1932

Abstract

Magic is an art form that has fascinated humans for centuries. Recently, the techniques used by magicians to make their audience experience the impossible have attracted the attention of psychologists, who, in just a couple of decades, have produced a large amount of research regarding how these effects operate, focusing on the blind spots in perception and roadblocks in cognition that magic techniques exploit. Most recently, this investigation has given a pathway to a new line of research that uses magic effects to explore the cognitive abilities of nonhuman animals. This new branch of the scientific study of magic has already yielded new evidence illustrating the power of magic effects as a psychological tool for nonhuman animals. This review aims to give a thorough overview of the research on both the human and nonhuman perception of magic effects by critically illustrating the most prominent works of both fields of inquiry.

Loading

Article metrics loading...

/content/journals/10.1146/annurev-psych-012723-100945
2024-01-18
2024-05-02
Loading full text...

Full text loading...

/deliver/fulltext/psych/75/1/annurev-psych-012723-100945.html?itemId=/content/journals/10.1146/annurev-psych-012723-100945&mimeType=html&fmt=ahah

Literature Cited

  1. Ariely D, Carmon Z 2000. Gestalt characteristics of experiences: the defining features of summarized events. J. Behav. Decis. Mak. 13:2191–201
    [Google Scholar]
  2. Ariely D, Levav J. 2000. Sequential choice in group settings: taking the road less traveled and less enjoyed. J. Consum. Res. 27:3279–90
    [Google Scholar]
  3. Ariely D, Zauberman G. 2000. On the making of an experience: the effects of breaking and combining experiences on their overall evaluation. J. Behav. Decis. Mak. 13:2219–32
    [Google Scholar]
  4. Ascanio A. 1964. Consideraciones sobre la misdirection. Misdirection 1:4–6
    [Google Scholar]
  5. Ascanio A. 2005 (1958). Conception of the magical atmosphere. The Magic of Ascanio, Vol. 1 The Structural Conception of Magic M Sisti, A Escamilla 10–58. New York: Paginas
    [Google Scholar]
  6. Barnhart AS. 2010. The exploitation of Gestalt principles by magicians. Perception 39:91286–89
    [Google Scholar]
  7. Bartlett FC. 1932. Remembering: A Study in Experimental and Social Psychology Cambridge, UK: Cambridge Univ. Press
  8. Baumgartner H, Sujan M, Padgett D. 1997. Patterns of affective reactions to advertisements: the integration of moment-to-moment responses into overall judgments. J. Mark. Res. 34:2219–32
    [Google Scholar]
  9. Beth T, Ekroll V. 2015. The curious influence of timing on the magical experience evoked by conjuring tricks involving false transfer: decay of amodal object permanence?. Psychol. Res. 79:4513–22
    [Google Scholar]
  10. Billard P, Clayton NS, Jozet-Alves C. 2020a. Cuttlefish retrieve whether they smelt or saw a previously encountered item. Sci. Rep. 10:5413
    [Google Scholar]
  11. Billard P, Schnell AK, Clayton NS, Jozet-Alves C. 2020b. Cuttlefish show flexible and future-dependent foraging cognition. Biol. Lett. 16:2 https://doi.org/10.1098/rsbl.2019.0743
    [Google Scholar]
  12. Binet A. 1896. Psychology of prestidigitation. Annual Report of the Board of Regents of the Smithsonian Institution555–71. Washington, DC: Gov. Print. Off.
