1932

Abstract

Executive function (EF) skills are neurocognitive skills that support the reflective, top-down coordination and control of other brain functions, and there is neural and behavioral evidence for a continuum from more “cool” EF skills activated in emotionally neutral contexts to more “hot” EF skills needed for the reversal of motivationally significant tendencies. Difficulties in EF are transdiagnostic indicators of atypical development. A neurodevelopmental model traces the pathway from adverse childhood experiences and stress to disruption of the development of neural systems supporting reflection and EF skills to an increased risk for general features of psychopathology. Research indicates that EF skills can be cultivated through scaffolded training and are a promising target for therapeutic and preventive intervention. Intervention efficacy can be enhanced by mitigating disruptive bottom-up influences such as stress, training both hot and cool EF skills, and adding a reflective, metacognitive component to promote far transfer of trained skills.

Loading

Article metrics loading...

/content/journals/10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-072319-024242
2020-05-07
2024-10-03
Loading full text...

Full text loading...

/deliver/fulltext/clinpsy/16/1/annurev-clinpsy-072319-024242.html?itemId=/content/journals/10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-072319-024242&mimeType=html&fmt=ahah

Literature Cited

  1. Achenbach TM, Edelbrock C, Howell CT 1987. Empirically based assessment of the behavioral/emotional problems of 2- and 3-year-old children. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 15:629–50
    [Google Scholar]
  2. Allan NP, Lonigan CJ. 2014. Exploring dimensionality of effortful control using hot and cool tasks in a sample of preschool children. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 122:33–47
    [Google Scholar]
  3. Archibald SJ, Kerns KA, Mateer CA, Ismay L 2005. Evidence of utilization behavior in children with ADHD. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 11:4367–75
    [Google Scholar]
  4. Badre D, D'Esposito M. 2007. Functional magnetic resonance imaging evidence for a hierarchical organization of the prefrontal cortex. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 19:2082–99
    [Google Scholar]
  5. Bansal S, Robinson BM, Leonard CJ, Hahn B, Luck SJ, Gold JM 2019. Failures in top-down control in schizophrenia revealed by patterns of saccadic eye movements. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 128:415–22
    [Google Scholar]
  6. Beauchaine TP, Cicchetti D. 2019. Emotion dysregulation and emerging psychopathology: a transdiagnostic, transdisciplinary perspective. Dev. Psychopathol. 31:799–804
    [Google Scholar]
  7. Bechara A, Damasio AR, Damasio H, Anderson SW 1994. Insensitivity to future consequences following damage to human prefrontal cortex. Cognition 50:1–37–15
    [Google Scholar]
  8. Bernier A, Carlson SM, Deschênes M, Matte‐Gagné C 2012. Social factors in the development of early executive functioning: a closer look at the caregiving environment. Dev. Sci. 15:112–24
    [Google Scholar]
  9. Bernstein A, Hadash Y, Lichtash Y, Tanay G, Shepherd K, Fresco DM 2015. Decentering and related constructs: a critical review and metacognitive processes model. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 10:5599–617
    [Google Scholar]
  10. Blair C, Raver CC. 2016. Poverty, stress, and brain development: new directions for prevention and intervention. Acad. Pediatr. 16:3 Suppl.S30–36
    [Google Scholar]
  11. Botvinick MM, Braver TS, Barch DM, Carter CS, Cohen JD 2001. Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. Psychol. Rev. 108:3624–52
    [Google Scholar]
  12. Brock LL, Rimm-Kaufman SE, Nathanson L, Grimm KJ 2009. The contributions of ‘hot’ and ‘cool’ executive function to children's academic achievement, learning-related behaviors, and engagement in kindergarten. Early Child. Res. Q. 24:3337–49
    [Google Scholar]
  13. Bronfenbrenner U. 1979. The Ecology of Human Development: Experiments by Nature and Design Cambridge, MA: Harvard Univ. Press
    [Google Scholar]
  14. Bunge SA, Wallis JD. 2008. Perspectives on Rule-Guided Behavior New York: Oxford Univ. Press
    [Google Scholar]
