1932

Abstract

To explain trade-offs in choice, researchers have proposed myriad phenomena and decision rules, each paired with separate theories and idiosyncratic vocabularies. Yet most choice problems are ultimately resolved with one of just two types of solutions: mixed or extreme. For example, people adopt mixed solutions for resolving trade-offs when they allow exercising to license indulgence afterward (balancing between goals), read different literary genres (variety seeking), and order medium-sized coffees (the compromise effect). By contrast, when people adopt extreme solutions for resolving these exact same trade-offs, they exhibit highlighting, consistency seeking, and compromise avoidance, respectively. Our review of the choice literature first illustrates how many seemingly unrelated phenomena actually share the same underlying psychology. We then identify variables that promote one solution versus the other. These variables, in turn, systematically influence which of opposite choice effects arise (e.g., highlighting versus balancing). Finally, we demonstrate how several mistakes people purport to make can potentially instead be reinterpreted as mixed solutions for resolving trade-offs. We conclude with guidance for distinguishing mistakes from mixed solutions.

Loading

Article metrics loading...

/content/journals/10.1146/annurev-psych-072420-125709
2021-01-04
2024-03-28
Loading full text...

Full text loading...

/deliver/fulltext/psych/72/1/annurev-psych-072420-125709.html?itemId=/content/journals/10.1146/annurev-psych-072420-125709&mimeType=html&fmt=ahah

Literature Cited

  1. Aknin LB, Dunn EW, Proulx J, Lok I, Norton MI 2020. Does spending money on others promote happiness? A registered replication report. J. Pers. Soc. Pyschol. 119:2e15–26
    [Google Scholar]
  2. Aronson E. 1999. The power of self-persuasion. Am. Pyschol. 54:11875–84
    [Google Scholar]
  3. Bartels DM, Urminsky O. 2011. On intertemporal selfishness: how the perceived instability of identity underlies impatient consumption. J. Consum. Res. 38:1182–98
    [Google Scholar]
  4. Baumeister RF, Vohs KD, Tice DM 2007. The strength model of self-control. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 16:6351–55
    [Google Scholar]
  5. Baumgartner H, Steenkamp JB. 1996. Exploratory consumer buying behavior: conceptualization and measurement. Int. J. Res. Mark. 13:2121–37
    [Google Scholar]
  6. Bem DJ. 1972. Self-perception theory. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology 6 L Berkowitz 1–62 New York: Academic
    [Google Scholar]
  7. Benartzi S, Beshears J, Milkman KL, Sunstein CR, Thaler RH et al. 2017. Should governments invest more in nudging. Psychol. Sci. 28:81041–55
    [Google Scholar]
  8. Benartzi S, Thaler RH. 2001. Naive diversification strategies in defined contribution saving plans. Am. Econ. Rev. 91:179–98
    [Google Scholar]
  9. Bonezzi A, Chernev A, Brough A 2012. When two is better than one: polarization and compromise in unrestricted choice. NA - Advances in Consumer Research 40 Z Gürhan-Canli, C Otnes, R Zhu 266–70 Duluth, MN: Assoc. Consum. Res.
