1932

Abstract

Implicit in the long-standing disagreements about whether humans’ fundamental nature is predominantly caring or callous is an assumption of uniformity. This article reviews evidence that instead supports inherent variation in caring motivation and behavior. The continuum between prosocial and antisocial extremes reflects variation in the structure and function of neurohormonal systems originally adapted to motivate parental care and since repurposed to support generalized forms of care. Extreme social behaviors such as extraordinary acts of altruism and aggression can often be best understood as reflecting variation in the neural systems that support care. A review of comparative, developmental, and neurobiological research finds consistent evidence that variations in caring motivations and behavior reflect individual differences in sensitivity to cues that signal vulnerability and distress and in the tendency to generalize care outward from socially close to distant others. The often complex relationships between caring motivation and various forms of altruism and aggression are discussed.

Loading

Article metrics loading...

/content/journals/10.1146/annurev-psych-010418-103010
2019-01-04
2024-03-28
Loading full text...

Full text loading...

/deliver/fulltext/psych/70/1/annurev-psych-010418-103010.html?itemId=/content/journals/10.1146/annurev-psych-010418-103010&mimeType=html&fmt=ahah

Literature Cited

  1. Akther S, Korshnova N, Zhong J, Liang M, Cherepanov SM et al. 2013. CD38 in the nucleus accumbens and oxytocin are related to paternal behavior in mice. Mol. Brain 6:41
    [Google Scholar]
  2. Anderson CA, Bushman BJ 2002. Human aggression. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 53:27–51
    [Google Scholar]
  3. Aquinas T 2007. Summa Theologica: Volume III, Part II, Second Section transl. Fathers Engl. Dominic. Prov New York: Cosimo Class
  4. Atzil S, Touroutoglou A, Rudy T, Salcedo S, Feldman R et al. 2017. Dopamine in the medial amygdala network mediates human bonding. PNAS 114:2361–66
    [Google Scholar]
  5. Aube W, Angulo-Perkins A, Peretz I, Concha L, Armony JL 2015. Fear across the senses: brain responses to music, vocalizations and facial expressions. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 10:399–407
    [Google Scholar]
  6. Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, van Ijzendoorn MH 2008. Oxytocin receptor (OXTR) and serotonin transporter (5-HTT) genes associated with observed parenting. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 3:128–34
    [Google Scholar]
  7. Bandstra NF, Chambers CT, McGrath PJ, Moore C 2011. The behavioural expression of empathy to others’ pain versus others’ sadness in young children. Pain 152:1074–82
    [Google Scholar]
  8. Bani M, Giussani B 2010. Gender differences in giving blood: a review of the literature. Blood Transfus 8:278–87
    [Google Scholar]
  9. Barrett J, Wonch KE, Gonzalez A, Ali N, Steiner M et al. 2012. Maternal affect and quality of parenting experiences are related to amygdala response to infant faces. Soc. Neurosci. 7:252–68
    [Google Scholar]
  10. Batson CD 2010. The naked emperor: seeking a more plausible genetic basis for psychological altruism. Econ. Philos. 26:149–64
    [Google Scholar]
  11. Baumeister RF, Bratslavsky E, Finkenauer C, Vohs KD 2001. Bad is stronger than good. Rev. Gen. Psychol. 5:323–70
    [Google Scholar]
  12. Ben-Ami Bartal I, Rodgers DA, Bernardez Sarria MS, Decety J, Mason P 2014. Pro-social behavior in rats is modulated by social experience. eLife 3:e01385
    [Google Scholar]
  13. Ben-Ami Bartal I, Shan H, Molasky NMR, Murray TM, Williams JZ 2016. Anxiolytic treatment impairs helping behavior in rats. Front. Psychol. 7:850
    [Google Scholar]
  14. Blair RJ 2010. Psychopathy, frustration, and reactive aggression: the role of ventromedial prefrontal cortex. Br. J. Psychol. 101:383–99
    [Google Scholar]
  15. Blair RJ, Peschardt KS, Budhani S, Mitchell DG, Pine DS 2006. The development of psychopathy. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 47:262–76
    [Google Scholar]