  15. Bunge SA, Zelazo PD. 2006. A brain-based account of the development of rule use in childhood. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 15:118–21
    [Google Scholar]
  16. Buodo G, Sarlo M, Palomba D 2002. Attentional resources measured by reaction times highlight differences within pleasant and unpleasant, high arousing stimuli. Motiv. Emot. 26:2123–38
    [Google Scholar]
  17. Bush G, Luu P, Posner MI 2000. Cognitive and emotional influences in anterior cingulate cortex. Trends Cogn. Sci. 4:215–22
    [Google Scholar]
  18. Butts KA, Weinberg J, Young AH, Phillips AG 2011. Glucocorticoid receptors in the prefrontal cortex regulate stress-evoked dopamine efflux and aspects of executive function. PNAS 108:18459–64
    [Google Scholar]
  19. Carlson SM, Davis AC, Leach JG 2005. Less is more: executive function and symbolic representation in preschool children. Psychol. Sci. 16:8609–16
    [Google Scholar]
  20. Carlson SM, Mandell D, Williams L 2004. Executive function and theory of mind: stability and prediction from ages 2 to 3. Dev. Psychol. 40:1105–22
    [Google Scholar]
  21. Caspi A, Houts RM, Belsky DW, Goldman-Mellor SJ, Harrington HL et al. 2014. The p factor: one general psychopathology factor in the structure of psychiatric disorders. Clin. Psychol. Sci. 2:2119–37
    [Google Scholar]
  22. Castellanos-Ryan N, Brière FN, O'Leary-Barrett M, Banaschewski T, Bokde A et al. 2016. The structure of psychopathology in adolescence and its common personality and cognitive correlates. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 125:81039–52
    [Google Scholar]
  23. Checa P, Fernández-Berrocal P. 2019. Cognitive control and emotional intelligence: effect of the emotional content of the task. Brief reports. Front. Psychol. 10:195
    [Google Scholar]
  24. Christoff K, Gabrieli JD. 2000. The frontopolar cortex and human cognition: evidence for a rostrocaudal hierarchical organization within the human prefrontal cortex. Psychobiology 28:168–86
    [Google Scholar]
  25. Cicchetti D. 1984. The emergence of developmental psychopathology. Child Dev 55:11–7
    [Google Scholar]
  26. Cirino PT, Ahmed Y, Miciak J, Taylor WP, Gerst EH, Barnes MA 2018. A framework for executive function in the late elementary years. Neuropsychology 32:2176–89
    [Google Scholar]
  27. Ciurli P, Bivona U, Barba C, Onder G, Silvestro D et al. 2010. Metacognitive unawareness correlates with executive function impairment after severe traumatic brain injury. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 16:360–68
    [Google Scholar]
  28. Cole MW, Reynolds JR, Power JD, Repovs G, Anticevic A, Braver TS 2013. Multi-task connectivity reveals flexible hubs for adaptive task control. Nat. Neurosci. 16:91348–55
    [Google Scholar]
  29. Conway CC, Raposa EB, Hammen C, Brennan PA 2018. Transdiagnostic pathways from early social stress to psychopathology: a 20-year prospective study. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 59:855–62
    [Google Scholar]
  30. Crone E, Steinbeis N. 2017. Neural perspectives on cognitive control development during childhood and adolescence. Trends Cogn. Sci. 21:205–15
    [Google Scholar]
  31. Cunningham WA, Van Bavel JJ, Johnsen IR 2008. Affective flexibility: evaluative processing goals shape amygdala activity. Psychol. Sci. 19:152–60
    [Google Scholar]
  32. Cunningham WA, Zelazo PD. 2007. Attitudes and evaluation: a social cognitive neuroscience perspective. Trends Cogn. Sci. 11:397–104
    [Google Scholar]
  33. De Brito SA, Viding E, Kumari V, Blackwood N, Hodgins S 2013. Cool and hot executive function impairments in violent offenders with antisocial personality disorder with and without psychopathy. PLOS ONE 8:6e65566
    [Google Scholar]
  34. Diamond A. 2013. Executive functions. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 64:135–68
    [Google Scholar]
  35. Diamond A, Lee K. 2011. Interventions shown to aid executive function development in children 4 to 12 years old. Science 333:959–64
    [Google Scholar]
  36. Duckworth AL, Seligman MEP. 2005. Self-discipline outdoes IQ in predicting academic performance of adolescents. Psychol. Sci. 16:939–44
    [Google Scholar]
  37. Duncan J. 2013. The structure of cognition: attentional episodes in mind and brain. Neuron 80:135–50
    [Google Scholar]