    [Google Scholar]
  10. Bonnefon JF, Shariff A, Rahwan I 2016. The social dilemma of autonomous vehicles. Science 352:62931573–76
    [Google Scholar]
  11. Brooks AW, Gino F, Schweitzer ME 2015. Smart people ask for (my) advice: seeking advice boosts perceptions of competence. Manag. Sci. 61:61421–35
    [Google Scholar]
  12. Brown AL, Lahey JN. 2015. Small victories: creating intrinsic motivation in task completion and debt repayment. J. Mark. Res. 52:6768–83
    [Google Scholar]
  13. Brucks M. 1985. The effects of product class knowledge on information search behavior. J. Consum. Res. 12:11–6
    [Google Scholar]
  14. Carmon Z, Simonson I. 1998. Price-quality trade-offs in choice versus matching: new insights into the prominence effect. J. Consum. Psychol. 7:4323–43
    [Google Scholar]
  15. Carter TJ, Gilovich T. 2010. The relative relativity of material and experiential purchases. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 98:1146–59
    [Google Scholar]
  16. Chan C, Mogilner C. 2017. Experiential gifts foster stronger social relationships than material gifts. J. Consum. Res. 43:6913–31
    [Google Scholar]
  17. Chang HH, Hung IW. 2018. Mirror, mirror on the retail wall: Self-focused attention promotes reliance on feelings in consumer decisions. J. Mark. Res. 55:4586–99
    [Google Scholar]
  18. Chang HH, Pham MT. 2018. Affective boundaries of scope insensitivity. J. Consum. Res. 45:2403–28
    [Google Scholar]
  19. Cheng YH, Chang SS, Chuang SC, Yu MW 2012. The impact of purchase quantity on the compromise effect: the balance heuristic. Judgm. Decis. Mak. 7:4499–512
    [Google Scholar]
  20. Chernev A. 2005. Context effects without a context: attribute balance as a reason for choice. J. Consum. Res. 32:2213–23
    [Google Scholar]
  21. Chernev A, Böckenholt U, Goodman J 2015. Choice overload: a conceptual review and meta-analysis. J. Consum. Psychol. 25:2333–58
    [Google Scholar]
  22. Ching AT, Erdem T, Keane MP 2013. Learning models: an assessment of progress, challenges, and new developments. Mark. Sci. 32:6913–38
    [Google Scholar]
  23. Chugani SK, Irwin JR, Redden JP 2015. Happily ever after: the effect of identity-consistency on product satiation. J. Consum. Res. 42:4564–77
    [Google Scholar]
  24. Cialdini RB, Trost MR, Newsom JT 1995. Preference for consistency: the development of a valid measure and the discovery of surprising behavioral implications. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 69:2318–29
    [Google Scholar]
  25. Cohn A, Fehr E, Maréchal MA 2014. Business culture and dishonesty in the banking industry. Nature 516:752986–89
    [Google Scholar]
  26. Conway P, Peetz J. 2012. When does feeling moral actually make you a better person? Conceptual abstraction moderates whether past moral deeds motivate consistency or compensatory behavior. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 38:7907–19
    [Google Scholar]
  27. Cornelissen G, Bashshur MR, Rode J, Le Menestrel M 2013. Rules or consequences? The role of ethical mind-sets in moral dynamics. Psychol. Sci. 24:4482–88
    [Google Scholar]
  28. Dai H, Milkman KL, Hofmann DA, Staats BR 2015. The impact of time at work and time off from work on rule compliance: the case of hand hygiene in health care. J. Appl. Psychol. 100:3846–62
    [Google Scholar]
  29. Dai H, Milkman KL, Riis J 2014. The fresh start effect: Temporal landmarks motivate aspirational behavior. Manag. Sci. 60:102563–82
    [Google Scholar]
  30. Dai H, Zhang DJ. 2019. Prosocial goal pursuit in crowdfunding: evidence from Kickstarter. J. Mark. Res. 56:3498–517
    [Google Scholar]
  31. Dai X, Fishbach A. 2013. When waiting to choose increases patience. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 121:2256–66
    [Google Scholar]
  32. Desvousges WH, Johnson FR, Dunford RW, Wilson KN, Boyle KJ 1993. Measuring natural resource damages with contingent valuation. Contingent Valuation: A Critical Assessment J Hausman 91–164 Bingley, UK: Emerald
    [Google Scholar]
  33. Dhar R, Gorlin M. 2013. A dual-system framework to understand preference construction processes in choice. J. Consum. Psychol. 23:4528–42
    [Google Scholar]
  34. Dhar R, Nowlis SM, Sherman SJ 2000. Trying hard or hardly trying: an analysis of context effects in choice. J. Consum. Pyschol. 9:4189–200