  16. Bogin B, Bragg J, Kuzawa C 2014. Humans are not cooperative breeders but practice biocultural reproduction. Ann. Hum. Biol. 41:368–80
    [Google Scholar]
  17. Bornstein MH, Putnick DL, Rigo P, Esposito G, Swain JE et al. 2017. Neurobiology of culturally common maternal responses to infant cry. PNAS 114:E9465–73
    [Google Scholar]
  18. Bosch OJ 2011. Maternal nurturing is dependent on her innate anxiety: the behavioral roles of brain oxytocin and vasopressin. Horm. Behav. 59:202–12
    [Google Scholar]
  19. Bosch OJ 2013. Maternal aggression in rodents: brain oxytocin and vasopressin mediate pup defence. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B 368:20130085
    [Google Scholar]
  20. Bosch OJ, Meddle SL, Beiderbeck DI, Douglas AJ, Neumann ID 2005. Brain oxytocin correlates with maternal aggression: link to anxiety. J. Neurosci. 25:6807–15
    [Google Scholar]
  21. Brethel-Haurwitz KM, Marsh AA 2014. Geographical differences in subjective well-being predict extraordinary altruism. Psychol. Sci. 25:762–71
    [Google Scholar]
  22. Brethel-Haurwitz KM, O'Connell K, Cardinale EM, Stoianova M, Stoycos SA et al. 2017. Amygdala-midbrain connectivity indicates a role for the mammalian parental care system in human altruism. Proc. Biol. Sci. 284:1865
    [Google Scholar]
  23. Brethel-Haurwitz KM, Stoycos SA, Cardinale EM, Huebner B, Marsh AA 2016. Is costly punishment altruistic? Exploring rejection of unfair offers in the Ultimatum Game in real-world altruists. Sci. Rep. 6:18974
    [Google Scholar]
  24. Buckholtz JW, Treadway MT, Cowan RL, Woodward ND, Benning SD et al. 2010. Mesolimbic dopamine reward system hypersensitivity in individuals with psychopathic traits. Nat. Neurosci. 13:419–21
    [Google Scholar]
  25. Burkart JM, Allon O, Amici F, Fichtel C, Finkenwirth C et al. 2014. The evolutionary origin of human hyper-cooperation. Nat. Commun. 5:4747First experimental evidence linking the prevalence of alloparenting to adult altruistic behavior across primate species.
    [Google Scholar]
  26. Burkett JP, Andari E, Johnson ZV, Curry DC, de Waal FB et al. 2016. Oxytocin-dependent consolation behavior in rodents. Science 351:375–78
    [Google Scholar]
  27. Calcagnoli F, Kreutzmann JC, de Boer SF, Althaus M, Koolhaas JM 2015. Acute and repeated intranasal oxytocin administration exerts anti-aggressive and pro-affiliative effects in male rats. Psychoneuroendocrinology 51:112–21
    [Google Scholar]
  28. Carré JM, Hyde LW, Neumann CS, Viding E, Hariri AR 2013. The neural signatures of distinct psychopathic traits. Soc. Neurosci. 8:122–35
    [Google Scholar]
  29. Carter CS, Pournajafi-Nazarloo H, Kramer KM, Ziegle TE, White-Traut R et al. 2007. Oxytocin: behavioral associations and potential as a salivary biomarker. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1098:312–22
    [Google Scholar]
  30. Caspi A, Houts RM, Belsky DW, Harrington H, Hogan S et al. 2017. Childhood forecasting of a small segment of the population with large economic burden. Nat. Hum. Behav. 1:0005
    [Google Scholar]
  31. Cecil CA, Lysenko LJ, Jaffee SR, Pingault JB, Smith RG et al. 2014. Environmental risk, Oxytocin Receptor Gene (OXTR) methylation and youth callous-unemotional traits: a 13-year longitudinal study. Mol. Psychiatry 19:1071–77
    [Google Scholar]
  32. Champagne F, Diorio J, Sharma S, Meaney MJ 2001. Naturally occurring variations in maternal behavior in the rat are associated with differences in estrogen-inducible central oxytocin receptors. PNAS 98:12736–41
    [Google Scholar]
  33. Charities Aid Found 2017. CAF World Giving Index 2017 Rep., Charities Aid Found Kent, UK: https://www.cafonline.org/about-us/publications/2017-publications/caf-world-giving-index-2017
  34. Chini B, Verhage M, Grinevich V 2017. The action radius of oxytocin release in the mammalian CNS: from single vesicles to behavior. Trends Pharmacol. Sci. 38:982–91
    [Google Scholar]
  35. Churchland PS, Winkielman P 2012. Modulating social behavior with oxytocin: How does it work? What does it mean?. Horm. Behav. 61:392–99
    [Google Scholar]
