1932

Abstract

Psychology has traditionally seen itself as the science of universal human cognition, but it has only recently begun seriously grappling with cross-cultural variation. Here we argue that the roots of cross-cultural variation often lie in the past. Therefore, to understand not only how but also why psychology varies, we need to grapple with cross-temporal variation. The traces of past human cognition accessible through historical texts and artifacts can serve as a valuable, and almost completely unutilized, source of psychological data. These data from dead minds open up an untapped and highly diverse subject pool. We review examples of research that may be classified as historical psychology, introduce sources of historical data and methods for analyzing them, explain the critical role of theory, and discuss how psychologists can add historical depth and nuance to their work. Psychology needs to become a historical science if it wants to be a genuinely universal science of human cognition and behavior.

Loading

Article metrics loading...

/content/journals/10.1146/annurev-psych-082820-111436
2021-01-04
2024-03-28
Loading full text...

Full text loading...

/deliver/fulltext/psych/72/1/annurev-psych-082820-111436.html?itemId=/content/journals/10.1146/annurev-psych-082820-111436&mimeType=html&fmt=ahah

Literature Cited

  1. Akbari M, Bahrami-Rad D, Kimbrough EO 2019. Kinship, fractionalization and corruption. J. Econ. Behav. Organ. 166:493–528
    [Google Scholar]
  2. Alesina A, Giuliano P, Nunn N 2013. On the origins of gender roles: women and the plough. Q. J. Econ. 128:469–530
    [Google Scholar]
  3. Algan Y, Cahuc P. 2010. Inherited trust and growth. Am. Econ. Rev. 100:52060–92
    [Google Scholar]
  4. Amir D, Jordan MR, McAuliffe K, Valeggia CR, Sugiyama LS et al. 2020. The developmental origins of risk and time preferences across diverse societies. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 149:4650–61
    [Google Scholar]
  5. Angrist JD, Imbens GW, Rubin DB 1996. Identification of causal effects using instrumental variables. J. Am. Stat. Assoc. 91:43444455
    [Google Scholar]
  6. Angrist JD, Pischke J-S. 2009. Mostly Harmless Econometrics: An Empiricist's Companion Princeton, NJ: Princeton Univ. Press
  7. Apicella CL, Azevedo EM, Christakis NA, Fowler JH 2014. Evolutionary origins of the endowment effect: evidence from hunter-gatherers. Am. Econ. Rev. 104:61793–805
    [Google Scholar]
  8. Arnett JJ. 2008. The neglected 95%: why American psychology needs to become less American. Am. Psychol. 63:7602–14
    [Google Scholar]
  9. Asch SE. 1956. Studies of independence and conformity: I. A minority of one against a unanimous majority. Psychol. Monogr. Gen. Appl. 70:9170
    [Google Scholar]
  10. Astuti R, Harris P. 2008. Understanding mortality and the life of the ancestors in rural Madagascar. Cogn. Sci. 32:4713–40
    [Google Scholar]
  11. Atkinson QD. 2011. Phonemic diversity supports a serial founder effect model of language expansion from Africa. Science 332:346–49
    [Google Scholar]
  12. Atran S, Henrich J. 2010. The evolution of religion: how cognitive by-products, adaptive learning heuristics, ritual displays, and group competition generate deep commitments to prosocial religions. Biol. Theory 5:118–30
    [Google Scholar]
  13. Aveyard ME. 2014. A call to honesty: extending religious priming of moral behavior to Middle Eastern Muslims. PLOS ONE 9:7e99447
    [Google Scholar]
  14. Banerjee K, Bloom P. 2015. “Everything happens for a reason”: children's beliefs about purpose in life events. Child Dev 86:2503–18
    [Google Scholar]
  15. Barrett HC, Bolyanatz A, Crittenden AN, Fessler DMT, Fitzpatrick S et al. 2016. Small-scale societies exhibit fundamental variation in the role of intentions in moral judgment. PNAS 113:174688–93
    [Google Scholar]
  16. Baumard N. 2019. Psychological origins of the Industrial Revolution. Behav. Brain Sci. 42:e189
    [Google Scholar]
  17. Baumard N, Chevallier C. 2015. The nature and dynamics of world religions: a life-history approach. Proc. R. Soc. B 282:181820151593
    [Google Scholar]
  18. Baumard N, Huillery E, Zabrocki L 2018. The origins of romantic love and asceticism: how economic prosperity changed human psychology in medieval Europe Work. Pap., Sciences Po Paris:
  19. Baumard N, Hyafil A, Morris I, Boyer P 2015. Increased affluence explains the emergence of ascetic wisdoms and moralizing religions. Curr. Biol. 25:110–15
    [Google Scholar]
  20. Becker A. 2019. On the economic origins of restrictions on women's sexuality CESifo Work. Pap. No. 7770, Cent. Econ. Stud Munich, Ger:.