    [Google Scholar]
  35. Dhar R, Wertenbroch K. 2000. Consumer choice between hedonic and utilitarian goods. J. Mark. Res. 37:160–71
    [Google Scholar]
  36. Dunn EW, Aknin LB, Norton MI 2008. Spending money on others promotes happiness. Science 319:58701687–88
    [Google Scholar]
  37. Dunn EW, Aknin LB, Norton MI 2014. Prosocial spending and happiness: Using money to benefit others pays off. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 23:141–47
    [Google Scholar]
  38. Ersner-Hershfield H, Garton MT, Ballard K, Samanez-Larkin GR, Knutson B 2009. Don't stop thinking about tomorrow: Individual differences in future self-continuity account for saving. Judgm. Decis. Mak. 4:4280–86
    [Google Scholar]
  39. Eskreis-Winkler L, Fishbach A, Duckworth AL 2018. Dear Abby: Should I give advice or receive it. Psychol. Sci. 29:111797–806
    [Google Scholar]
  40. Etkin J. 2016. The hidden cost of personal quantification. J. Consum. Res. 42:6967–84
    [Google Scholar]
  41. Etkin J, Mogilner C. 2016. Does variety among activities increase happiness. J. Consum. Res. 43:2210–29
    [Google Scholar]
  42. Evers ER, Inbar Y, Zeelenberg M 2014. Set-fit effects in choice. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 143:2504–9
    [Google Scholar]
  43. Falk A, Graeber T. 2020. Delayed negative effects of prosocial spending on happiness. PNAS 117:126463–68
    [Google Scholar]
  44. Festinger L. 1957. A Theory of Cognitive Dissonance Stanford, CA: Stanford Univ. Press
  45. Festjens A, Janiszewski C. 2015. The value of time. J. Consum. Res. 42:2178–95
    [Google Scholar]
  46. Finkelstein SR, Fishbach A. 2011. Tell me what I did wrong: Experts seek and respond to negative feedback. J. Consum. Res. 39:122–38
    [Google Scholar]
  47. Fischhoff B, Broomell SB. 2020. Judgment and decision making. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 71:331–55
    [Google Scholar]
  48. Fishbach A, Dhar R. 2005. Goals as excuses or guides: the liberating effect of perceived goal progress on choice. J. Consum. Res. 32:3370–77
    [Google Scholar]
  49. Fishbach A, Dhar R, Zhang Y 2006. Subgoals as substitutes or complements: the role of goal accessibility. J. Pers. Soc. Pyschol. 91:2232–42
    [Google Scholar]
  50. Fishbach A, Friedman RS, Kruglanski AW 2003. Leading us not into temptation: momentary allurements elicit overriding goal activation. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 84:2296–309
    [Google Scholar]
  51. Fishbach A, Ratner RK, Zhang Y 2011. Inherently loyal or easily bored? Nonconscious activation of consistency versus variety‐seeking behavior. J. Consum. Psychol. 21:138–48
    [Google Scholar]
  52. Fishbach A, Shaddy F. 2016. When choices substitute for versus reinforce each other. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 10:39–43
    [Google Scholar]
  53. Fishbach A, Zhang Y. 2008. Together or apart: when goals and temptations complement versus compete. J. Pers. Soc. Pyschol. 94:4547–59
    [Google Scholar]
  54. Fishbach A, Zhang Y, Koo M 2009. The dynamics of self-regulation. Eur. Rev. Soc. Psychol. 20:1315–44
    [Google Scholar]
  55. Gal D, McShane BB. 2012. Can small victories help win the war? Evidence from consumer debt management. J. Mark. Res. 49:4487–501
    [Google Scholar]
  56. Galak J, Kruger J, Loewenstein G 2013. Slow down! Insensitivity to rate of consumption leads to avoidable satiation. J. Consum. Res. 39:5993–1009
    [Google Scholar]
  57. Galla BM, Duckworth AL. 2015. More than resisting temptation: Beneficial habits mediate the relationship between self-control and positive life outcomes. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 109:3508–25
    [Google Scholar]
  58. Gallo I, Sood S, Mann TC, Gilovich T 2017. The heart and the head: on choosing experiences intuitively and possessions deliberatively. J. Behav. Decis. Mak. 30:3754–68
    [Google Scholar]
  59. Gilovich T, Gallo I. 2020. Consumers’ pursuit of material and experiential purchases: a review. Consum. Psychol. Rev. 3:120–33
    [Google Scholar]
  60. Goenka S, Thomas M. 2020. The malleable morality of conspicuous consumption. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 118:3562–83
    [Google Scholar]