  36. Cikara M, Bruneau EG, Saxe RR 2011. Us and them: intergroup failures of empathy. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 20:149–53
    [Google Scholar]
  37. Cong X, Ludington-Hoe SM, Hussain N, Cusso RM, Walsh S et al. 2015. Parental oxytocin responses during skin-to-skin contact in pre-term infants. Early Hum. Dev. 91:401–6
    [Google Scholar]
  38. Dadds MR, Moul C, Cauchi A, Dobson-Stone C, Hawes DJ et al. 2014. Methylation of the oxytocin receptor gene and oxytocin blood levels in the development of psychopathy. Dev. Psychopathol. 26:33–40
    [Google Scholar]
  39. Dawel A, O'Kearney R, McKone E, Palermo R 2012. Not just fear and sadness: meta-analytic evidence of pervasive emotion recognition deficits for facial and vocal expressions in psychopathy. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 36:2288–304
    [Google Scholar]
  40. de Gelder B, Snyder J, Greve D, Gerard G, Hadjikhani N 2004. Fear fosters flight: a mechanism for fear contagion when perceiving emotion expressed by a whole body. PNAS 101:16701–6
    [Google Scholar]
  41. de Waal FB 2009. Putting the altruism back into altruism: the evolution of empathy. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 59:279–300
    [Google Scholar]
  42. DellaVigna S, List JA, Malmendier JA, Rao G 2013. The importance of being marginal: gender differences in generosity. Am. Econ. Rev. 103:586–90
    [Google Scholar]
  43. Edens JF, Marcus DK, Lilienfeld SO, Poythress NGJ 2006. Psychopathic, not psychopath: taxometric evidence for the dimensional structure of psychopathy. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 115:131–44
    [Google Scholar]
  44. Eisenberg N, Lennon R 1983. Sex differences in empathy and related capacities. Psychol. Bull. 94:100–31
    [Google Scholar]
  45. Eisner M 2003. Long-term historical trends in violent crime. Crime Justice 30:83–142
    [Google Scholar]
  46. Falk O, Wallinius M, Lundström S, Frisell T, Anckarsäter H et al. 2014. The 1% of the population accountable for 63% of all violent crime convictions. Soc. Psychiatry Psychiatr. Epidemiol 49:559–71
    [Google Scholar]
  47. Farthing GW 2005. Attitudes toward heroic and nonheroic physical risk takers as mates and as friends. Evol. Hum. Behav 26:171–85
    [Google Scholar]
  48. Feldman R 2015. The adaptive human parental brain: implications for children's social development. Trends Neurosci 38:387–99
    [Google Scholar]
  49. Feldman R, Gordon I, Schneiderman I, Weisman O, Zagoory-Sharon O 2010. Natural variations in maternal and paternal care are associated with systematic changes in oxytocin following parent-infant contact. Psychoneuroendocrinology 35:1133–41
    [Google Scholar]
  50. Feldman R, Monakhov M, Pratt M, Ebstein RP 2016. Oxytocin pathway genes: evolutionary ancient system impacting on human affiliation, sociality, and psychopathology. Biol. Psychiatry 79:174–84
    [Google Scholar]
  51. Fusar-Poli P, Placentino A, Carletti F, Landi P, Allen P et al. 2009. Functional atlas of emotional faces processing: a voxel-based meta-analysis of 105 functional magnetic resonance imaging studies. J. Psychiatry Neurosci. 34:418–32
    [Google Scholar]
  52. Galbally M, Lewis AJ, van Ijzendoorn M, Permezel M 2011. The role of oxytocin in mother-infant relations: a systematic review of human studies. Harv. Rev. Psychiatry 19:1–14
    [Google Scholar]
  53. Glenn AL, Efferson LM, Iyer R, Graham J 2017. Values, goals, and motivations associated with psychopathy. J. Soc. Clin. Psychol. 36:108–25
    [Google Scholar]
  54. Glenn AL, Raine A 2009. Psychopathy and instrumental aggression: evolutionary, neurobiological, and legal perspectives. Int. J. Law Psychiatry 32:253–58
    [Google Scholar]
  55. Glocker ML, Langleben DD, Ruparel K, Loughead JW, Gur RC et al. 2009. Baby schema in infant faces induces cuteness perception and motivation for caretaking in adults. Ethology 115:257–63
    [Google Scholar]
  56. Hall JR, Drislane LE, Patrick CJ, Morano M, Lilienfeld SO et al. 2014. Development and validation of triarchic construct scales from the Psychopathic Personality Inventory. Psychol. Assess. 26:447–61
    [Google Scholar]