  21. Beheim B, Atkinson Q, Bulbulia J, Gervais WM, Gray R et al. 2019. Treatment of missing data determines conclusions regarding moralizing gods Work. Pap., Max Planck Inst. Evol. Anthropol Leipzig, Ger: https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/jwa2n
    [Crossref]
  22. Bell AV, Richerson PJ, McElreath R 2009. Culture rather than genes provides greater scope for the evolution of large-scale human prosociality. PNAS 106:4217671–74
    [Google Scholar]
  23. Benenson JF, Abadzi H. 2020. Contest versus scramble competition: sex differences in the quest for status. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 33:62–68
    [Google Scholar]
  24. Billing J, Sherman PW. 1998. Antimicrobial functions of spices: why some like it hot. Q. Rev. Biol. 73:13–49
    [Google Scholar]
  25. Blake PR, McAuliffe K, Corbit J, Callaghan TC, Barry O et al. 2015. The ontogeny of fairness in seven societies. Nature 528:7581258–61
    [Google Scholar]
  26. Blaydes L, Chaney E. 2013. The feudal revolution and Europe's rise: political divergence of the Christian West and the Muslim world before 1500 CE. Am. Political Sci. Rev. 107:116–34
    [Google Scholar]
  27. Blei DM. 2012. Probabilistic topic models. Commun. ACM 55:477–84
    [Google Scholar]
  28. Bloom P. 2004. Descartes’ Baby: How the Science of Child Development Explains What Makes Us Human New York: Basic Books
  29. Bond R, Smith PB. 1996. Culture and conformity: a meta-analysis of studies using Asch's (1952b, 1956) line judgment task. Psychol. Bull. 119:1111–37
    [Google Scholar]
  30. Botero CA, Gardner B, Kirby KR, Bulbulia J, Gavin MC, Gray RD 2014. The ecology of religious beliefs. PNAS 111:4716784–89
    [Google Scholar]
  31. Boyd R. 2018. A Different Kind of Animal: How Culture Transformed Our Species Princeton, NJ: Princeton Univ. Press
  32. Boyd R, Richerson PJ. 1985. Culture and the Evolutionary Process Chicago: Univ. Chicago Press
  33. Boyd RL. 2017. Psychological text analysis in the digital humanities. Data Analytics in Digital Humanities S Hai-Jew 161–89 Cham, Switz: Springer
    [Google Scholar]
  34. Brinch CN, Galloway TA. 2012. Schooling in adolescence raises IQ scores. PNAS 109:425–30
    [Google Scholar]
  35. Bullinaria JA, Levy JP. 2012. Extracting semantic representations from word co-occurrence statistics: stop-lists, stemming, and SVD. Behav. Res. 44:3890–907
    [Google Scholar]
  36. Buss DM. 1994. The Evolution of Desire: Strategies of Human Mating New York: Basic Books
  37. Bussey K, Bandura A. 1999. Social cognitive theory of gender development and differentiation. Psychol. Rev. 106:4676–713
    [Google Scholar]
  38. Caliskan A, Bryson JJ, Narayanan A 2017. Semantics derived automatically from language corpora contain human-like biases. Science 356:6334183–86
    [Google Scholar]
  39. Campany RF. 2003. On the very idea of religions (in the modern West and in early medieval China). Hist. Relig. 42:4287–319
    [Google Scholar]
  40. Chaney E. 2016. Religion and the rise and fall of Islamic science Work. Pap., Harvard Univ Cambridge, MA:
  41. Chatterjee K, Pavlogiannis A, Adlam B, Nowak MA 2014. The time scale of evolutionary innovation. PLOS Comput. Biol. 10:9e1003818
    [Google Scholar]
  42. Cheng JT, Anderson C, Tenney ER, Brion S, Moore DA, Logg JM 2020. The social transmission of overconfidence. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. In press. https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0000787
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  43. Chua RYJ, Huang KG, Jin M 2019. Mapping cultural tightness and its links to innovation, urbanization, and happiness across 31 provinces in China. PNAS 116:146720–25
    [Google Scholar]
  44. Chudek M, McNamara RA, Birch S, Bloom P, Henrich J 2018. Do minds switch bodies? Dualist interpretations across ages and societies. Relig. Brain Behav. 8:4354–68
    [Google Scholar]
  45. Chudek M, Muthukrishna M, Henrich J 2015. Cultural evolution. The Handbook of Evolutionary Psychology: Integrations DM Buss 749–69 New York: Wiley
    [Google Scholar]
  46. Clark KJ, Winslett JT. 2011. The evolutionary psychology of Chinese religion: pre-Qin high gods as punishers and rewarders. J. Am. Acad. Relig. 79:4928–60
    [Google Scholar]
  47. Cohn A, Maréchal MA, Tannenbaum D, Zünd CL 2019. Civic honesty around the globe. Science 365:644870–73
    [Google Scholar]
  48. Cooperrider K. 2019. What happens to cognitive diversity when everyone is more WEIRD. AeonJan. 23 https://aeon.co/ideas/what-happens-to-cognitive-diversity-when-everyone-is-more-weird
    [Google Scholar]