  38. Dunning DL, Griffiths K, Kuyken W, Crane C, Foulkes L et al. 2019. The effects of mindfulness-based interventions on cognition and mental health in children and adolescents: a meta-analysis of randomised controlled trials. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 60:3244–58
    [Google Scholar]
  39. Espinet SD, Anderson JE, Zelazo PD 2013. Reflection training improves executive function in preschool-age children: behavioral and neural effects. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 4:3–15
    [Google Scholar]
  40. Felitti VJ, Anda RF, Nordenberg D, Williamson DF, Spitz AM et al. 1998. Relationship of childhood abuse and household dysfunction to many of the leading causes of death in adults. The Adverse Childhood Experiences (ACE) Study. Am. J. Prev. Med. 14:245–58
    [Google Scholar]
  41. Fonseca RP, Zimmermann N, Cotrena C, Cardoso C, Kristensen CH, Grassi-Oliveira R 2012. Neuropsychological assessment of executive functions in traumatic brain injury: hot and cold components. Psychol. Neurosci. 5:2183–90
    [Google Scholar]
  42. Friedman NP, Miyake A. 2017. Unity and diversity of executive functions: individual differences as a window on cognitive structure. Cortex 86:186–204
    [Google Scholar]
  43. Frye D, Zelazo PD, Palfai T 1995. Theory of mind and rule-based reasoning. Cogn. Dev. 10:483–527
    [Google Scholar]
  44. Geurts HM, Van der Oord S, Crone EA 2006. Hot and cool aspects of cognitive control in children with ADHD: decision making and inhibition. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 34:6811–22
    [Google Scholar]
  45. Gogtay N, Giedd JN, Lusk L, Hayashi KM, Greenstein D et al. 2004. Dynamic mapping of human cortical development during childhood through early adulthood. PNAS 101:8174–79
    [Google Scholar]
  46. Gooch D, Thompson P, Nash HM, Snowling MJ, Hulme C 2016. The development of executive function and language skills in the early school years. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 57:180–87
    [Google Scholar]
  47. Gottlieb G. 1992. Individual Development and Evolution: The Genesis of Novel Behavior New York: Oxford Univ. Press
    [Google Scholar]
  48. Greenwald AG, Banaji MR. 1995. Implicit social cognition: attitudes, self-esteem, and stereotypes. Psychol. Rev. 102:14–27
    [Google Scholar]
  49. Groppe K, Elsner B. 2014. Executive function and food approach behavior in middle childhood. Front. Psychol. 5:477
    [Google Scholar]
  50. Gu S, Satterthwaite TD, Medaglia JD, Yang M, Gur RE et al. 2015. Emergence of system roles in normative neurodevelopment. PNAS 112:13681–86
    [Google Scholar]
  51. Güngör B, Budak E, Taymur I, Zorlu N, Ucgun B et al. 2018. The comparison of risky and ambiguity decision making and cool executive functions between patients with obsessive compulsive disorder and healthy controls. Arch. Clin. Psychiatry 45:5112–18
    [Google Scholar]
  52. Hadley LV, Acluche F, Chevalier N 2020. Encouraging performance monitoring promotes proactive control in children. Dev. Sci. 23:1e12861
    [Google Scholar]
  53. Hanson JL, Chung MK, Avants BB, Shirtcliff EA, Gee JC et al. 2010. Early stress is associated with alterations in the orbitofrontal cortex: a tensor-based morphometry investigation of brain structure and behavioral risk. J. Neurosci. 30:7466–72
    [Google Scholar]
  54. Happaney K, Zelazo PD, Stuss DT 2004. Development of orbitofrontal function: current themes and future directions. Brain Cogn 55:11–10
    [Google Scholar]
  55. Hodel AS, Brumbaugh JE, Morris AR, Thomas KM 2016. Hot executive function following moderate-to-late preterm birth: altered delay discounting at 4 years of age. Dev. Sci. 19:221–34
    [Google Scholar]
  56. Hoppen TH, Chalder T. 2018. Childhood adversity as a transdiagnostic risk factor for affective disorders in adulthood: a systematic review focusing on biopsychosocial moderating and mediating variables. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 65:81–151
    [Google Scholar]
  57. Hostinar CE, Sullivan RM, Gunnar MR 2014. Psychobiological mechanisms underlying the social buffering of the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenocortical axis: a review of animal models and human studies across development. Psychol. Bull. 140:1256–82
    [Google Scholar]