  57. Hammer JL, Marsh AA 2015. Why do fearful facial expressions elicit behavioral approach? Evidence from a combined approach-avoidance implicit association test. Emotion 15:223–31
    [Google Scholar]
  58. Hammond S 2016. Bone marrow mismatch: how compensating bone marrow donors can end the transplant shortage and save lives. Niskanen Center Nov. 15. https://niskanencenter.org/blog/bone-marrow-mismatch/
  59. Harbaugh WT, Mayr U, Burghart DR 2007. Neural responses to taxation and voluntary giving reveal motives for charitable donations. Science 316:1622–25
    [Google Scholar]
  60. Hare B 2017. Survival of the friendliest: Homo sapiens evolved via selection for prosociality. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 68:155–86
    [Google Scholar]
  61. Hein G, Silani G, Preuschoff K, Batson CD, Singer T 2010. Neural responses to ingroup and outgroup members’ suffering predict individual differences in costly helping. Neuron 68:149–60
    [Google Scholar]
  62. Henderson AJ, Landolt MA, McDonald MF, Barrable WM, Soos JG et al. 2003. The living anonymous kidney donor: lunatic or saint?. Am. J. Transplant. 3:203–13
    [Google Scholar]
  63. Hrdy SB 2009. Mothers and Others: The Evolutionary Origins of Mutual Understanding Cambridge, MA: Harvard Univ. Press
  64. Hubble K, Daughters K, Manstead ASR, Rees A, Thapar A et al. 2017. Oxytocin increases attention to the eyes and selectively enhances self-reported affective empathy for fear. Neuropsychologia 106:350–57
    [Google Scholar]
  65. Hung LW, Neuner S, Polepalli JS, Beier KT, Wright M et al. 2017. Gating of social reward by oxytocin in the ventral tegmental area. Science 357:1406–11
    [Google Scholar]
  66. Hyde LW, Waller R, Trentacosta CJ, Shaw DS, Neiderhiser JM et al. 2016. Heritable and nonheritable pathways to early callous-unemotional behaviors. Am. J. Psychiatry 173:903–10
    [Google Scholar]
  67. Jaeggi AV, Burkart JM, Van Schaik CP 2010. On the psychology of cooperation in humans and other primates: combining the natural history and experimental evidence of prosociality. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B 365:2723–35
    [Google Scholar]
  68. Jones B, Rachlin H 2006. Social discounting. Psychol. Sci. 17:283–86
    [Google Scholar]
  69. Keating CF, Randall DW, Kendrick T, Gutshall KA 2003. Do babyfaced adults receive more help? The (cross-cultural) case of the lost resume. J. Nonverbal Behav. 27:89–109
    [Google Scholar]
  70. Keebaugh AC, Barrett CE, Laprairie JL, Jenkins JJ, Young LJ 2015. RNAi knockdown of oxytocin receptor in the nucleus accumbens inhibits social attachment and parental care in monogamous female prairie voles. Soc. Neurosci. 10:561–70
    [Google Scholar]
  71. Keebaugh AC, Young LJ 2011. Increasing oxytocin receptor expression in the nucleus accumbens of pre-pubertal female prairie voles enhances alloparental responsiveness and partner preference formation as adults. Horm. Behav. 60:498–504
    [Google Scholar]
  72. Kim P 2016. Human maternal brain plasticity: adaptation to parenting. New Dir. Child Adolesc. Dev. 2016:47–58
    [Google Scholar]
  73. Kim P, Capistrano C, Congleton C 2016. Socioeconomic disadvantages and neural sensitivity to infant cry: role of maternal distress. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 11:1597–607
    [Google Scholar]
  74. Knafo A, Israel S, Ebstein RP 2011. Heritability of children's prosocial behavior and differential susceptibility to parenting by variation in the dopamine receptor D4 gene. Dev. Psychopathol. 23:53–67
    [Google Scholar]
  75. Koenigs M, Kruepke M, Newman JP 2010. Economic decision-making in psychopathy: a comparison with ventromedial prefrontal lesion patients. Neuropsychologia 48:2198–204
    [Google Scholar]
  76. Kohlhoff J, Eapen V, Dadds M, Khan F, Silove D et al. 2017. Oxytocin in the postnatal period: associations with attachment and maternal caregiving. Compr. Psychiatry 76:56–68
    [Google Scholar]
  77. Korndörfer M, Egloff B, Schmukle SC 2015. A large scale test of the effect of social class on prosocial behavior. PLOS ONE 10:e0133193
    [Google Scholar]