  61. Goodman JK, Malkoc SA, Rosenboim M 2019. The material-experiential asymmetry in discounting: when experiential purchases lead to more impatience. J. Consum. Res. 46:4671–88
    [Google Scholar]
  62. Greene J, Baron J. 2001. Intuitions about declining marginal utility. J. Behav. Decis. Mak. 14:3243–55
    [Google Scholar]
  63. Gullo K, Berger J, Etkin J, Bollinger B 2018. Does time of day affect variety-seeking. J. Consum. Res. 46:120–35
    [Google Scholar]
  64. Guo L, Trueblood JS, Diederich A 2017. Thinking fast increases framing effects in risky decision making. Psychol. Sci. 28:4530–43
    [Google Scholar]
  65. Hadar L, Sood S. 2014. When knowledge is demotivating: subjective knowledge and choice overload. Psychol. Sci 25:91739–47
    [Google Scholar]
  66. Harbaugh WT, Mayr U, Burghart DR 2007. Neural responses to taxation and voluntary giving reveal motives for charitable donations. Science 316:58311622–25
    [Google Scholar]
  67. Haws KL, Poynor C. 2008. Seize the day! Encouraging indulgence for the hyperopic consumer. J. Consum. Res. 35:4680–91
    [Google Scholar]
  68. Hershfield HE, Shu S, Benartzi S 2020. Temporal reframing and participation in a savings program: a field experiment. Mark. Sci. In press. http://doi.org/10.1287/mksc.2019.1177
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  69. Hofmann W, Baumeister RF, Förster G, Vohs KD 2012. Everyday temptations: an experience sampling study of desire, conflict, and self-control. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 102:61318–35
    [Google Scholar]
  70. Hofmann W, Friese M, Strack F 2009. Impulse and self-control from a dual-systems perspective. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 4:2162–76
    [Google Scholar]
  71. Holyoak KJ, Powell D. 2016. Deontological coherence: a framework for commonsense moral reasoning. Psychol. Bull. 142:111179–203
    [Google Scholar]
  72. Hsee CK. 1996. The evaluability hypothesis: an explanation for preference reversals between joint and separate evaluations of alternatives. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 67:3247–57
    [Google Scholar]
  73. Hsee CK, Rottenstreich Y. 2004. Music, pandas, and muggers: on the affective psychology of value. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 133:123–30
    [Google Scholar]
  74. Hsee CK, Zhang J, Lu ZY, Xu F 2013. Unit asking: a method to boost donations and beyond. Psychol. Sci. 24:91801–8
    [Google Scholar]
  75. Huang Z, Liang Y, Weinberg CB, Gorn GJ 2019. The sleepy consumer and variety seeking. J. Mark. Res. 56:2179–96
    [Google Scholar]
  76. Huh YE, Vosgerau J, Morewedge CK 2016. More similar but less satisfying: comparing preferences for and the efficacy of within- and cross-category substitutes for food. Psychol. Sci. 27:6894–903
    [Google Scholar]
  77. Inzlicht M, Schmeichel BJ. 2012. What is ego depletion? Toward a mechanistic revision of the resource model of self-control. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 7:5450–63
    [Google Scholar]
  78. Jang JM, Yoon SO. 2016. The effect of attribute-based and alternative-based processing on consumer choice in context. Mark. Lett. 27:3511–24
    [Google Scholar]
  79. Jeong HG, Christensen K, Drolet A 2016. The short-lived benefits of variety seeking among the chronically indecisive. J. Exp. Psychol. Appl. 22:4423–35
    [Google Scholar]
  80. Jhang JH, Lynch JG Jr 2015. Pardon the interruption: goal proximity, perceived spare time, and impatience. J. Consum. Res. 41:51267–83
    [Google Scholar]
  81. Jia L, Hirt ER, Fishbach A 2019. Protecting an important goal: when prior self-control increases motivation for active goal pursuit. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 85:103875
    [Google Scholar]
  82. Jonas E, Frey D. 2003. Information search and presentation in advisor-client interactions. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 91:2154–68
    [Google Scholar]
  83. Kahneman D, Frederick S. 2002. Representativeness revisited: attribute substitution in intuitive judgment. Heuristics and Biases: The Psychology of Intuitive Judgment T Gilovich, D Kahneman 49–81 New York: Cambridge Univ. Press
    [Google Scholar]
  84. Kahneman D, Fredrickson BL, Schreiber CA, Redelmeier DA 1993. When more pain is preferred to less: adding a better end. Psychol. Sci. 4:6401–5