  58. Humphreys KL, Watts EL, Dennis EL, King LS, Thompson PM, Gotlib IH 2019. Stressful life events, ADHD symptoms, and brain structure in early adolescence. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 47:421–32
    [Google Scholar]
  59. Hybel KA, Mortensen EL, Lambek R, Thastum M, Thomsen PH 2017. Cool and hot aspects of executive function in childhood obsessive-compulsive disorder. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 45:1195–205
    [Google Scholar]
  60. Insel TR, Cuthbert BN, Garvey M, Heinssen R, Pine DS et al. 2010. Research Domain Criteria (RDoC): toward a new classification framework for research on mental disorders. Am. J. Psychiatry 167:7748–51
    [Google Scholar]
  61. Jaeggi SM, Buschkuehl M, Jonides J, Perrig WJ 2008. Improving fluid intelligence with training on working memory. PNAS 105:196829–33
    [Google Scholar]
  62. Johnson AC. 2015. Developmental pathways to attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and disruptive behavior disorders: investigating the impact of the stress response on executive functioning. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 36:1–12
    [Google Scholar]
  63. Johnson MH. 2011. Interactive specialization: a domain-general framework for human functional brain development. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 1:17–12
    [Google Scholar]
  64. Kabat-Zinn J. 1990. Full Catastrophe Living: Using the Wisdom of Your Body and Mind to Face Stress, Pain and Illness New York: Delacorte
    [Google Scholar]
  65. Kaller MS, Lazari A, Blanco-Duque C, Sampaio-Baptista C, Johansen-Berg H 2017. Myelin plasticity and behaviour: connecting the dots. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 47:86–92
    [Google Scholar]
  66. Keyes KM, Eaton NR, Krueger RF, McLaughlin KA, Wall MM et al. 2012. Childhood maltreatment and the structure of common psychiatric disorders. Br. J. Psychiatry 200:107–15
    [Google Scholar]
  67. Kolb B, Harker L, de Melo S, Gibb R 2017. Stress and pre-frontal cortical plasticity in the developing brain. Cogn. Dev. 42:15–26
    [Google Scholar]
  68. Koopman-Verhoeff ME, Serdarevic F, Desana K, Bodrij FF, Mileva‐Seitz VR et al. 2019. Preschool family irregularity and the development of sleep problems in childhood: a longitudinal study. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 60:857–65
    [Google Scholar]
  69. Korucu I, Rolan E, Napoli AR, Purpura DJ, Schmitt SA 2019. Development of the Home Executive Function Environment (HEFE) scale: assessing its relation to preschoolers’ executive function. Early Child. Res. Q. 47:9–19
    [Google Scholar]
  70. Kouklari E-C, Tsermentseli S, Monks CP 2019. Developmental trends of hot and cool executive function in school-aged children with and without autism spectrum disorder: links with theory of mind. Dev. Psychopathol. 31:541–56
    [Google Scholar]
  71. Kross E, Duckworth A, Ayduk O, Tsukayama E, Mischel W 2011. The effect of self-distancing on adaptive versus maladaptive self-reflection in children. Emotion 11:1032–39
    [Google Scholar]
  72. Krueger RF. 1999. The structure of common mental disorders. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 56:921–26
    [Google Scholar]
  73. Kully-Martens K, Treit S, Pei J, Rasmussen C 2013. Affective decision-making on the Iowa Gambling Task in children and adolescents with fetal alcohol spectrum disorders. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 19:137–44
    [Google Scholar]
  74. Lahey BB, Krueger RF, Rathouz PJ, Waldman ID, Zald DH 2017. A hierarchical causal taxonomy of psychopathology across the life span. Psychol. Bull. 143:142–86
    [Google Scholar]
  75. Lemire-Rodger S, Lam J, Viviano JD, Stevens WD, Spreng RN, Turner GR 2019. Inhibit, switch, and update: a within-subject fMRI investigation of executive control. Neuropsychologia 132:107134
    [Google Scholar]
  76. Lhermitte F. 1983. “Utilization behavior” and its relation to lesions to the frontal lobes. Brain 106:237–55
    [Google Scholar]
  77. Liston C, McEwen BS, Casey BJ 2009. Psychosocial stress reversibly disrupts prefrontal processing and attentional control. PNAS 106:912–17
    [Google Scholar]
  78. Lupien SJ, McEwen BS, Gunnar MR, Heim C 2009. Effects of stress throughout the lifespan on the brain, behaviour and cognition. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 10:6434–45
    [Google Scholar]