  49. Davies M, Hegedűs I, Fodor A 2012. The 400 million word corpus of historical American English (1810–2009) Paper presented at the Sixteenth International Conference on English Historical Linguistics (ICEHL 16) Pécs, Hung Aug 23–27
  50. De Leersnyder J, Mesquita B, Kim HS 2011. Where do my emotions belong? A study of immigrants’ emotional acculturation. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 37:4451–63
    [Google Scholar]
  51. Dehaene S, Pegado F, Braga LW, Ventura P, Nunes Filho G et al. 2010. How learning to read changes the cortical networks for vision and language. Science 330:60091359–64
    [Google Scholar]
  52. Derex M, Beugin M-P, Godelle B, Raymond M 2013. Experimental evidence for the influence of group size on cultural complexity. Nature 503:7476389–91
    [Google Scholar]
  53. Derex M, Bonnefon J-F, Boyd R, Mesoudi A 2019. Causal understanding is not necessary for the improvement of culturally evolving technology. Nat. Hum. Behav. 3:5446–52
    [Google Scholar]
  54. Dinesen PT. 2012. Does generalized (dis)trust travel? Examining the impact of cultural heritage and destination-country environment on trust of immigrants. Political Psychol 33:4495–511
    [Google Scholar]
  55. Duhaime EP. 2015. Is the call to prayer a call to cooperate? A field experiment on the impact of religious salience on prosocial behavior. Judgm. Decis. Mak. 10:6593–96
    [Google Scholar]
  56. Enke B. 2019. Kinship, cooperation, and the evolution of moral systems. Q. J. Econ. 134:2953–1019
    [Google Scholar]
  57. Evans AM, Krueger JI. 2009. The psychology (and economics) of trust. Soc. Personal. Psychol. Compass 3:61003–17
    [Google Scholar]
  58. Falk A, Hermle J. 2018. Relationship of gender differences in preferences to economic development and gender equality. Science 362:6412eaas9899
    [Google Scholar]
  59. Fan S, Hansen MEB, Lo Y, Tishkoff SA 2016. Going global by adapting local: a review of recent human adaptation. Science 354:630854–59
    [Google Scholar]
  60. Finkel EJ, Hui CM, Carswell KL, Larson GM 2014. The suffocation of marriage: climbing Mount Maslow without enough oxygen. Psychol. Inq. 25:11–41
    [Google Scholar]
  61. Francois P, Manning J, Whitehouse H, Brennan R, Currie T et al. 2016. A macroscope for global history: Seshat Global History Databank: a methodological overview. Digit. Humanit. Q. 10:4272
    [Google Scholar]
  62. Frankenhuis WE, Panchanathan K, Nettle D 2016. Cognition in harsh and unpredictable environments. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 7:76–80
    [Google Scholar]
  63. Garg N, Schiebinger L, Jurafsky D, Zou J 2018. Word embeddings quantify 100 years of gender and ethnic stereotypes. PNAS 115:16E3635–44
    [Google Scholar]
  64. Garvert MM, Moutoussis M, Kurth-Nelson Z, Behrens TEJ, Dolan RJ 2015. Learning-induced plasticity in medial prefrontal cortex predicts preference malleability. Neuron 85:2418–28
    [Google Scholar]
  65. Gavin MC, Kavanagh PH, Haynie HJ, Bowern C, Ember CR et al. 2018. The global geography of human subsistence. R. Soc. Open Sci. 5:9171897
    [Google Scholar]
  66. Gelfand MJ. 2018. Rule Makers, Rule Breakers: How Tight and Loose Cultures Wire Our World New York: Scribner
  67. Gelfand MJ, Nishii LH, Raver JL 2006. On the nature and importance of cultural tightness-looseness. J. Appl. Psychol. 91:61225–44
    [Google Scholar]
  68. Gelfand MJ, Raver JL, Nishii L, Leslie LM, Lun J et al. 2011. Differences between tight and loose cultures: a 33-nation study. Science 332:60331100–4
    [Google Scholar]
  69. Gernet J. 1985. China and the Christian Impact: A Conflict of Cultures Cambridge, UK: Cambridge Univ. Press
  70. Giavazzi F, Petkov I, Schiantarelli F 2019. Culture: persistence and evolution. J. Econ. Growth 24:117–54
    [Google Scholar]
  71. Gibson E, Futrell R, Jara-Ettinger J, Mahowald K, Bergen L et al. 2017. Color naming across languages reflects color use. PNAS 114:4010785–90
    [Google Scholar]
  72. Giuliano P, Nunn N. 2013. The transmission of democracy: from the village to the nation-state. Am. Econ. Rev. 103:386–92
    [Google Scholar]
  73. Goody J. 1983. The Development of the Family and Marriage in Europe Cambridge, UK: Cambridge Univ. Press
  74. Graham J, Haidt J, Koleva S, Motyl M, Iyer R et al. 2013. Moral foundations theory: the pragmatic validity of moral pluralism. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology 47 P Devine, A Plant 55–130 Amsterdam: Elsevier
    [Google Scholar]
  75. Gray RD, Bryant D, Greenhill SJ 2010. On the shape and fabric of human history. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 365:15593923–33