    [Google Scholar]
  13. Bobrowicz K, Osvath M. 2019. Cognition in the fast lane: Ravens’ gazes are half as short as humans’ when choosing objects. Anim. Behav. Cogn. 6:281–97
    [Google Scholar]
  14. Boström JE, Dimitrova M, Canton C, Håstad O, Qvarnström A, Ödeen A. 2016. Ultra-rapid vision in birds. PLOS ONE 11:33–9
    [Google Scholar]
  15. Bräuer J, Hanus D. 2012. Fairness in non-human primates?. Soc. Justice Res. 25:3256–76
    [Google Scholar]
  16. Bridgeman B, Van der Heijden AHC, Velichkovsky BM. 1994. A theory of visual stability across saccadic eye movements. Behav. Brain Sci. 17:2247–58
    [Google Scholar]
  17. Brosnan SF, De Waal FBM. 2003. Monkeys reject unequal pay. Nature 425:6955297–99
    [Google Scholar]
  18. Brown C, Garwood MP, Williamson JE. 2012. It pays to cheat: tactical deception in a cephalopod social signalling system. Biol. Lett. 8:5729–32
    [Google Scholar]
  19. Bruno J. 1978. Anatomy of Misdirection Stony Brook, NY: Stony Brook Press
  20. Bugnyar T, Heinrich B. 2006. Pilfering ravens, Corvus corax, adjust their behaviour to social context and identity of competitors. Anim. Cogn. 9:4369–76
    [Google Scholar]
  21. Bugnyar T, Kotrschal K. 2002. Observational learning and the raiding of food caches in ravens, Corvus corax: Is it “tactical” deception?. Anim. Behav. 64:2185–95
    [Google Scholar]
  22. Byrne RW. 1996. Machiavellian intelligence. Evol. Anthropol. 5:5172–80
    [Google Scholar]
  23. Byrne RW, Whiten A. 1985. Tactical deception of familiar individuals in baboons (Papio ursinus). Anim. Behav. 33:2669–73
    [Google Scholar]
  24. Call J, Tomasello M. 1999. A nonverbal false belief task: the performance of children and great apes. Child Dev 70:2381–95
    [Google Scholar]
  25. Call J, Tomasello M. 2008. Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? 30 years later. Trends Cogn. Sci. 12:5187–92
    [Google Scholar]
  26. Camerer CF, Ho TH. 1994. Violations of the betweenness axiom and nonlinearity in probability. J. Risk Uncertain. 8:2167–96
    [Google Scholar]
  27. Cheke LG, Clayton NS. 2012. Eurasian jays (Garrulus glandarius) overcome their current desires to anticipate two distinct future needs and plan for them appropriately. Biol. Lett. 8:2171–75
    [Google Scholar]
  28. Clayton NS, Dally JM, Emery NJ. 2007. Social cognition by food-caching corvids: the western scrub-jay as a natural psychologist. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B 362:1480507–22
    [Google Scholar]
  29. Clayton NS, Dickinson A. 1998. Episodic-like memory during cache recovery by scrub jays. Nature 395:6699272–74
    [Google Scholar]
  30. Clayton NS, Wilkins C. 2017. Memory, mental time travel and the Moustachio Quartet. Interface Focus 7:3 https://doi.org/10.1098/rsfs.2016.0112
    [Google Scholar]
  31. Clayton NS, Yu KS, Dickinson A. 2003. Interacting cache memories: evidence for flexible memory use by Western scrub-jays (Aphelocoma californica). J. Exp. Psychol. Anim. Behav. Process. 29:114–22
    [Google Scholar]
  32. Correia SPC, Dickinson A, Clayton NSS. 2007. Western scrub-jays anticipate future needs independently of their current motivational state. Curr. Biol. 17:10856–61
    [Google Scholar]
  33. Cui J, Otero-Millan J, Macknik S, King M, Martinez-Conde S. 2011. Stronger perception of magic without social misdirection. J. Vis. 11:11475
    [Google Scholar]
  34. Dally JM, Emery NJ, Clayton NS. 2005. Cache protection strategies by western scrub-jays, Aphelocoma californica: implications for social cognition. Anim. Behav. 70:61251–63