    [Google Scholar]
  85. Kahneman D, Ritov I, Schkade D, Sherman SJ, Varian HR 1999. Economic preferences or attitude expressions? An analysis of dollar responses to public issues. Elicitation of Preferences B Fischoff, CF Manski 203–42 New York: Springer
    [Google Scholar]
  86. Keysar B, Hayakawa SL, An SG 2012. The foreign-language effect: Thinking in a foreign tongue reduces decision biases. Psychol. Sci. 23:6661–68
    [Google Scholar]
  87. Khan U, Dhar R. 2007. Where there is a way, is there a will? The effect of future choices on self-control. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 136:2277–88
    [Google Scholar]
  88. Khan U, Zhu M, Kalra A 2011. When trade-offs matter: the effect of choice construal on context effects. J. Mark. Res. 48:162–71
    [Google Scholar]
  89. Kivetz R, Keinan A. 2006. Repenting hyperopia: an analysis of self-control regrets. J. Consum. Res. 33:2273–82
    [Google Scholar]
  90. Kivetz R, Netzer O, Srinivasan V 2004. Alternative models for capturing the compromise effect. J. Mark. Res. 41:3237–57
    [Google Scholar]
  91. Kivetz R, Simonson I. 2002. Earning the right to indulge: effort as a determinant of customer preferences toward frequency program rewards. J. Mark. Res. 39:2155–70
    [Google Scholar]
  92. Kivetz R, Urminsky O, Zheng Y 2006. The goal-gradient hypothesis resurrected: purchase acceleration, illusionary goal progress, and customer retention. J. Mark. Res. 43:139–58
    [Google Scholar]
  93. Klein N, O'Brien E. 2017. The power and limits of personal change: when a bad past does (and does not) inspire in the present. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 113:2210–29
    [Google Scholar]
  94. Koffka K. 1935. Principles of Gestalt Psychology London: Routledge
  95. Koo M, Fishbach A. 2012. The small-area hypothesis: effects of progress monitoring on goal adherence. J. Consum. Res. 39:3493–509
    [Google Scholar]
  96. Kouchaki M. 2011. Vicarious moral licensing: the influence of others’ past moral actions on moral behavior. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 101:4702–15
    [Google Scholar]
  97. Lee JC, Hall DL, Wood W 2018. Experiential or material purchases? Social class determines purchase happiness. Psychol. Sci. 29:71031–39
    [Google Scholar]
  98. Lee L, Amir O, Ariely D 2009. In search of Homo Economicus: cognitive noise and the role of emotion in preference consistency. J. Consum. Res. 36:2173–87
    [Google Scholar]
  99. Lichters M, Brunnlieb C, Nave G, Sarstedt M, Vogt B 2016. The influence of serotonin deficiency on choice deferral and the compromise effect. J. Mark. Res. 53:2183–98
    [Google Scholar]
  100. Liu PJ, Haws KL, Lamberton C, Campbell TH, Fitzsimons GJ 2015. Vice-virtue bundles. Manag. Sci. 61:1204–28
    [Google Scholar]
  101. Loewenstein GF, Prelec D. 1993. Preferences for sequences of outcomes. Psychol. Rev. 100:191–108
    [Google Scholar]
  102. Love E, Staton M, Rotman JD 2016. Loyalty as a matter of principle: the influence of standards of judgment on customer loyalty. Mark. Lett. 27:4661–74
    [Google Scholar]
  103. Luce MF. 1998. Choosing to avoid: coping with negatively emotion-laden consumer decisions. J. Consum. Res. 24:4409–33
    [Google Scholar]
  104. Magen E, Gross JJ. 2007. Harnessing the need for immediate gratification: Cognitive reconstrual modulates the reward value of temptations. Emotion 7:2415–28
    [Google Scholar]
  105. Malkoc SA, Zauberman G, Ulu C 2005. Consuming now or later? The interactive effect of timing and attribute alignability. Psychol. Sci. 16:5411–17
    [Google Scholar]
  106. Mao W. 2016. When one desires too much of a good thing: the compromise effect under maximizing tendencies. J. Consum. Psychol. 26:166–80
    [Google Scholar]
  107. May F, Irmak C. 2014. Licensing indulgence in the present by distorting memories of past behavior. J. Consum. Res. 41:3624–41
    [Google Scholar]
  108. Mazar N, Zhong CB. 2010. Do green products make us better people. Psychol. Sci. 21:4494–98
    [Google Scholar]
  109. McAlister L, Pessemier E. 1982. Variety seeking behavior: an interdisciplinary review. J. Consum. Res. 9:3311–22
    [Google Scholar]
  110. McGraw AP, Davis DF, Scott SE, Tetlock PE 2016. The price of not putting a price on love. Judgm. Decis. Mak. 11:140–47