    [Google Scholar]
  76. Guida A, Megreya AM, Lavielle-Guida M, Noël Y, Mathy F et al. 2018. Spatialization in working memory is related to literacy and reading direction: Culture “literarily” directs our thoughts. Cognition 175:96–100
    [Google Scholar]
  77. Gurven M, von Rueden C, Massenkoff M, Kaplan H, Lero Vie M 2013. How universal is the Big Five? Testing the five-factor model of personality variation among forager-farmers in the Bolivian Amazon. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 104:2354–70
    [Google Scholar]
  78. Hadnes M, Schumacher H. 2012. The gods are watching: an experimental study of religion and traditional belief in Burkina Faso. J. Sci. Study Relig. 51:4689–704
    [Google Scholar]
  79. Han S, Northoff G, Vogeley K, Wexler BE, Kitayama S, Varnum MEW 2013. A cultural neuroscience approach to the biosocial nature of the human brain. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 64:335–59
    [Google Scholar]
  80. Harrington JR, Gelfand MJ. 2014. Tightness-looseness across the 50 United States. PNAS 111:7990–95
    [Google Scholar]
  81. Harrison MA, Hughes SM, Gott AJ 2019. Sex differences in serial killers. Evol. Behav. Sci. 13:4295–310
    [Google Scholar]
  82. Henrich J. 2004. Demography and cultural evolution: how adaptive cultural processes can produce maladaptive losses—the Tasmanian case. Am. Antiq. 69:2197–214
    [Google Scholar]
  83. Henrich J. 2009. The evolution of costly displays, cooperation and religion credibility enhancing displays and their implications for cultural evolution. Evol. Hum. Behav. 30:4244–60
    [Google Scholar]
  84. Henrich J. 2016. The Secret of Our Success: How Culture Is Driving Human Evolution, Domesticating Our Species, and Making Us Smarter Princeton, NJ: Princeton Univ. Press
  85. Henrich J. 2020. The WEIRDest People in the World: How the West Became Psychologically Peculiar and Particularly Prosperous New York: Farrar, Straus and Giroux
  86. Henrich J, Boyd R. 2008. Division of labor, economic specialization, and the evolution of social stratification. Curr. Anthropol. 49:4715–24
    [Google Scholar]
  87. Henrich J, Boyd R, Bowles S, Camerer C, Fehr E et al. 2001. In search of Homo economicus: behavioral experiments in 15 small-scale societies. Am. Econ. Rev. 91:273–84
    [Google Scholar]
  88. Henrich J, Boyd R, Derex M, Kline MA, Mesoudi A et al. 2016. Understanding cumulative cultural evolution. PNAS 113:44E6724–25
    [Google Scholar]
  89. Henrich J, Boyd R, Richerson PJ 2012. The puzzle of monogamous marriage. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 367:1589657–69
    [Google Scholar]
  90. Henrich J, Ensminger J, McElreath R, Barr A, Barrett C et al. 2010a. Markets, religion, community size, and the evolution of fairness and punishment. Science 327:59721480–84
    [Google Scholar]
  91. Henrich J, Heine SJ, Norenzayan A 2010b. The weirdest people in the world. Behav. Brain Sci. 33:2–3 61–83; discussion 83–135
    [Google Scholar]
  92. Henrich J, Muthukrishna M. 2020. The origins and psychology of human cooperation. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 72:20740
    [Google Scholar]
  93. Herrmann B, Thoni C, Gachter S 2008. Antisocial punishment across societies. Science 319:58681362–67
    [Google Scholar]
  94. Hidalgo CA, Klinger B, Barabási A-L, Hausmann R 2007. The product space conditions the development of nations. Science 317:5837482–87
    [Google Scholar]
  95. Hodge KM. 2008. Descartes’ mistake: how afterlife beliefs challenge the assumption that humans are intuitive Cartesian substance dualists. J. Cogn. Cult. 8:3–4387–415
    [Google Scholar]
  96. Hoehl S, Keupp S, Schleihauf H, McGuigan N, Buttelmann D, Whiten A 2019. “Over-imitation”: a review and appraisal of a decade of research. Dev. Rev. 51:90–108
    [Google Scholar]
  97. Hruschka D, Efferson C, Jiang T, Falletta-Cowden A, Sigurdsson S et al. 2014. Impartial institutions, pathogen stress and the expanding social network. Hum. Nat. 25:4567–79
    [Google Scholar]
  98. Hruschka DJ, Henrich J. 2013. Economic and evolutionary hypotheses for cross-population variation in parochialism. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:559
    [Google Scholar]
  99. Iliev R, Hoover J, Dehghani M, Axelrod R 2016. Linguistic positivity in historical texts reflects dynamic environmental and psychological factors. PNAS 113:49E7871–79
    [Google Scholar]
  100. Irons W. 2001. Religion as a hard-to-fake sign of commitment. Evolution and the Capacity for Commitment RM Nesse 292–309 New York: Russell Sage Found.