  79. Luria AR. 1966. Higher Cortical Functions in Man New York: Basic Books, 2nd ed..
    [Google Scholar]
  80. Lyons KE, Zelazo PD. 2011. Monitoring, metacognition, and executive function: elucidating the role of self-reflection in the development of self-regulation. Adv. Child Dev. Behav. 40:379–412
    [Google Scholar]
  81. MacKenzie LE, Patterson VC, Zwicker A, Drobinin V, Fisher HL et al. 2017. Hot and cold executive functions in youth with psychotic symptoms. Psychol. Med. 47:2844–53
    [Google Scholar]
  82. Mackey AP, Hill SS, Stone SI, Bunge SA 2011. Differential effects of reasoning and speed training in children. Dev. Sci. 14:3582–90
    [Google Scholar]
  83. Madole JW, Rhemtulla M, Grotzinger AD, Tucker-Drob EM, Paige Harden K 2019. Testing cold and hot cognitive control as moderators of a network of comorbid psychopathology symptoms in adolescence. Clin. Psychol. Sci. 7:701–18
    [Google Scholar]
  84. Manes F, Sahakian B, Clark L, Rogers R, Antoun N et al. 2002. Decision-making processes following damage to prefrontal cortex. Brain 125:624–39
    [Google Scholar]
  85. Marchette LK, Weisz JR. 2017. Practitioner review: empirical evolution of youth psychotherapy toward transdiagnostic approaches. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 58:970–84
    [Google Scholar]
  86. Martel MM, Pan PM, Hoffmann MS, Gadelha A, do Rosário MC et al. 2017. A general psychopathology factor (P factor) in children: structural model analysis and external validation through familial risk and child global executive function. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 126:137–48
    [Google Scholar]
  87. Marulis LM, Baker ST, Whitebread D 2020. Integrating metacognition and executive function to enhance young children's perception of and agency in their learning. Early Child. Res. Q. 50:Part 246–54
    [Google Scholar]
  88. Masten AS, Herbers JE, Desjardins CD, Cutuli JJ, McCormick CM et al. 2012. Executive function skills and school success in young children experiencing homelessness. Educ. Res. 41:373–84
    [Google Scholar]
  89. Matheny A Jr., Wachs TD, Ludwig J, Phillips K. 1995. Bringing order out of chaos: psychometric characteristics of the Confusion, Hub-bub, and Order Scale. J. Appl. Dev. Psychol. 16:429–44
    [Google Scholar]
  90. McClelland MM, Acock AC, Piccinin A, Rhea SA, Stallings MC 2013. Relations between preschool attention span-persistence and age 25 educational outcomes. Early Child. Res. Q. 28:314–24
    [Google Scholar]
  91. McElroy E, Belsky J, Carragher N, Fearon P, Patalay P 2018. Developmental stability of general and specific factors of psychopathology from early childhood to adolescence: dynamic mutualism or p‐differentiation. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 59:667–75
    [Google Scholar]
  92. McLaughlin KA. 2016. Future directions in childhood adversity and youth psychopathology. J. Clin. Child Adolesc. Psychol. 45:361–82
    [Google Scholar]
  93. McNab F, Varrone A, Farde L, Jucaite A, Bystritsky P et al. 2009. Changes in cortical dopamine D1 receptor binding associated with cognitive training. Science 323:5915800–2
    [Google Scholar]
  94. McTeague LM, Huemer J, Carreon DM, Ying J, Eickhoff SB, Etkin A 2017. Identification of common neural circuit disruptions in cognitive control across psychiatric disorders. Am. J. Psychiatry 174:7676–85
    [Google Scholar]
  95. Melby-Lervåg M, Redick TS, Hulme C 2016. Working memory training does not improve performance on measures of intelligence or other measures of “far transfer”: evidence from a meta-analytic review. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 11:512–34
    [Google Scholar]
  96. Merz EC, Desai PM, Maskus EA, Melvin SA, Rehman R et al. 2020. Socioeconomic disparities in chronic physiologic stress are associated with brain structure in children. Biol. Psychiatry 86:12921–29
    [Google Scholar]
  97. Meuwissen AS, Carlson SM. 2018. An experimental study of the effects of autonomy support on preschoolers’ self-regulation. J. Appl. Dev. Psychol. 60:11–23
    [Google Scholar]
  98. Miller MR, Giesbrecht GF, Müller U, McInerney RJ, Kerns KA 2012. A latent variable approach to determining the structure of executive function in preschool children. J. Cogn. Dev. 13:395–423