    [Google Scholar]
  111. McGraw AP, Schwartz JA, Tetlock PE 2012. From the commercial to the communal: reframing taboo trade-offs in religious and pharmaceutical marketing. J. Consum. Res. 39:1157–73
    [Google Scholar]
  112. McGraw AP, Tetlock PE. 2005. Taboo trade‐offs, relational framing, and the acceptability of exchanges. J. Consum. Psychol. 15:12–15
    [Google Scholar]
  113. McGraw AP, Warren C. 2010. Benign violations: making immoral behavior funny. Psychol. Sci. 21:81141–49
    [Google Scholar]
  114. McGuire JT, Kable JW. 2013. Rational temporal predictions can underlie apparent failures to delay gratification. Psychol Rev 120:2395–410
    [Google Scholar]
  115. Michaelson LE, Munakata Y. 2016. Trust matters: Seeing how an adult treats another person influences preschoolers’ willingness to delay gratification. Dev. Sci. 19:61011–19
    [Google Scholar]
  116. Milkman KL, Minson JA, Volpp KG 2014. Holding the Hunger Games hostage at the gym: an evaluation of temptation bundling. Manag. Sci. 60:2283–99
    [Google Scholar]
  117. Mischel W, Shoda Y, Rodriguez MI 1989. Delay of gratification in children. Science 244:4907933–38
    [Google Scholar]
  118. Mittelman M, Andrade EB, Chattopadhyay A, Brendl CM 2014. The offer framing effect: choosing single versus bundled offerings affects variety seeking. J. Consum. Res. 41:4953–64
    [Google Scholar]
  119. Monin B, Miller DT. 2001. Moral credentials and the expression of prejudice. J. Pers. Soc. Pyschol. 81:133–43
    [Google Scholar]
  120. Morewedge CK, Kassam KS, Hsee CK, Caruso EM 2009. Duration sensitivity depends on stimulus familiarity. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 138:2177–86
    [Google Scholar]
  121. Mullen E, Monin B. 2016. Consistency versus licensing effects of past moral behavior. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 67:363–85
    [Google Scholar]
  122. Neal DT, Wood W, Drolet A 2013. How do people adhere to goals when willpower is low? The profits (and pitfalls) of strong habits. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 104:6959–75
    [Google Scholar]
  123. Newman KP, Brucks M. 2018. The influence of corporate social responsibility efforts on the moral behavior of high self‐brand overlap consumers. J. Consum. Psychol. 28:2253–71
    [Google Scholar]
  124. Nowlis SM, Simonson I. 1997. Attribute-task compatibility as a determinant of consumer preference reversals. J. Mark. Res. 34:2205–18
    [Google Scholar]
  125. O'Brien E, Kassirer S. 2019. People are slow to adapt to the warm glow of giving. Psychol. Sci. 30:2193–204
    [Google Scholar]
  126. O'Donnell M, Evers ER. 2018. Preference reversals in willingness to pay and choice. J. Consum. Res. 45:61315–30
    [Google Scholar]
  127. Orehek E, Mauro R, Kruglanski AW, van der Bles AM 2012. Prioritizing association strength versus value: the influence of self-regulatory modes on means evaluation in single goal and multigoal contexts. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 102:122–31
    [Google Scholar]
  128. Paolacci G, Burson KA, Rick SI 2011. The intermediate alternative effect: Considering a small tradeoff increases subsequent willingness to make large tradeoffs. J. Consum. Psychol. 21:4384–92
    [Google Scholar]
  129. Park J, Hedgcock WM. 2016. Thinking concretely or abstractly: the influence of fit between goal progress and goal construal on subsequent self-regulation. J. Consum. Psychol. 26:3395–409
    [Google Scholar]
  130. Parker JR, Lehmann DR. 2014. How and when grouping low-calorie options reduces the benefits of providing dish-specific calorie information. J. Consum. Res. 41:1213–35
    [Google Scholar]
  131. Payne JW, Bettman JR, Johnson EJ 1988. Adaptive strategy selection in decision making. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 14:3534–52
    [Google Scholar]
  132. Payne JW, Bettman JR, Johnson EJ 1992. Behavioral decision research: a constructive processing perspective. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 43:187–131
    [Google Scholar]
  133. Pettibone JC. 2012. Testing the effect of time pressure on asymmetric dominance and compromise decoys in choice. Judgm. Decis. Mak. 7:4513–23
    [Google Scholar]