    [Google Scholar]
  101. Jablonski NG. 2018. Evolution of human skin color and Vitamin D. Vitamin D, Vol. 1: Biochemistry, Physiology and Diagnostics D Feldman 29–44 Amsterdam: Elsevier
    [Google Scholar]
  102. Jablonski NG, Chaplin G. 2010. Human skin pigmentation as an adaptation to UV radiation. PNAS 107:Suppl. 28962–68
    [Google Scholar]
  103. Jablonski NG, Chaplin G. 2017. The colours of humanity: the evolution of pigmentation in the human lineage. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B. 372:172420160349
    [Google Scholar]
  104. Jackson JC, Gelfand M, De S, Fox A 2019. The loosening of American culture over 200 years is associated with a creativity–order trade-off. Nat. Hum. Behav. 3:244–50
    [Google Scholar]
  105. Kaiser T, Del Giudice M, Booth T 2020. Global sex differences in personality: replication with an open online dataset. J. Personal. 88:3415–29
    [Google Scholar]
  106. Kauffman SA. 2003. Investigations New York: Oxford Univ. Press
  107. Kendal RL, Boogert NJ, Rendell L, Laland KN, Webster M, Jones PL 2018. Social learning strategies: bridge-building between fields. Trends Cogn. Sci. 22:7651–65
    [Google Scholar]
  108. Kitayama S, Duffy S, Kawamura T, Larsen JT 2003. Perceiving and object and its context in different cultures: a cultural look at new look. Psychol. Sci. 14:3201–6
    [Google Scholar]
  109. Kleven H, Landais C, Posch J, Steinhauer A, Zweimüller J 2019. Child penalties across countries: evidence and explanations NBER Work. Pap. 25524
  110. Kline MA, Boyd R. 2010. Population size predicts technological complexity in Oceania. Proc. R. Soc. B 277:16932559–64
    [Google Scholar]
  111. Kurdi B, Mann TC, Charlesworth TES, Banaji MR 2019. The relationship between implicit intergroup attitudes and beliefs. PNAS 116:135862–71
    [Google Scholar]
  112. Kwiatkowski DP. 2005. How malaria has affected the human genome and what human genetics can teach us about malaria. Am. J. Hum. Genet. 77:2171–92
    [Google Scholar]
  113. Lang M, Purzycki BG, Apicella CL, Atkinson QD, Bolyanatz A et al. 2019. Moralizing gods, impartiality and religious parochialism across 15 societies. Proc. R. Soc. B 286:189820190202
    [Google Scholar]
  114. Layton TJ, Barnett ML, Hicks TR, Jena AB 2018. Attention deficit–hyperactivity disorder and month of school enrollment. N. Engl. J. Med. 379:222122–30
    [Google Scholar]
  115. Lippa RA. 2010. Gender differences in personality and interests: when, where, and why. Soc. Personal. Psychol. Compass 4:111098–110
    [Google Scholar]
  116. Lukaszewski AW, Gurven M, von Rueden CR, Schmitt DP 2017. What explains personality covariation? A test of the socioecological complexity hypothesis. Soc. Psychol. Personal. Sci. 8:8943–52
    [Google Scholar]
  117. Martin L. 2014. Introduction to the issue. J. Cogn. Hist. 1:110–13
    [Google Scholar]
  118. Matthews LJ, Edmonds J, Wildman WJ, Nunn CL 2013. Cultural inheritance or cultural diffusion of religious violence? A quantitative case study of the Radical Reformation. Relig. Brain Behav. 3:13–15
    [Google Scholar]
  119. McCrae RR, Terracciano A. 2005. Personality profiles of cultures: aggregate personality traits. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 89:3407–25
    [Google Scholar]
  120. McNamara RA, Henrich J. 2018. Jesus versus the ancestors: how specific religious beliefs shape prosociality on Yasawa Island, Fiji. Relig. Brain Behav. 8:2185–204
    [Google Scholar]
  121. McNamara RA, Willard AK, Norenzayan A, Henrich J 2019. Weighing outcome versus intent across societies: how cultural models of mind shape moral reasoning. Cognition 182:95–108
    [Google Scholar]
  122. McNeil WH. 1991. The Rise of the West: A History of the Human Community Chicago: Univ. Chicago Press
  123. Mesoudi A, Magid K, Hussain D 2016. How do people become W.E.I.R.D.? Migration reveals the cultural transmission mechanisms underlying variation in psychological processes. PLOS ONE 11:1e0147162
    [Google Scholar]
  124. Mitterauer M, Chapple G. 2010. Why Europe? The Medieval Origins of Its Special Path Chicago: Univ. Chicago Press
  125. Moore DA, Dev AS, Goncharova EY 2018. Overconfidence across cultures. Collabra Psychol 4:136
    [Google Scholar]
  126. Moretti F. 2013. Distant Reading London: Verso
  127. Munson J, Amati V, Collard M, Macri MJ 2014. Classic Maya bloodletting and the cultural evolution of religious rituals: quantifying patterns of variation in hieroglyphic texts. PLOS ONE 9:9e107982
    [Google Scholar]