    [Google Scholar]
  35. Dally JM, Emery NJ, Clayton NS. 2006. Food-caching western scrub-jays keep track of who was watching when. Science 312:57801662–65
    [Google Scholar]
  36. Dally JM, Emery NJ, Clayton NS. 2010. Avian theory of mind and counter espionage by food-caching western scrub-jays (Aphelocoma californica). Eur. J. Dev. Psychol. 7:117–37
    [Google Scholar]
  37. Danek AH, Öllinger M, Fraps T, Grothe B, Flanagin VL. 2015. An fMRI investigation of expectation violation in magic tricks. Front. Psychol. 6: https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00084
    [Google Scholar]
  38. de Waal FBM. 1986. Of chimpanzees. Deception: Perspectives on Human and Nonhuman Deceit RW Mitchell, NS Thompson 224–25. Albany, NY: SUNY Press
    [Google Scholar]
  39. DeMartini DG, Ghoshal A, Pandolfi E, Weaver AT, Baum M, Morse DE. 2013. Dynamic biophotonics: Female squid exhibit sexually dimorphic tunable leucophores and iridocytes. J. Exp. Biol. 216:193733–41
    [Google Scholar]
  40. Dukas R, Kamil AC. 2000. The cost of limited attention in blue jays. Behav. Ecol. 11:5502–6
    [Google Scholar]
  41. Dukas R, Kamil AC. 2001. Limited attention: the constraint underlying search image. Behav. Ecol. 12:2192–99
    [Google Scholar]
  42. Ekroll V, Sayim B, Wagemans J. 2017. The other side of magic: the psychology of perceiving hidden things. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 12:191–106
    [Google Scholar]
  43. Ekroll V, Wagemans J. 2016. Conjuring deceptions: fooling the eye or fooling the mind?. Trends Cogn. Sci. 20:7486–89
    [Google Scholar]
  44. Emery NJ, Clayton NS. 2001. Effects of experience and social context on prospective caching strategies by scrub jays. Nature 414:6862443–46
    [Google Scholar]
  45. Emery NJ, Dally JM, Clayton NS. 2004. Western scrub-jays (Aphelocoma californica) use cognitive strategies to protect their caches from thieving conspecifics. Anim. Cogn. 7:137–43
    [Google Scholar]
  46. Finn JK, Tregenza T, Norman MD. 2009. Defensive tool use in a coconut-carrying octopus. Curr. Biol. 19:23R1069–70
    [Google Scholar]
  47. Fox DM, Mundinano IC, Bourne JA. 2019. Prehensile kinematics of the marmoset monkey: implications for the evolution of visually-guided behaviors. J. Comp. Neurol. 527:91495–507
    [Google Scholar]
  48. Fragaszy DM, Crast J 2016. Functions of the hand in primates. The Evolution of the Primate Hand: Anatomical, Developmental, Functional, and Paleontological Evidence TL Kivell, P Lemelin, BG Richmond, D Schmitt 313–44. New York: Springer
    [Google Scholar]
  49. Garcia-Pelegrin E. 2022. The comparative science of magic: using sleight of hand as a tool to investigate cognition in diverse taxa PhD Diss. Univ. Cambridge Cambridge, UK:
  50. Garcia-Pelegrin E, Miller R, Wilkins C, Clayton NS. 2023. Manual action expectation and biomechanical ability in three species of New World monkey. Curr. Biol. 33:91803–8.e2
    [Google Scholar]
  51. Garcia-Pelegrin E, Schnell AK, Wilkins C, Clayton NS. 2020. An unexpected audience. Science 369:65101424–26
    [Google Scholar]
  52. Garcia-Pelegrin E, Schnell AK, Wilkins C, Clayton NS. 2021a. Exploring the perceptual inabilities of Eurasian jays (Garrulus glandarius) using magic effects. PNAS 118:24e2026106118
    [Google Scholar]
  53. Garcia-Pelegrin E, Schnell AK, Wilkins C, Clayton NS. 2022a. Could it be proto magic? Deceptive tactics in nonhuman animals resemble magician's misdirection. Psychol. Conscious. Theory Res. Pract. 9:3267–84