  78. Lang PJ, Bradley MM, Cuthbert BN 1999. International Affective Picture System (IAPS): Technical Manual and Affective Ratings Gainesville, FL: Cent. Res. Psychophysiol
  79. Lederman D 2010. Estimation of infants’ cry fundamental frequency using a modified SIFT algorithm. arXiv:1009.2796 [cs.SD]
  80. Lee MR, Scheidweiler KB, Diao XX, Akhlaghi F, Cummins A et al. 2018. Oxytocin by intranasal and intravenous routes reaches the cerebrospinal fluid in rhesus macaques: determination using a novel oxytocin assay. Mol. Psychiatry 23:115–22
    [Google Scholar]
  81. Leerkes EM, Nayena Blankson A, O'Brien M 2009. Differential effects of maternal sensitivity to infant distress and nondistress on social-emotional functioning. Child Dev 80:762–75
    [Google Scholar]
  82. Leppanen J, Ng KW, Tchanturia K, Treasure J 2017. Meta-analysis of the effects of intranasal oxytocin on interpretation and expression of emotions. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 78:125–44
    [Google Scholar]
  83. Li T, Chen X, Mascaro J, Haroon E, Rilling JK 2017. Intranasal oxytocin, but not vasopressin, augments neural responses to toddlers in human fathers. Horm. Behav. 93:193–202
    [Google Scholar]
  84. Lickley RA, Sebastian CL 2017. The neural basis of reactive aggression and its development in adolescence. Psychol. Crime Law 24:313–33
    [Google Scholar]
  85. Lingle S, Riede T 2014. Deer mothers are sensitive to infant distress vocalizations of diverse mammalian species. Am. Nat. 184:510–22
    [Google Scholar]
  86. Long G 1904. Discourses of Epictetus New York: Appleton
  87. Lorenz K 1966. On Aggression London: MethuenSeminal exploration of how nonverbal displays evoking infantile qualities can elicit care.
  88. Lozier LM, Cardinale EM, VanMeter JW, Marsh AA 2014. Mediation of the relationship between callous-unemotional traits and proactive aggression by amygdala response to fear among children with conduct problems. JAMA Psychiatry 71:627–36
    [Google Scholar]
  89. Lucion MK, Oliveira V, Bizarro L, Bischoff AR, Silveira PP et al. 2017. Attentional bias toward infant faces: review of the adaptive and clinical relevance. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 114:1–8
    [Google Scholar]
  90. Lynam DR, Caspi A, Moffitt TE, Loeber R, Stouthamer-Loeber M 2007. Longitudinal evidence that psychopathy scores in early adolescence predict adult psychopathy. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 116:155–65
    [Google Scholar]
  91. Madden JR, Clutton-Brock TH 2011. Experimental peripheral administration of oxytocin elevates a suite of cooperative behaviours in a wild social mammal. Proc. Biol. Sci. 278:1189–94
    [Google Scholar]
  92. Marsh AA 2016. Understanding amygdala responsiveness to fearful expressions through the lens of psychopathy and altruism. J. Neurosci. Res. 94:513–25
    [Google Scholar]
  93. Marsh AA 2017. The Fear Factor New York: Hachette
  94. Marsh AA 2018. The neuroscience of empathy. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 19:110–15
    [Google Scholar]
  95. Marsh AA, Adams RB, Kleck RE 2005. Why do fear and anger look the way they do? Form and social function in facial expressions. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 31:73–86First experimental evidence demonstrating that fearful expressions evoke properties of infants.
    [Google Scholar]
  96. Marsh AA, Ambady N 2007. The influence of the fear facial expression on prosocial responding. Cogn. Emotion 21:225–47
    [Google Scholar]
  97. Marsh AA, Blair RJ 2008. Deficits in facial affect recognition among antisocial populations: a meta-analysis. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 32:454–65
    [Google Scholar]
  98. Marsh AA, Kozak MN, Ambady N 2007. Accurate identification of fear facial expressions predicts prosocial behavior. Emotion 7:239–51
    [Google Scholar]
  99. Marsh AA, Stoycos SA, Brethel-Haurwitz KM, Robinson P, Cardinale EM 2014. Neural and cognitive characteristics of extraordinary altruists. PNAS 111:15036–41First evidence linking variation in brain structure and function to extremes in human altruism.