  128. Muthukrishna M. 2017. Corruption, cooperation, and the evolution of prosocial institutions Work. Pap., London Sch. Econ. Political Sci. London:
  129. Muthukrishna M, Bell AV, Henrich J, Curtin CM, Gedranovich A et al. 2020. Beyond Western, educated, industrial, rich, and democratic (WEIRD) psychology: measuring and mapping scales of cultural and psychological distance. Psychol. Sci. 31:6678–701
    [Google Scholar]
  130. Muthukrishna M, Francois P, Pourahmadi S, Henrich J 2017. Corrupting cooperation and how anti-corruption strategies may backfire. Nat. Hum. Behav. 1:70138
    [Google Scholar]
  131. Muthukrishna M, Henrich J. 2016. Innovation in the collective brain. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 371:1690137–48
    [Google Scholar]
  132. Muthukrishna M, Henrich J. 2019. A problem in theory. Nat. Hum. Behav. 3:221–29
    [Google Scholar]
  133. Muthukrishna M, Henrich J, Toyokawa W, Hamamura T, Kameda T, Heine SJ 2018. Overconfidence is universal? Elicitation of Genuine Overconfidence (EGO) procedure reveals systematic differences across domain, task knowledge, and incentives in four populations. PLOS ONE 13:8e0202288
    [Google Scholar]
  134. Muthukrishna M, Schaller M. 2020. Are collectivistic cultures more prone to rapid transformation? Computational models of cross-cultural differences, social network structure, dynamic social influence, and cultural change. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 24:2103–20
    [Google Scholar]
  135. Muthukrishna M, Shulman BW, Vasilescu V, Henrich J 2013. Sociality influences cultural complexity. Proc. R. Soc. B 281:177420132511
    [Google Scholar]
  136. Nettle D. 2010. Dying young and living fast: variation in life history across English neighborhoods. Behav. Ecol. 21:2387–95
    [Google Scholar]
  137. Nichols R, Slingerland E, Nielbo KL, Kirby P, Logan C 2020. Supernatural agents and prosociality in historical China: micro-modeling the cultural evolution of gods and morality in textual corpora. Relig. Brain Behav. In press https://doi.org/10.1080/2153599X.2020.1742778
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  138. Nielsen M, Haun D, Kärtner J, Legare CH 2017. The persistent sampling bias in developmental psychology: a call to action. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 162:31–38
    [Google Scholar]
  139. Nisbett RE. 2003. The Geography of Thought: How Asians and Westerners Think Differently… and Why New York: The Free Press
  140. Norenzayan A, Shariff AF. 2008. The origin and evolution of religious prosociality. Science 322:589858–62
    [Google Scholar]
  141. Norenzayan A, Shariff AF, Gervais WM, Willard AK, McNamara RA et al. 2016. The cultural evolution of prosocial religions. Behav. Brain Sci. 39:e1
    [Google Scholar]
  142. Norris P, Inglehart RF. 2012. Muslim integration into Western cultures: between origins and destinations. Political Stud 60:2228–51
    [Google Scholar]
  143. Nunn N. 2008. The long-term effects of Africa's slave trades. Q. J. Econ. 123:1139–76
    [Google Scholar]
  144. Nunn N. 2009. The importance of history for economic development. Annu. Rev. Econ. 1:65–92
    [Google Scholar]
  145. Nunn N. 2020. The historical roots of economic development. Science 367:6485eaaz9986
    [Google Scholar]
  146. Nunn N, Wantchekon L. 2011. The slave trade and the origins of mistrust in Africa. Am. Econ. Rev. 101:73221–52
    [Google Scholar]
  147. Obschonka M, Stuetzer M, Rentfrow PJ, Shaw-Taylor L, Satchell M et al. 2018. In the shadow of coal: how large-scale industries contributed to present-day regional differences in personality and well-being. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 115:5903–27
    [Google Scholar]
  148. Orians GH. 1969. On the evolution of mating systems in birds and mammals. Am. Nat. 103:934589–603
    [Google Scholar]
  149. Pacheco Coelho MT, Pereira EB, Haynie HJ, Rangel TF, Kavanagh P et al. 2019. Drivers of geographical patterns of North American language diversity. Proc. R. Soc. B 286:189920190242
    [Google Scholar]
  150. Page SE. 2006. Path dependence. Q. J. Political Sci. 1:187–115
    [Google Scholar]
  151. Pearl J, Glymour M, Jewell NP 2016. Causal Inference in Statistics: A Primer New York: Wiley
  152. Pearl J, Mackenzie D. 2018. The Book of Why: The New Science of Cause and Effect New York: Basic Books
  153. Pennington J, Socher R, Manning C 2014. Glove: global vectors for word representation. Proceedings of the 2014 Conference on Empirical Methods in Natural Language Processing (EMNLP)1532–43 Doha, Qatar: Assoc. Comput. Linguist.
