Perrin AJ, Bezila K, Hoffman SG, Kindleberger K et al. 2009. “There must be a reason”: Osama, Saddam, and inferred justification. Sociol. Inq. 79:142–62
    [Google Scholar]
  86. Prior M, Sood G, Khanna K 2015. You cannot be serious: the impact of accuracy incentives on partisan bias in reports of economic perceptions. Q. J. Political Sci. 10:489–518
    [Google Scholar]
  87. Redlawsk DP, Civettini AJ, Emmerson KM 2010. The affective tipping point: Do motivated reasoners ever “get it”?. Political Psychol 31:563–93
    [Google Scholar]
  88. Rojecki R, Meraz S. 2016. Rumors and factitious information blends: the role of the web in speculative politics. New Media Soc 18:25–43
    [Google Scholar]
  89. Sangalang A, Ophir Y, Cappella JN 2019. The potential for narrative correctives to combat misinformation. J. Commun. 69:298–319
    [Google Scholar]
  90. Schaffner BF, Luks S. 2018. Misinformation or expressive responding? What an inauguration crowd can tell us about the source of political misinformation in surveys. Public Opin. Q. 82:135–47
    [Google Scholar]
  91. Schaffner BF, Roche C. 2017. Misinformation and motivated reasoning. Public Opin. Q. 81:86–110
    [Google Scholar]
  92. Shapiro RY, Bloch‐Elkon Y. 2008. Do the facts speak for themselves? Partisan disagreement as a challenge to democratic competence. Crit. Rev. 20:115–39
    [Google Scholar]
  93. Shin J, Jian L, Driscoll K, Bar F 2017. Political rumoring on Twitter during the 2012 US presidential election: rumor diffusion and correction. New Media Soc 19:1214–35
    [Google Scholar]
  94. Shin J, Jian L, Driscoll K, Bar F 2018. The diffusion of misinformation on social media: temporal pattern, message, and source. Comput. Hum. Behav. 83:278–87
    [Google Scholar]
  95. Sunstein CR. 2009. On Rumors: How Falsehoods Spread, Why We Believe Them, and What Can Be Done Princeton, NJ: Princeton Univ. Press
  96. Sunstein CR, Vermeule A. 2009. Conspiracy theories: causes and cures. J. Political Philos. 17:202–27
    [Google Scholar]
  97. Swire B, Berinsky AJ, Lewandowsky S, Ecker UK 2017. Processing political misinformation: comprehending the Trump phenomenon. R. Soc. Open Sci. 4:160802
    [Google Scholar]
  98. Swire B, Ecker U. 2018. Misinformation and its correction: cognitive mechanisms and recommendations for mass communication. Misinformation and Mass Audiences BG Southwell, EA Thorson, L Sheble 195–211 Austin: Univ. Texas Press
    [Google Scholar]
  99. Thorson E. 2016. Belief echoes: the persistent effects of corrected misinformation. Political Commun 33:460–80
    [Google Scholar]
  100. Trevors GJ, Muis KR, Pekrun R, Sinatra GM, Winne PH 2016. Identity and epistemic emotions during knowledge revision: a potential account for the backfire effect. Discourse Process 53:339–70
    [Google Scholar]
  101. Vraga EK, Bode L. 2017. Using expert sources to correct health misinformation in social media. Sci. Commun. 39:621–45
    [Google Scholar]
  102. Walter N, Murphy ST. 2018. How to unring the bell: a meta-analytic approach to correction of misinformation. Commun. Monogr. 85:423–41
    [Google Scholar]
  103. Weeks BE. 2015. Emotions, partisanship, and misperceptions: how anger and anxiety moderate the effect of partisan bias on susceptibility to political misinformation. J. Commun. 65:699–719
    [Google Scholar]
  104. Weeks BE. 2018. Media and political misperceptions. Misinformation and Mass Audiences BG Southwell, EA Thorson, L Sheble 140–56 Austin: Univ. Texas Press
    [Google Scholar]
  105. Weeks BE, Garrett RK. 2014. Electoral consequences of political rumors: motivated reasoning, candidate rumors, and vote choice during the 2008 US presidential election. Int. J. Public Opin. Res. 26:401–22
    [Google Scholar]
  106. Wilkes A, Leatherbarrow M. 1988. Editing episodic memory following the identification of error. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 40:361–87
    [Google Scholar]
  107. Wood T, Porter E. 2019. The elusive backfire effect: mass attitudes’ steadfast factual adherence. Political Behav 41:135–63
    [Google Scholar]
  108. Yair O, Huber GA. 2018. How robust is evidence of partisan perceptual bias in survey responses? A new approach for studying expressive responding Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the Midwest Political Science Association Chicago, IL:
  109. Young DG, Jamieson KH, Poulsen S, Goldring A 2018. Fact-checking effectiveness as a function of format and tone: evaluating FactCheck.org and FlackCheck.org. J. Mass Commun. Q. 95:49–75
    [Google Scholar]
  110. Zaller JR. 1992. The Nature and Origins of Mass Opinion Cambridge, UK: Cambridge Univ. Press
  111. Zhou J. 2016. Boomerangs versus javelins: how polarization constrains communication on climate change. Environ. Politics 25:788–811
    [Google Scholar]
/content/journals/10.1146/annurev-polisci-050718-032814
Loading
  • Article Type: Review Article
This is a required field
Please enter a valid email address
Approval was a Success
Invalid data
An Error Occurred
Approval was partially successful, following selected items could not be processed due to error