  134. Popkowski Leszczyc PTL, Häubl G 2010. To bundle or not to bundle: determinants of the profitability of multi-item auctions. J. Mark. 74:4110–24
    [Google Scholar]
  135. Rahinel R, Redden JP. 2013. Brands as product coordinators: Matching brands make joint consumption experiences more enjoyable. J. Consum. Res. 39:61290–99
    [Google Scholar]
  136. Ratner RK, Kahn BE. 2002. The impact of private versus public consumption on variety-seeking behavior. J. Consum. Res. 29:2246–57
    [Google Scholar]
  137. Read D, Loewenstein G. 1995. Diversification bias: explaining the discrepancy in variety seeking between combined and separated choices. J. Exp. Psychol. Appl. 1:134–49
    [Google Scholar]
  138. Reed A II, Kay A, Finnel S, Aquino K, Levy E 2016. I don't want the money, I just want your time: how moral identity overcomes the aversion to giving time to prosocial causes. J. Pers. Soc. Pyschol 110:3435–57
    [Google Scholar]
  139. Rifkin JR, Etkin J. 2019. Variety in self-expression undermines self-continuity. J. Consum. Res. 46:4725–49
    [Google Scholar]
  140. Roberts A, Shaddy F, Fishbach A 2020. Love is patient: People are more willing to wait for something they like Work. Pap., Univ. Chicago Chicago, IL:
  141. Roth AE. 2007. Repugnance as a constraint on markets. J. Econ. Perspect. 21:337–58
    [Google Scholar]
  142. Salisbury LC, Feinberg FM. 2008. Future preference uncertainty and diversification: the role of temporal stochastic inflation. J. Consum. Res. 35:2349–59
    [Google Scholar]
  143. Salisbury LC, Feinberg FM. 2012. All things considered? The role of choice set formation in diversification. J. Mark. Res. 49:3320–35
    [Google Scholar]
  144. Sarantopoulos P, Theotokis A, Pramatari K, Roggeveen AL 2019. The impact of a complement-based assortment organization on purchases. J. Mark. Res. 56:3459–78
    [Google Scholar]
  145. Savary J, Li CX, Newman GE 2020. Exalted purchases or tainted donations? Self‐signaling and the evaluation of charitable incentives. J. Consum. Psychol. In press. https://doi.org/10.1002/jcpy.1157
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  146. Schley DR, De Langhe B, Long AR 2020. System 1 is not scope insensitive: a new, dual-process account of subjective value. J. Consum. Res. In press https://doi.org/10.1093/jcr/ucaa015
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  147. Schley DR, Peters E. 2014. Assessing “economic value” symbolic-number mappings predict risky and riskless valuations. Psychol. Sci. 25:3753–61
    [Google Scholar]
  148. Schrift RY, Parker JR. 2014. Staying the course: the option of doing nothing and its impact on postchoice persistence. Psychol. Sci. 25:3772–80
    [Google Scholar]
  149. Schwartz B, Ward A, Monterosso J, Lyubomirsky S, White K, Lehman DR 2002. Maximizing versus satisficing: Happiness is a matter of choice. J. Pers. Soc. Pyschol. 83:51178–97
    [Google Scholar]
  150. Scott SE, Inbar Y, Rozin P 2016. Evidence for absolute moral opposition to genetically modified food in the United States. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 11:3315–24
    [Google Scholar]
  151. Sela A, Hadar L, Morgan S, Maimaran M 2019. Variety‐seeking and perceived expertise. J. Consum Psychol. 29:4671–679
    [Google Scholar]
  152. Sevilla J, Lu J, Kahn BE 2019. Variety seeking, satiation, and maximizing enjoyment over time. J. Consum. Psychol. 29:189–103
    [Google Scholar]
  153. Shaddy F, Fishbach A. 2017. Seller beware: how bundling affects valuation. J. Mark. Res. 54:5737–51
    [Google Scholar]
  154. Shaddy F, Fishbach A. 2018. Eyes on the prize: the preference to invest resources in goals over means. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 115:4624–37
    [Google Scholar]
  155. Shaddy F, Fishbach A, Simonson I 2020. Why invoking the self attenuates variety seeking, the compromise effect, and balancing Work. Pap., Univ. Calif Los Angeles:
  156. Shaddy F, Lee L. 2020. Price promotions cause impatience. J. Mark. Res. 57:1118–33
    [Google Scholar]
  157. Shafir E, LeBoeuf RA. 2002. Rationality. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 53:1491–517
    [Google Scholar]
  158. Shallow C, Iliev R, Medin D 2011. Trolley problems in context. Judgm. Decis. Mak. 6:7593–601
    [Google Scholar]