    [Google Scholar]
  154. Pitchford NJ, Mullen KT. 2002. Is the acquisition of basic-colour terms in young children constrained. Perception 31:111349–70
    [Google Scholar]
  155. Polity IV. 2014. Polity IV project: political regime characteristics and transitions, 1800–2010 Cent. Syst. Peace Vienna, VA: updated June 6. https://www.systemicpeace.org/polity/polity4.htm
  156. Powell A, Shennan S, Thomas MG 2009. Late Pleistocene demography and the appearance of modern human behavior. Science 324:59321298–301
    [Google Scholar]
  157. Purzycki BG, Apicella CL, Atkinson QD, Cohen E, McNamara RA et al. 2016. Moralistic gods, supernatural punishment and the expansion of human sociality. Nature 530:7590327–30
    [Google Scholar]
  158. Purzycki BG, Henrich J, Norenzayan A 2018a. The evolution of religion and morality project. Religion Brain Behav8
    [Google Scholar]
  159. Purzycki BG, Ross CT, Apicella C, Atkinson QD, Cohen E et al. 2018b. Material security, life history, and moralistic religions: a cross-cultural examination. PLOS ONE 13:3e0193856
    [Google Scholar]
  160. Rand DG, Dreber A, Haque OS, Kane RJ, Nowak MA, Coakley S 2014. Religious motivations for cooperation: an experimental investigation using explicit primes. Relig. Brain Behav. 4:131–48
    [Google Scholar]
  161. Renfrew C. 2008. Prehistory: The Making of the Human Mind New York: Random House
  162. Richerson P, Baldini R, Bell AV, Demps K, Frost K et al. 2016. Cultural group selection plays an essential role in explaining human cooperation: a sketch of the evidence. Behav. Brain Sci. 39:e30
    [Google Scholar]
  163. Rockmore DN, Fang C, Foti NJ, Ginsburg T, Krakauer DC 2018. The cultural evolution of national constitutions. J. Assoc. Inf. Sci. Technol. 69:3483–94
    [Google Scholar]
  164. Rozenblit L, Keil F. 2002. The misunderstood limits of folk science: an illusion of explanatory depth. Cogn. Sci. 26:5521–62
    [Google Scholar]
  165. Salali GD, Dyble M, Chaudhary N, Sikka G, Derkx I et al. 2020. Global WEIRDing: transitions in wild plant knowledge and treatment preferences in Congo hunter-gatherers. Evol. Hum. Sci. 2:e24
    [Google Scholar]
  166. Santos HC, Varnum MEW, Grossmann I 2017. Global increases in individualism. Psychol. Sci. 28:91228–39
    [Google Scholar]
  167. Schulz JF, Bahrami-Rad D, Beauchamp JP, Henrich J 2019. The Church, intensive kinship, and global psychological variation. Science 366:6466eaau5141
    [Google Scholar]
  168. Schwartz SH. 2006. A theory of cultural value orientations: explication and applications. Comp. Sociol. 5:2–3137–82
    [Google Scholar]
  169. Seligman M 2020. Agency in Greco-Roman philosophy. J. Posit. Psychol In press
    [Google Scholar]
  170. Shadish WR, Cook TD, Campbell DT 2001. Experimental and Quasi-Experimental Designs for Generalized Causal Inference Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin
  171. Shariff AF, Norenzayan A. 2007. God is watching you: Priming God concepts increases prosocial behavior in an anonymous economic game. Psychol. Sci. 18:9803–9
    [Google Scholar]
  172. Shariff AF, Willard AK, Andersen T, Norenzayan A 2016. Religious priming: a meta-analysis with a focus on prosociality. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 20:127–48
    [Google Scholar]
  173. Sheehy-Skeffington J, Thomsen L. 2020. Egalitarianism: psychological and socio-ecological foundations. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 32:146–52
    [Google Scholar]
  174. Sinding Bentzen J. 2019. Acts of God? Religiosity and natural disasters across subnational world districts. Econ. J. 129:6222295–321
    [Google Scholar]
  175. Sinding Bentzen J, Hariri JG, Robinson JA 2019. Power and persistence: the indigenous roots of representative democracy. Econ. J. 129:618678–714
    [Google Scholar]
  176. Slingerland E. 2008. What Science Offers the Humanities: Integrating Body and Culture Cambridge, UK: Cambridge Univ. Press
  177. Slingerland E. 2014. Toward a second wave of consilience in the cognitive scientific study of religion. J. Cogn. Hist. 1:1121–30
    [Google Scholar]
  178. Slingerland E. 2015. Big Gods, historical explanation, and the value of integrating the history of religion into the broader academy. Religion 45:4585–602
    [Google Scholar]
  179. Slingerland E, Atkinson QD, Ember CR, Sheehan O, Muthukrishna M et al. 2020. Coding culture: challenges and recommendations for comparative cultural databases. Evol. Hum. Sci. 2:e29
    [Google Scholar]
  180. Slingerland E, Chudek M. 2011. The prevalence of mind-body dualism in early China. Cogn. Sci. 35:5997–1007
    [Google Scholar]
  181. Slingerland E, Nichols R, Neilbo K, Logan C 2017. The distant reading of religious texts: a “big data” approach to mind-body concepts in early China. J. Am. Acad. Relig. 85:4985–1016
    [Google Scholar]
  182. Slingerland E, Sullivan B. 2017. Durkheim with data: the database of religious history. J. Am. Acad. Relig. 85:2312–47
    [Google Scholar]
  183. Sloman SA, Fernbach P. 2017. The Knowledge Illusion: Why We Never Think Alone New York: Riverhead Books
  184. Smaldino PE, Lukaszewski A, von Rueden C, Gurven M 2019. Niche diversity can explain cross-cultural differences in personality structure. Nat. Hum. Behav. 3:1276–83
    [Google Scholar]
  185. Sosis R, Bressler ER. 2003. Cooperation and commune longevity: a test of the costly signaling theory of religion. Cross-Cult. Res. 37:2211–39
    [Google Scholar]