    [Google Scholar]
  54. Garcia-Pelegrin E, Wilkins C, Clayton NS. 2021b. The ape that lived to tell the tale. The evolution of the art of storytelling and its relationship to mental time travel and theory of mind. Front. Psychol. 12: https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.755783
    [Google Scholar]
  55. Garcia-Pelegrin E, Wilkins C, Clayton NS. 2022b. Investigating expert performance when observing magic effects. Sci. Rep. 12:5141
    [Google Scholar]
  56. Grahn JA, Parkinson JA, Owen AM. 2008. The cognitive functions of the caudate nucleus. Prog. Neurobiol. 86:3141–55
    [Google Scholar]
  57. Grimes J. 1996. On the failure to detect changes in scenes across saccades. Perception 5:89–110
    [Google Scholar]
  58. Haines RF 1991. A breakdown in simultaneous information processing. Presbyopia Research: From Molecular Biology to Visual Adaptation G Obrecht, LW Stark 171–75. Boston: Springer
    [Google Scholar]
  59. Hanlon R. 2007. Cephalopod dynamic camouflage. Curr. Biol. 17:11R400–4
    [Google Scholar]
  60. Hanlon RT, Naud M-J, Shaw PW, Havenhand JN. 2005. Transient sexual mimicry leads to fertilization. Nature 433:7023212
    [Google Scholar]
  61. Heesy CP. 2009. Seeing in stereo: the ecology and evolution of primate binocular vision and stereopsis. Evol. Anthropol. 18:121–35
    [Google Scholar]
  62. Howard IP, Rogers BJ. 1995. Binocular Vision and Stereopsis New York: Oxford Univ. Press
  63. Huffard CL. 2006. Locomotion by Abdopus aculeatus (Cephalopoda: Octopodidae): walking the line between primary and secondary defenses. J. Exp. Biol. 209:193697–707
    [Google Scholar]
  64. Hyman R. 1989. The psychology of deception. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 40:133–54
    [Google Scholar]
  65. Jozet-Alves C, Bertin M, Clayton NS. 2013. Evidence of episodic-like memory in cuttlefish. Curr. Biol. 23:23R1033–35
    [Google Scholar]
  66. Kahneman D, Fredrickson BL, Schreiber CA, Redelmeier DA. 1993. When more pain is preferred to less: adding a better end. Psychol. Sci. 4:6401–5
    [Google Scholar]
  67. Kahneman D, Riis J 2005. Living, and thinking about it: two perspectives on life. The Science of Well-Being FA Huppert, N Baylis, B Keverne 285–304. Oxford, UK: Oxford Univ. Press
    [Google Scholar]
  68. Kahneman D, Wakker PP, Sarin R. 1997. Back to Bentham? Explorations of experienced utility. Q. J. Econ. 112:2375–406
    [Google Scholar]
  69. Kanizsa G, Gerbino W. 1982. Amodal completion: seeing or thinking?. Organization and Representation in Perception J Beck 167–90. Chicago: Lawrence Erlbaum
    [Google Scholar]
  70. Kelley TA, Chun MM, Chua K-P. 2003. Effects of scene inversion on change detection of targets matched for visual salience. J. Vis. 3:1 https://doi.org/10.1167/3.1.1
    [Google Scholar]
  71. Kensinger EA. 2004. Remembering emotional experiences: the contribution of valence and arousal. Rev. Neurosci. 15:4241–52
    [Google Scholar]
  72. Kensinger EA. 2009. Remembering the details: effects of emotion. Emot. Rev. 1:299–113
    [Google Scholar]
  73. Kerns JG, Cohen JD, MacDonald AW, Cho RY, Stenger VA, Carter CS. 2004. Anterior cingulate conflict monitoring and adjustments in control. Science 303:56601023–26
    [Google Scholar]
  74. Koboroff A, Kaplan G, Rogers LJ. 2008. Hemispheric specialization in Australian magpies (Gymnorhina tibicen) shown as eye preferences during response to a predator. Brain Res. Bull. 76:3304–6