    [Google Scholar]
  99. Mischel W, Shoda Y, Rodriguez ML 1989. Delay of gratification in children. Science 244:933–38
    [Google Scholar]
  100. Miyake A, Friedman NP, Emerson MJ, Witzki AH, Howerter A 2000. The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: a latent variable analysis. Cogn. Psychol. 41:49–100
    [Google Scholar]
  101. Moffitt TE, Arseneault L, Belsky D, Dickson N, Hancox RJ et al. 2011. A gradient of childhood self-control predicts health, wealth, and public safety. PNAS 108:2693–98
    [Google Scholar]
  102. Montroy JJ, Merz C, Williams JM, Landry SH, Johnson UY et al. 2019. Hot and cool dimensionality of executive function: model invariance across age and maternal education in preschool children. Early Child. Res. Q. 49:188–201
    [Google Scholar]
  103. Moriguchi Y, Shinahara I. 2019. Less is more activation: the involvement of the lateral prefrontal regions in a “less is more” task. Dev. Neuropsychol. 44:273–81
    [Google Scholar]
  104. Munakata Y, Snyder HR, Chatham CH 2012. Developing cognitive control: three key transitions. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 21:271–77
    [Google Scholar]
  105. Mungas D, Widaman K, Zelazo PD, Tulsky D, Heaton R et al. 2013. NIH Toolbox Cognition Battery (CB): factor structure for 3- to 15-year-olds. Monogr. Soc. Res. Child Dev. 78:4103–18
    [Google Scholar]
  106. Murray AL, Eisner M, Ribeaud D 2016. The development of the general factor of psychopathology ‘p factor’ through childhood and adolescence. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 44:1573–86
    [Google Scholar]
  107. Narayan AJ, Kalstabakken AW, Labella MH, Nerenberg LM, Monn AR, Masten AS 2017. Intergenerational continuity of adverse childhood experiences in homeless families: unpacking exposure to maltreatment versus family dysfunction. Am. J. Orthopsychiatry 87:13–14
    [Google Scholar]
  108. Nejati V, Salehinejad MA, Nitsche MA 2018. Interaction of the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (L-dlPFC) and right orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) in hot and cold executive functions: evidence from transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS). Neuroscience 369:Suppl. C109–23
    [Google Scholar]
  109. Nelson TD, Kidwell KM, Nelson JM, Tomaso CC, Hankey M, Espy KA 2018. Preschool executive control and internalizing symptoms in elementary school. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 46:71509–20
    [Google Scholar]
  110. Nesbitt KT, Baker-Ward L, Willoughby MT 2013. Executive function mediates socio-economic and racial differences in early academic achievement. Early Child. Res. Q. 28:4774–83
    [Google Scholar]
  111. Niendam TA, Laird AR, Ray KL, Dean YM, Glahn DC, Carter CS 2012. Meta-analytic evidence for a superordinate cognitive control network subserving diverse executive functions. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 12:2241–68
    [Google Scholar]
  112. Noble KG, Houston SM, Brito NH, Bartsch H, Kan E et al. 2015. Family income, parental education and brain structure in children and adolescents. Nat. Neurosci. 18:773–78
    [Google Scholar]
  113. O'Hearn K, Asato M, Ordaz S, Luna B 2008. Neurodevelopment and executive function in autism. Dev. Psychopathol. 20:1103–32
    [Google Scholar]
  114. Ortner CMN, Kilner S, Zelazo PD 2007. Mindfulness meditation and emotional interference in a simple cognitive task. Motiv. Emot. 31:271–83
    [Google Scholar]
  115. Paige Harden K, Engelhardt LE, Mann FD, Patterson MW, Grotzinger AD et al. 2019. Genetic associations between executive functions and a general factor of psychopathology. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry. In press. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2019.05.006
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  116. Petrovic P, Castellanos FX. 2016. Top-down dysregulation—from ADHD to emotional instability. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 10:70
    [Google Scholar]
  117. Pietrefesa AS, Evans DW. 2007. Affective and neuropsychological correlates of children's rituals and compulsive-like behaviors: continuities and discontinuities with obsessive-compulsive disorder. Brain Cogn 65:136–46