  186. Sun AXD. 2013. Confucianism as a World Religion: Contested Histories and Contemporary Realities Princeton, NJ: Princeton Univ. Press
  187. Sznycer D, Patrick C. 2020. The origins of criminal law. Nat. Hum. Behav. 4:506–16
    [Google Scholar]
  188. Talhelm T, Zhang X, Oishi S, Shimin C, Duan D et al. 2014. Large-scale psychological differences within China explained by rice versus wheat agriculture. Science 344:6184603–8
    [Google Scholar]
  189. Tamir DI, Thornton MA, Contreras JM, Mitchell JP 2016. Neural evidence that three dimensions organize mental state representation: rationality, social impact, and valence. PNAS 113:1194–99
    [Google Scholar]
  190. Tausczik YR, Pennebaker JW. 2010. The psychological meaning of words: LIWC and computerized text analysis methods. J. Lang. Soc. Psychol. 29:124–54
    [Google Scholar]
  191. Tewksbury JJ, Nabhan GP. 2001. Directed deterrence by capsaicin in chillies. Nature 412:6845403–4
    [Google Scholar]
  192. Thalmayer AG, Toscanelli C, Arnett JJ 2020. The neglected 95% revisited: Is American psychology becoming less American. Am. Psychol. In press. https://doi.org/10.1037/amp0000622
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  193. Thistlethwaite DL, Campbell DT. 1960. Regression-discontinuity analysis: an alternative to the ex post facto experiment. J. Educ. Psychol. 51:6309–17
    [Google Scholar]
  194. Thomson R, Yuki M, Talhelm T, Schug J, Kito M et al. 2018. Relational mobility predicts social behaviors in 39 countries and is tied to historical farming and threat. PNAS 115:297521–26
    [Google Scholar]
  195. Thornton MA, Wolf S, Reilly BJ, Slingerland E, Tamir D 2020. The 3D Mind Model characterizes how people understand mental states across modern and historical cultures Work. Pap., Dartmouth Coll Hanover, NH:
  196. Trivers R. 1972. Parental investment and sexual selection. Sexual Selection and the Descent of Man, 1871–1971 B Campbell 136–79 Chicago: Aldine
    [Google Scholar]
  197. Turchin P, Currie TE, Whitehouse H, François P, Feeney K et al. 2018. Quantitative historical analysis uncovers a single dimension of complexity that structures global variation in human social organization. PNAS 115:2E144–51
    [Google Scholar]
  198. Uchiyama R, Spicer R, Muthukrishna M 2020. Cultural evolution of genetic heritability. bioRxiv 167676. https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.06.23.167676
    [Crossref]
  199. Verner J, Willson MF. 1966. The influence of habitats on mating systems of North American passerine birds. Ecology 47:1143–47
    [Google Scholar]
  200. Watts J, Greenhill SJ, Atkinson QD, Currie TE, Bulbulia J, Gray RD 2015a. Broad supernatural punishment but not moralizing high gods precede the evolution of political complexity in Austronesia. Proc. R. Soc. B 282:180420142556
    [Google Scholar]
  201. Watts J, Sheehan O, Greenhill SJ, Gomes-Ng S, Atkinson QD et al. 2015b. Pulotu: database of Austronesian supernatural beliefs and practices. PLOS ONE 10:9 1–17.
    [Google Scholar]
  202. White CJM, Kelly JM, Shariff AF, Norenzayan A 2019. Supernatural norm enforcement: thinking about karma and God reduces selfishness among believers. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 84:103797
    [Google Scholar]
  203. Whitehouse H, François P, Savage PE, Currie TE, Feeney KC et al. 2019. Complex societies precede moralizing gods throughout world history. Nature 568:226–29
    [Google Scholar]
  204. Wierzbicka A. 2006. On folk conceptions of mind, agency and morality. J. Cogn. Cult. 6:1–2165–79
    [Google Scholar]
  205. Wilson EO. 1998. Consilience: The Unity of Knowledge New York: Knopf
  206. Winkler M. 2020. Shocks, norms and cooperation Res. Pap., Harvard Univ Cambridge, MA:
  207. Wood W, Eagly AH. 2012. Biosocial construction of sex differences and similarities in behavior. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology 46 M Zabba, J Olson 55–123 Amsterdam: Elsevier
    [Google Scholar]
  208. Wright R. 2009. The Evolution of God Boston: Little, Brown and Co.
  209. Xiang C. 2010. [The “heart as ruler” metaphor: a cognitive account].. Yuyan Jiaoxue yu Yanjiu 1:80–87
    [Google Scholar]
  210. Xygalatas D. 2013. Effects of religious setting on cooperative behavior: a case study from Mauritius. Relig. Brain Behav. 3:291–102
    [Google Scholar]
  211. Xygalatas D, Mitkidis P, Fischer R, Reddish P, Skewes J et al. 2013. Extreme rituals promote prosociality. Psychol. Sci. 24:81602–5
    [Google Scholar]
  212. Yi X, Liang Y, Huerta-Sanchez E, Jin X, Cuo ZXP et al. 2010. Sequencing of 50 human exomes reveals adaptation to high altitude. Science 329:598775–78
    [Google Scholar]
  213. Yilmaz O, Bahçekapili HG. 2016. Supernatural and secular monitors promote human cooperation only if they remind of punishment. Evol. Hum. Behav. 37:179–84
    [Google Scholar]
  214. Yu N. 2007. Heart and cognition in ancient Chinese philosophy. J. Cogn. Cult. 7:1–227–47
    [Google Scholar]
  215. Zhao Y, Karypis G, Fayyad U 2005. Hierarchical clustering algorithms for document datasets. Data Min. Knowl. Discov. 10:2141–68
    [Google Scholar]
/content/journals/10.1146/annurev-psych-082820-111436
Loading
/content/journals/10.1146/annurev-psych-082820-111436
Loading

Data & Media loading...

  • Article Type: Review Article
This is a required field
Please enter a valid email address
Approval was a Success
Invalid data
An Error Occurred
Approval was partially successful, following selected items could not be processed due to error