    [Google Scholar]
  75. Köhler W. 1929. Gestalt Psychology. New York: Liveright
  76. Kuhn G. 2019. Experiencing the Impossible: The Science of Magic Cambridge, MA: MIT Press
  77. Kuhn G, Amlani AA, Rensink RA. 2008a. Towards a science of magic. Trends Cogn. Sci. 12:9349–54
    [Google Scholar]
  78. Kuhn G, Caffaratti HA, Teszka R, Rensink RA. 2014. A psychologically-based taxonomy of misdirection. Front. Psychol. 5: https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01392
    [Google Scholar]
  79. Kuhn G, Findlay JM. 2010. Misdirection, attention and awareness: Inattentional blindness reveals temporal relationship between eye movements and visual awareness. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 63:1136–46
    [Google Scholar]
  80. Kuhn G, Land MF. 2006. There's more to magic than meets the eye. Curr. Biol. 16:22950–51
    [Google Scholar]
  81. Kuhn G, Rensink RA. 2016. The vanishing ball illusion: a new perspective on the perception of dynamic events. Cognition 148:64–70
    [Google Scholar]
  82. Kuhn G, Tatler BW. 2005. Magic and fixation: Now you don't see it, now you do. Perception 34:91155–61
    [Google Scholar]
  83. Kuhn G, Tatler BW. 2011. Misdirected by the gap: the relationship between inattentional blindness and attentional misdirection. Conscious. Cogn. 20:2432–36
    [Google Scholar]
  84. Kuhn G, Tatler BW, Cole GG. 2009. You look where I look! Effect of gaze cues on overt and covert attention in misdirection. Vis. Cogn. 17:6–7925–44
    [Google Scholar]
  85. Kuhn G, Tatler BW, Findlay JM, Cole GG. 2008b. Misdirection in magic: implications for the relationship between eye gaze and attention. Vis. Cogn. 16:2–3391–405
    [Google Scholar]
  86. Kuhn G, Thomas C 2022. What can magic reveal about the brain. Encyclopedia of Behavioral Neuroscience G Koob, S Della Sala 597–604. Amsterdam: Elsevier. , 2nd ed..
    [Google Scholar]
  87. Lamont P. 2015. Problems with the mapping of magic tricks. Front. Psychol. 6: https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00855
    [Google Scholar]
  88. Lamont P, Henderson JM, Smith TJ. 2010. Where science and magic meet: the illusion of a “science of magic. .” Rev. Gen. Psychol. 14:116–21
    [Google Scholar]
  89. Lamont P, Wiseman R. 1999. Magic in Theory: An Introduction to the Theoretical and Psychological Elements of Conjuring Hatfield, UK: Univ. Hertfordshire Press
  90. Legg EW, Clayton NS. 2014. Eurasian jays (Garrulus glandarius) conceal caches from onlookers. Anim. Cogn. 17:51223–26
    [Google Scholar]
  91. Levin DT, Simons DJ, Angelone BL, Chabris CF. 2002. Memory for centrally attended changing objects in an incidental real-world change detection paradigm. Br. J. Psychol. 93:3289–302
    [Google Scholar]
  92. Mack A, Rock I. 1998. Inattentional blindness: perception without attention. Vis. Atten. 8:55–76
    [Google Scholar]
  93. Macknik SL, King M, Randi J, Robbins A, Thompson J, Martinez-Conde S 2008. Attention and awareness in stage magic: turning tricks into research. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 9:871–79
    [Google Scholar]
  94. Macknik SL, Martinez-Conde S. 2009. Real magic: Future studies of magic should be grounded in neuroscience. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 10:241
    [Google Scholar]
  95. Macknik SL, Martinez-Conde S, Blakeslee S. 2010. Sleights of Mind: What the Neuroscience of Magic Reveals About Our Everyday Deceptions New York: Henry Holt & Co.