    [Google Scholar]
  100. Marsh AA, Yu HH, Pine DS, Gorodetsky EK, Goldman D et al. 2012. The influence of oxytocin administration on responses to infant faces and potential moderation by OXTR genotype. Psychopharmacology 224:469–76
    [Google Scholar]
  101. Massey EK, Kranenburg LW, Zuidema WC, Hak G, Erdman RA et al. 2010. Encouraging psychological outcomes after altruistic donation to a stranger. Am. J. Transplant. 10:1445–52
    [Google Scholar]
  102. McDougall W 1908. An Introduction to Social Psychology London: Methuen
  103. Mileva-Seitz V, Steiner M, Atkinson L, Meaney MJ, Levitan R et al. 2013. Interaction between oxytocin genotypes and early experience predicts quality of mothering and postpartum mood. PLOS ONE 8:e61443
    [Google Scholar]
  104. Miller GF 2007. Sexual selection for moral virtues. Q. Rev. Biol. 82:97–125
    [Google Scholar]
  105. Naber F, van Ijzendoorn MH, Deschamps P, van Engeland H, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ 2010. Intranasal oxytocin increases fathers’ observed responsiveness during play with their children: a double-blind within-subject experiment. Psychoneuroendocrinology 35:1583–86
    [Google Scholar]
  106. Newman JD 2007. Neural circuits underlying crying and cry responding in mammals. Behav. Brain Res. 182:155–65
    [Google Scholar]
  107. Nissen HW 1930. A study of maternal behavior in the white rat by means of the obstruction method. Pedag. Semin. J. Genet. Psychol. 37:377–92
    [Google Scholar]
  108. Numan M 2006. Hypothalamic neural circuits regulating maternal responsiveness toward infants. Behav. Cogn. Neurosci. Rev. 5:163–90
    [Google Scholar]
  109. Oftedal OT 2002. The mammary gland and its origin during synapsid evolution. J. Mammary Gland Biol. Neoplasia 7:225–52
    [Google Scholar]
  110. Okabe S, Tsuneoka Y, Takahashi A, Ooyama R, Watarai A et al. 2017. Pup exposure facilitates retrieving behavior via the oxytocin neural system in female mice. Psychoneuroendocrinology 79:20–30
    [Google Scholar]
  111. Olazábal DE 2014. Comparative analysis of oxytocin receptor density in the nucleus accumbens: an adaptation for female and male alloparental care. J. Physiol. Paris 108:213–20
    [Google Scholar]
  112. Oliner SP 2002. Extraordinary acts of ordinary people. Altruism and Altruistic Love SG Post, LG Underwood, J Schloss, WB Hurlbut 123–39 Oxford, UK: Oxford Univ. PressFirst formal exploration of the motivations underlying extraordinary care-based altruism.
    [Google Scholar]
  113. Orwell G 1968. My Country, Right or Left, 1940–1943 Boston: Nonpareil
  114. Panksepp J 1997. Affective Neuroscience: The Foundations of Human and Animal Emotions Oxford, UK: Oxford Univ. Press
  115. Pardini DA, Erickson K, Loeber R, Raine A 2014. Lower amygdala volume in men is associated with childhood aggression, early psychopathic traits, and future violence. Biol. Psychiatry 75:73–80
    [Google Scholar]
  116. Patton CL, Smith SF, Lilienfeld SO 2018. Psychopathy and heroism in first responders: traits cut from the same cloth?. Personal. Disord. 9:354–68
    [Google Scholar]
  117. Pedersen CA, Ascher JA, Monroe YL, Prange AJJ 1982. Oxytocin induces maternal behavior in virgin female rats. Science 216:648–50
    [Google Scholar]
  118. Pedersen CA, Prange AJ 1979. Induction of maternal behavior in virgin rats after intracerebroventricular administration of oxytocin. PNAS 76:6661–65First experimental evidence for the essential role oxytocin plays in parental care.
    [Google Scholar]
  119. Platek SM, Kemp SM 2009. Is family special to the brain? An event-related fMRI study of familiar, familial, and self-face recognition. Neuropsychologia 47:849–58
    [Google Scholar]
  120. Polier GG, Herpertz-Dahlmann B, Matthias K, Konrad K, Vloet TD 2010. Associations between trait anxiety and psychopathological characteristics of children at high risk for severe antisocial development. Atten. Defic. Hyperact. Disord. 2:185–93
    [Google Scholar]
  121. Pond CM 1977. The significance of lactation in the evolution of mammals. Evolution 31:177–99
    [Google Scholar]
  122. Preston SD 2013. The origins of altruism in offspring care. Psychol. Bull. 139:1305–41Theoretical review of evidence that altruism emerges from activity in parental care systems.