    [Google Scholar]
  118. Pozuelos JP, Combita LM, Abundis A, Paz-Alonso PM, Conejero A et al. 2019. Metacognitive scaffolding boosts cognitive and neural benefits following executive attention training in children. Dev. Sci. 22:e12756
    [Google Scholar]
  119. Rahimi-Golkhandan S, Steenbergen B, Piek JP, Caeyenberghs K, Wilson PH 2016. Revealing hot executive function in children with motor coordination problems: what's the go. Brain Cogn 106:55–64
    [Google Scholar]
  120. Riggs NR, Greenberg MT, Kusché CA, Pentz MA 2006. The mediational role of neurocognition in the behavioral outcomes of a social-emotional prevention program in elementary school students: effects of the PATHS curriculum. Prev. Sci. 7:91–102
    [Google Scholar]
  121. Robbins TW, Arnsten AFT. 2009. The neuropsychopharmacology of fronto-executive function: monoaminergic modulation. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 32:267–87
    [Google Scholar]
  122. Roberts A. 2011. The importance of serotonin for orbitofrontal function. Biol. Psychiatry 69:1185–91
    [Google Scholar]
  123. Roebers C. 2017. Executive function and metacognition: towards a unifying framework of cognitive self-regulation. Dev. Rev. 45:31–51
    [Google Scholar]
  124. Rolls ET. 2004. The functions of the orbitofrontal cortex. Brain Cogn 55:111–29
    [Google Scholar]
  125. Rowland AS, Skipper BJ, Rabiner DL, Qeadan F, Campbell RA et al. 2018. Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): interaction between socioeconomic status and parental history of ADHD determines prevalence. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 59:213–22
    [Google Scholar]
  126. Rubia K. 2011. “Cool” inferior frontostriatal dysfunction in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder versus “hot” ventromedial orbitofrontal-limbic dysfunction in conduct disorder: a review. Biol. Psychiatry 69:e69–87
    [Google Scholar]
  127. Schmitt SA, Simpson AM, Friend M 2011. A longitudinal assessment of the home literacy environment and early language. Infant Child Dev 20:409–31
    [Google Scholar]
  128. Schonert-Reichl KA, Oberle E, Lawlor MS, Abbott D, Thomson K et al. 2015. Enhancing cognitive and social-emotional development through a simple-to-administer mindfulness-based school program for elementary school children: a randomized controlled trial. Dev. Psychol. 51:152–66
    [Google Scholar]
  129. Shanmugan S, Wolf DH, Calkins ME, Moore TM, Ruparel K et al. 2016. Common and dissociable mechanisms of executive system dysfunction across psychiatric disorders in youth. Am. J. Psychiatry 173:5517–26
    [Google Scholar]
  130. Shi R, Sharpe L, Abbott M 2019. A meta-analysis of the relationship between anxiety and attentional control. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 72:101754
    [Google Scholar]
  131. Shonkoff JP, Garner AS, Siegel BS, Dobbins MI, Earls MF et al. 2012. The lifelong effects of early childhood adversity and toxic stress. Pediatrics 129:1e232–46
    [Google Scholar]
  132. Smith SM. 1982. Enhancement of recall using multiple environmental contexts during learning. Mem. Cogn. 10:405–12
    [Google Scholar]
  133. Sonuga-Barke EJS. 2003. The dual pathway model of AD/HD: an elaboration of neuro-developmental characteristics. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 27:593–604
    [Google Scholar]
  134. Sonuga-Barke EJS, Cortese S, Fairchild G, Stringaris A 2016. Annual research review: transdiagnostic neuroscience of child and adolescent mental disorders—differentiating decision making in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder, conduct disorder, depression, and anxiety. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 57:3321–49
    [Google Scholar]
  135. Teachman BA, Clerkin EM, Cunningham WA, Dreyer-Oren S, Werntz A 2019. Implicit cognition and psychopathology: looking back and looking forward. Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 15:123–48
    [Google Scholar]
  136. Toll SW, Van der Ven SH, Kroesbergen EH, Van Luit JE 2011. Executive functions as predictors of math learning disabilities. J. Learn. Disabil. 44:6521–32
    [Google Scholar]