  96. Martin GR. 2007. Visual fields and their functions in birds. J. Ornithol. 148:2547–62
    [Google Scholar]
  97. Memmert D. 2006. The effects of eye movements, age, and expertise on inattentional blindness. Conscious. Cogn. 15:3620–27
    [Google Scholar]
  98. Memmert D. 2010. The gap between inattentional blindness and attentional misdirection. Conscious. Cogn. 19:41097–101
    [Google Scholar]
  99. Michotte A. 1950. A propos de la permanence phénoménale: faits et théories. Acta Psychol. 7:298–322
    [Google Scholar]
  100. Michotte A, Thinès G, Crabbé G 1991. Amodal completion of perceptual structures. Michotte's Experimental Phenomenology of Perception G Thines, A Costall, G Butterworth 140–67. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum
    [Google Scholar]
  101. Mitchell RW. 1986. A framework for discussing deception. Deception: Perspectives on Human and Nonhuman Deceit RW Mitchell, NS Thompson 3–40. New York: SUNY Press
    [Google Scholar]
  102. Mitchell RW, Thompson NS. 1986. Deception: Perspectives on Human and Nonhuman Deceit New York: SUNY Press
  103. Napier JR. 1967. Evolutionary aspects of primate locomotion. Am. J. Biol. Anthropol. 27:3333–41
    [Google Scholar]
  104. Novikova M, Kuznetsov A. 2017. Palmar flexion creases and finger linkage groups in New World monkeys—functional and evolutionary palmistry. Biol. Commun. 62:3181–201
    [Google Scholar]
  105. Otero-Millan J, Macknik SL, Robbins A, Martinez-Conde S. 2011. Stronger misdirection in curved than in straight motion. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 5:133
    [Google Scholar]
  106. Pailhès A, Kuhn G. 2020. The apparent action causation: using a magician forcing technique to investigate our illusory sense of agency over the outcome of our choices. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 73:111784–95
    [Google Scholar]
  107. Panetta D, Buresch K, Hanlon RT. 2017. Dynamic masquerade with morphing three-dimensional skin in cuttlefish. Biol. Lett. 13:320170070
    [Google Scholar]
  108. Parris BA, Kuhn G, Mizon GA, Benattayallah A, Hodgson TL. 2009. Imaging the impossible: an fMRI study of impossible causal relationships in magic tricks. NeuroImage 45:31033–39
    [Google Scholar]
  109. Phillips F, Natter MB, Egan EJL. 2015. Magically deceptive biological motion—the French Drop Sleight. Front. Psychol. 6:371
    [Google Scholar]
  110. Pochon J-B, Riis J, Sanfey AG, Nystrom LE, Cohen JD. 2008. Functional imaging of decision conflict. J. Neurosci. 28:133468–73
    [Google Scholar]
  111. Povinelli DJ, Eddy TJ. 1996. What Young Chimpanzees Know About Seeing. Chicago: Univ. Chicago Press
  112. Povinelli DJ, Rulf AB, Bierschwale DT. 1994. Absence of knowledge attribution and self-recognition in young chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). J. Comp. Psychol. 108:174–80
    [Google Scholar]
  113. Raby C, Alexis D, Dickinson A, Clayton NS 2007. Planning for the future by western scrub-jays. Nature 445:7130919–21
    [Google Scholar]
  114. Rensink RA, Kuhn G. 2015. The possibility of a science of magic. Front. Psychol. 6: https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01576
    [Google Scholar]
  115. Rensink RA, O'Regan JK, Clark JJ. 1997. To see or not to see: the need for attention to perceive changes in scenes. Psychol. Sci. 8:5368–73
    [Google Scholar]
  116. Reynolds V, Napier JR, Napier PH. 1969. A Handbook of Living Primates London: Academic
  117. Rogers L, Kaplan G. 2006. An eye for a predator: lateralization in birds, with particular reference to the Australian magpie. Behavioural and Morphological Asymmetries in Vertebrates YB Malashichev, W Deckel 47–57. London: Taylor & Francis
    [Google Scholar]
  118. Schnell AK, Boeckle M, Rivera M, Clayton NS, Hanlon RT. 2021a. Cuttlefish exert self-control in a delay of gratification task. Proc. R. Soc. B. 288:194620203161
    [Google Scholar]
  119. Schnell AK, Loconsole M, Garcia-Pelegrin E, Wilkins C, Clayton NS. 2021b. Jays are sensitive to cognitive illusions. R. Soc. Open Sci. 8:8202358
    [Google Scholar]