    [Google Scholar]
  123. Quintana DS, Alvares GA, Hickie IB, Guastella AJ 2015. Do delivery routes of intranasally administered oxytocin account for observed effects on social cognition and behavior? A two-level model. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 49:182–92
    [Google Scholar]
  124. Rajhans P, Altvater-Mackensen N, Vaish A, Grossmann T 2016. Children's altruistic behavior in context: the role of emotional responsiveness and culture. Sci. Rep. 6:24089
    [Google Scholar]
  125. Rand DG, Epstein ZG 2014. Risking your life without a second thought: intuitive decision-making and extreme altruism. PLOS ONE 9:e109687
    [Google Scholar]
  126. Rickenbacher E, Perry RE, Sullivan RM, Moita MA 2017. Freezing suppression by oxytocin in central amygdala allows alternate defensive behaviours and mother-pup interactions. eLife 6:e24080Experimental evidence that oxytocin in the amygdala plays a causal role in care-based altruism.
    [Google Scholar]
  127. Riem MM, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, Pieper S, Tops M, Boksem MA et al. 2011. Oxytocin modulates amygdala, insula, and inferior frontal gyrus responses to infant crying: a randomized controlled trial. Biol. Psychiatry 70:291–97
    [Google Scholar]
  128. Rilling JK, Sanfey AG 2011. The neuroscience of social decision-making. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 62:23–48
    [Google Scholar]
  129. Rodrigue JR, Schold JD, Morrissey P, Whiting J, Vella J et al. 2016. Direct and indirect costs following living kidney donation: findings from the KDOC Study. Am. J. Transplant. 16:869–76
    [Google Scholar]
  130. Rogers-Carter MM, Varela JA, Gribbons KB, Pierce AF, McGoey MT et al. 2018. Insular cortex mediates approach and avoidance responses to social affective stimuli. Nat. Neurosci. 21:404–14
    [Google Scholar]
  131. Romano A, Balliet D, Yamagishi T, Liu JH 2017. Parochial trust and cooperation across 17 societies. PNAS 114:12702–7
    [Google Scholar]
  132. Ross HE, Freeman SM, Spiegel LL, Ren X, Terwilliger EF et al. 2009. Variation in oxytocin receptor density in the nucleus accumbens has differential effects on affiliative behaviors in monogamous and polygamous voles. J. Neurosci. 29:1312–18
    [Google Scholar]
  133. Rushton JP, Fulker DW, Neale MC, Nias DK, Eysenck HJ 1986. Altruism and aggression: the heritability of individual differences. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 50:1192–98
    [Google Scholar]
  134. Saltzman W, Maestripieri D 2011. The neuroendocrinology of primate maternal behavior. Prog. Neuropsychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 35:1192–204
    [Google Scholar]
  135. Santos HC, Varnum MEW, Grossmann I 2017. Global increases in individualism. Psychol. Sci. 28:1228–39
    [Google Scholar]
  136. Schmidt PJ 2002. Blood and disaster: supply and demand. N. Engl. J. Med. 346:617–20
    [Google Scholar]
  137. Sherman GD, Haidt J, Iyer R, Coan JA 2013. Individual differences in the physical embodiment of care: prosocially oriented women respond to cuteness by becoming more physically careful. Emotion 38:2300–7
    [Google Scholar]
  138. Silk JB, House BR 2011. Evolutionary foundations of human prosocial sentiments. PNAS 108:10910–17
    [Google Scholar]
  139. Sinaceur M, Kopelman S, Vasiljevic D, Haag C 2015. Weep and get more: when and why sadness expression is effective in negotiations. J. Appl. Psychol. 100:1847–71
    [Google Scholar]
  140. Singh N, Albert FW, Plyusnina I, Trut L, Pääbo S et al. 2017. Facial shape differences between rats selected for tame and aggressive behaviors. PLOS ONE 12:e0175043
    [Google Scholar]
  141. Slovic P, Västfjäll D, Erlandsson A, Gregory R 2017. Iconic photographs and the ebb and flow of empathic response to humanitarian disasters. PNAS 114:640–44
    [Google Scholar]
  142. Small DA, Verrochi NM 2009. The face of need: facial emotion expression on charity advertisements. J. Mark. Res. 46:777–87
    [Google Scholar]
  143. Smearman EL, Almli LM, Conneely KN, Brody GH, Sales JM et al. 2016. Oxytocin receptor genetic and epigenetic variations: association with child abuse and adult psychiatric symptoms. Child. Dev. 87:122–34