  159. Sheng S, Parker AM, Nakamoto K 2005. Understanding the mechanism and determinants of compromise effects. Psychol. Mark. 22:7591–609
    [Google Scholar]
  160. Simonson I. 1989. Choice based on reasons: the case of attraction and compromise effects. J. Consum. Res. 16:2158–74
    [Google Scholar]
  161. Simonson I. 1990. The effect of purchase quantity and timing on variety-seeking behavior. J. Mark. Res. 27:2150–62
    [Google Scholar]
  162. Simonson I, Carmon Z, Dhar R, Drolet A, Nowlis SM 2001. Consumer research: in search of identity. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 52:1249–75
    [Google Scholar]
  163. Simonson I, Tversky A. 1992. Choice in context: tradeoff contrast and extremeness aversion. J. Mark. Res. 29:3281–95
    [Google Scholar]
  164. Simonson I, Winer RS. 1992. The influence of purchase quantity and display format on consumer preference for variety. J. Consum. Res. 19:1133–38
    [Google Scholar]
  165. Tetlock PE, Kristel OV, Elson SB, Green MC, Lerner JS 2000. The psychology of the unthinkable: taboo trade-offs, forbidden base rates, and heretical counterfactuals. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 78:5853–70
    [Google Scholar]
  166. Thaler RH. 1999. Mental accounting matters. J. Behav. Decis. Mak. 12:3183–206
    [Google Scholar]
  167. Trueblood JS, Brown SD, Heathcote A, Busemeyer JR 2013. Not just for consumers: Context effects are fundamental to decision making. Psychol. Sci. 24:6901–8
    [Google Scholar]
  168. Tully SM, Hershfield HE, Meyvis T 2015. Seeking lasting enjoyment with limited money: financial constraints increase preference for material goods over experiences. J. Consum. Res. 42:159–75
    [Google Scholar]
  169. Van Boven L, Gilovich T 2003. To do or to have? That is the question. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 85:61193–202
    [Google Scholar]
  170. Vosgerau J, Scopelliti I, Huh YE 2019. Exerting self‐control ≠ sacrificing pleasure. J. Consum. Psychol. 30:1181–200
    [Google Scholar]
  171. Wadhwa M, Kim JC. 2015. Can a near win kindle motivation? The impact of nearly winning on motivation for unrelated rewards. Psychol. Sci. 26:6701–8
    [Google Scholar]
  172. Warren C, McGraw AP. 2016. Differentiating what is humorous from what is not. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 110:3407–30
    [Google Scholar]
  173. Weaver K, Garcia SM, Schwarz N 2012. The presenter's paradox. J. Consum. Res. 39:3445–60
    [Google Scholar]
  174. Weidman AC, Dunn EW. 2016. The unsung benefits of material things: Material purchases provide more frequent momentary happiness than experiential purchases. Soc. Psychol. Pers. Sci. 7:4390–99
    [Google Scholar]
  175. Weingarten E, Goodman JK. 2020. The experiential advantage: a meta-analysis Work. Pap., Ariz. State Univ., Tempe
  176. Whillans AV, Aknin LB, Ross CJ, Chen L, Chen FS 2019. Common variants of the oxytocin receptor gene do not predict the positive mood benefits of prosocial spending. Emotion 20:5734–49
    [Google Scholar]
  177. White K, Simpson B, Argo JJ 2014. The motivating role of dissociative out-groups in encouraging positive consumer behaviors. J. Mark. Res. 51:4433–47
    [Google Scholar]
  178. Yang AX, Urminsky O. 2015. The foresight effect: Local optimism motivates consistency and local pessimism motivates variety. J. Consum. Res. 42:3361–77
    [Google Scholar]
  179. Yang Y, Galak J. 2015. Sentimental value and its influence on hedonic adaptation. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 109:5767–90
    [Google Scholar]
  180. Yoon S, Kim HC. 2016. Keeping the American dream alive: the interactive effect of perceived economic mobility and materialism on impulsive spending. J. Mark. Res. 53:5759–72
    [Google Scholar]
  181. Zeelenberg M. 1999. Anticipated regret, expected feedback and behavioral decision making. J. Behav. Decis. Mak. 12:293–106
    [Google Scholar]
/content/journals/10.1146/annurev-psych-072420-125709
Loading
/content/journals/10.1146/annurev-psych-072420-125709
Loading

Data & Media loading...

  • Article Type: Review Article
This is a required field
Please enter a valid email address
Approval was a Success
Invalid data
An Error Occurred
Approval was partially successful, following selected items could not be processed due to error