  137. Tomalski P, Moore DG, Ribeiro H, Axelsson EL, Murphy E et al. 2013. Socioeconomic status and functional brain development—associations in early infancy. Dev. Sci. 16:5676–87
    [Google Scholar]
  138. Travers-Hill E, Dunn B, Hoppitt L, Hitchcock C, Dalgleish T 2017. Beneficial effects of training in self-distancing and perspective broadening for people with a history of recurrent depression. Behav. Res. Ther. 95:19–28
    [Google Scholar]
  139. Trope Y, Liberman N. 2010. Construal-level theory of psychological distance. Psychol. Rev. 117:440–63
    [Google Scholar]
  140. Vliegenthart J, Noppe G, van Rossum EFC, Koper JW, Raat H, van den Akker ELT 2016. Socioeconomic status in children is associated with hair cortisol levels as a biological measure of chronic stress. Psychoneuroendocrinology 65:9–14
    [Google Scholar]
  141. Wiebe S, Sheffield T, Nelson JM, Clark CAC, Chevalier N, Espy KA 2011. The structure of executive function in 3-year-old children. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 108:3436–52
    [Google Scholar]
  142. Willoughby M, Kupersmidt J, Voegler-Lee M, Bryant D 2011. Contributions of hot and cool self-regulation to preschool disruptive behavior and academic achievement. Dev. Neuropsychol. 36:162–80
    [Google Scholar]
  143. Wilson JW, Andrews G, Hogan C, Wang S, Shum DHK 2018. Executive function in middle childhood and the relationship with theory of mind. Dev. Neuropsychol. 43:3163–82
    [Google Scholar]
  144. Woolgar A, Afshar S, Williams MA, Richet AN 2015. Flexible coding of task rules in frontoparietal cortex: an adaptive system for flexible cognitive control. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 27:101895–911
    [Google Scholar]
  145. Zatorre RJ, Fields RD, Johansen-Berg H 2013. Plasticity in gray and white: neuroimaging changes in brain structure during learning. Nat. Neurosci. 15:4528–36
    [Google Scholar]
  146. Zelazo PD. 2006. The dimensional change card sort (DCCS): a method of assessing executive function in children. Nat. Protoc. 1:1297–301
    [Google Scholar]
  147. Zelazo PD. 2015. Executive function: reflection, iterative reprocessing, complexity, and the developing brain. Dev. Rev. 38:55–68
    [Google Scholar]
  148. Zelazo PD, Anderson JE, Richler J, Wallner-Allen K, Beaumont JL, Weintraub S 2013. NIH Toolbox Cognition Battery (CB): measuring executive function and attention. Monogr. Soc. Res. Child Dev. 78:416–33
    [Google Scholar]
  149. Zelazo PD, Blair CB, Willoughby MT 2016. Executive function: implications for education Rep. NCER 2017-2000, US Dep. Educ Washington, DC: https://ies.ed.gov/ncer/pubs/20172000/pdf/20172000.pdf
    [Google Scholar]
  150. Zelazo PD, Carlson SM. 2012. Hot and cool executive function in childhood and adolescence: development and plasticity. Child Dev. Perspect. 6:354–60
    [Google Scholar]
  151. Zelazo PD, Forston JL, Masten AS, Carlson SM 2018. Mindfulness plus reflection training: effects on executive function in early childhood. Front. Psychol. 9:208
    [Google Scholar]
  152. Zelazo PD, Frye D, Rapus T 1996. An age-related dissociation between knowing rules and using them. Cogn. Dev. 11:137–63
    [Google Scholar]
  153. Zelazo PD, Müller U. 2002. Executive functions in typical and atypical development. Handbook of Childhood Cognitive Development U Goswami 445–69 Oxford, UK: Blackwell
    [Google Scholar]
  154. Zelazo PD, Müller U, Frye D, Marcovitch S 2003. The development of executive function in early childhood. Monogr. Soc. Res. Child Dev. 68:3vii–137
    [Google Scholar]
  155. Zimmerman DL, Ownsworth T, O'Donovan A, Roberts J, Gullo MJ 2016. Independence of hot and cold executive function deficits in high-functioning adults with autism spectrum disorder. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 10:24
    [Google Scholar]
/content/journals/10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-072319-024242
Loading
/content/journals/10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-072319-024242
Loading

Data & Media loading...

  • Article Type: Review Article
This is a required field
Please enter a valid email address
Approval was a Success
Invalid data
An Error Occurred
Approval was partially successful, following selected items could not be processed due to error