    [Google Scholar]
  144. Staub E 1974. Helping a distressed person: social, personality, and stimulus determinants. Adv. Exper. Soc. Psychol. 7:293–341
    [Google Scholar]
  145. Staub E, Vollhardt J 2008. Altruism born of suffering: the roots of caring and helping after victimization and other trauma. Am. J. Orthopsychiatry 78:267–80
    [Google Scholar]
  146. Stoop R 2012. Neuromodulation by oxytocin and vasopressin. Neuron 76:142–59
    [Google Scholar]
  147. Swain JE, Konrath S, Brown SL, Finegood ED, Akce LB et al. 2012. Parenting and beyond: common neurocircuits underlying parental and altruistic caregiving. Parent. Sci. Pract. 12:115–23
    [Google Scholar]
  148. Thompson-Booth C, Viding E, Mayes LC, Rutherford HJ, Hodsoll S et al. 2014. I can't take my eyes off of you: attentional allocation to infant, child, adolescent and adult faces in mothers and non-mothers. PLOS ONE 9:e109362
    [Google Scholar]
  149. Tottenham N, Tanaka J, Leon AC, McCarry T, Nurse M et al. 2009. The NimStim set of facial expressions: judgments from untrained research participants. Psychiatry Res 168:242–49
    [Google Scholar]
  150. Triandis HC 1978. Some universals of social behavior. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 4:1–16
    [Google Scholar]
  151. van der Linden S 2018. Warm glow is associated with low- but not high-cost sustainable behaviour. Nat. Sustain. 1:28–30
    [Google Scholar]
  152. Vekaria KM, Brethel-Haurwitz KM, Cardinale EM, Stoycos SA, Marsh AA 2017. Social discounting and distance perceptions in costly altruism. Nat. Hum. Behav. 1:0100Experimental evidence linking the social discounting function to both care-based altruism and psychopathy.
    [Google Scholar]
  153. Viding E, McCrory EJ 2017. Understanding the development of psychopathy: progress and challenges. Psychol. Med. 48:566–77
    [Google Scholar]
  154. Vieira JB, Ferreira-Santos F, Almeida PR, Barbosa F, Marques-Teixeira J, Marsh AA 2015. Psychopathic traits are associated with cortical and subcortical volume alterations in healthy individuals. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 10:1693–704
    [Google Scholar]
  155. Viviani D, Charlet A, van den Burg E, Robinet C, Hurni N et al. 2011. Oxytocin selectively gates fear responses through distinct outputs from the central amygdala. Science 333:104–7
    [Google Scholar]
  156. Wang D, Yan X, Li M, Ma Y 2017. Neural substrates underlying the effects of oxytocin: a quantitative meta-analysis of pharmaco-imaging studies. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 12:1565–73
    [Google Scholar]
  157. White SF, Briggs-Gowan MJ, Voss JL, Petitclerc A, McCarthy K et al. 2016. Can the fear recognition deficits associated with callous-unemotional traits be identified in early childhood?. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 38:672–84
    [Google Scholar]
  158. Wrangham RW 2018. Two types of aggression in human evolution. PNAS 115:245–53
    [Google Scholar]
  159. Xiao L, Priest MF, Nasenbeny J, Lu T, Kozorovitskiy Y 2017. Biased oxytocinergic modulation of midbrain dopamine systems. Neuron 95:368–84.e5
    [Google Scholar]
  160. Yang Y, Raine A, Narr KL, Colletti P, Toga AW 2009. Localization of deformations within the amygdala in individuals with psychopathy. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 66:986–94
    [Google Scholar]
  161. Yildirim BO, Derksen JJ 2015. Mesocorticolimbic dopamine functioning in primary psychopathy: a source of within-group heterogeneity. Psychiatry Res 229:633–77
    [Google Scholar]
  162. Zebrowitz LA, Luevano VX, Bronstad PM, Aharon I 2009. Neural activation to babyfaced men matches activation to babies. Soc. Neurosci. 4:1–10
    [Google Scholar]
/content/journals/10.1146/annurev-psych-010418-103010
Loading
/content/journals/10.1146/annurev-psych-010418-103010
Loading

Data & Media loading...

  • Article Type: Review Article
This is a required field
Please enter a valid email address
Approval was a Success
Invalid data
An Error Occurred
Approval was partially successful, following selected items could not be processed due to error