  120. Sharpe SH. 1988. Conjurers’ Psychological Secrets Calgary, Can.: Hades Publ.
  121. Shaw RC, Clayton NS. 2012. Eurasian jays, Garrulus glandarius, flexibly switch caching and pilfering tactics in response to social context. Anim. Behav. 84:51191–200
    [Google Scholar]
  122. Shaw RC, Clayton NS. 2013. Careful cachers and prying pilferers: Eurasian jays (Garrulus glandarius) limit auditory information available to competitors. Proc. R. Soc. B 280:175220122238
    [Google Scholar]
  123. Simons DJ, Chabris CF. 1999. Gorillas in our midst: sustained inattentional blindness for dynamic events. Perception 28:91059–74
    [Google Scholar]
  124. Simons DJ, Levin DT. 1997. Change blindness. Trends Cogn. Sci. 1:7261–67
    [Google Scholar]
  125. Stulp G, Emery NJ, Verhulst S, Clayton NS. 2009. Western scrub-jays conceal auditory information when competitors can hear but cannot see. Biol. Lett. 5:5583–85
    [Google Scholar]
  126. Thomas C, Didierjean A. 2016. No need for a social cue! A masked magician can also trick the audience in the vanishing ball illusion. Atten. Percept. Psychophys. 78:121–29
    [Google Scholar]
  127. Thomas C, Didierjean A, Kuhn G. 2018a. It is magic! How impossible solutions prevent the discovery of obvious ones?. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 71:122481–87
    [Google Scholar]
  128. Thomas C, Didierjean A, Kuhn G. 2018b. The Flushtration Count Illusion: Attribute substitution tricks our interpretation of a simple visual event sequence. Br. J. Psychol. 109:4850–61
    [Google Scholar]
  129. Tompkins M. 2019. The Spectacle of Illusion. Deception, Magic and the Paranormal New York: Distrib. Art Publ.
  130. Triesch J, Ballard DH, Hayhoe MM, Sullivan BT. 2003. What you see is what you need. J. Vis. 3:186–94
    [Google Scholar]
  131. Triplett N. 1900. The psychology of conjuring deceptions. Am. J. Psychol. 11:4439–510
    [Google Scholar]
  132. Tversky A, Kahneman D. 1992. Advances in prospect theory: cumulative representation of uncertainty. J. Risk Uncertain. 5:4297–323
    [Google Scholar]
  133. van Hagen M, Bron P. 2014. Enhancing the experience of the train journey: changing the focus from satisfaction to emotional experience of customers. Transp. Res. Procedia 1:1253–63
    [Google Scholar]
  134. van Veen V, Carter CS. 2006. Conflict and cognitive control in the brain. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 15:5237–40
    [Google Scholar]
  135. Waugh CE, Hamilton JP, Gotlib IH. 2010. The neural temporal dynamics of the intensity of emotional experience. NeuroImage 49:21699–707
    [Google Scholar]
  136. Wexler M, Klam F. 2001. Movement prediction and movement production. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 27:148–64
    [Google Scholar]
  137. Whiten A, Byrne RW. 1988a. Tactical deception in primates. Behav. Brain Sci. 11:2233–44
    [Google Scholar]
  138. Whiten A, Byrne RW. 1988b. The Machiavellian intelligence hypotheses: editorial. Machiavellian Intelligence: Social Expertise and the Evolution of Intellect in Monkeys, Apes, and Humans RW Byrne, A Whiten 1–9. Oxford, UK: Clarendon
    [Google Scholar]
  139. Wilkins C, Clayton N 2019. Reflections on the spoon test. Neuropsychologia 134:107221
    [Google Scholar]
  140. Williams P, Simons DJ. 2000. Detecting changes in novel, complex three-dimensional objects. Vis. Cogn. 7:1–3297–322
    [Google Scholar]
  141. Wiseman R, Greening E. 2005. “It's still bending”: verbal suggestion and alleged psychokinetic ability. Br. J. Psychol. 96:1115–27
    [Google Scholar]
  142. Wundt W. 1879. Spiritualism as a scientific question. Pop. Sci. Mon. 15:577–93
    [Google Scholar]
  143. Zöllner JC. 1878. On space of four dimensions. Q. J. Sci. 8:227–37
    [Google Scholar]
/content/journals/10.1146/annurev-psych-012723-100945
Loading
/content/journals/10.1146/annurev-psych-012723-100945
Loading

Data & Media loading...

  • Article Type: Review Article
This is a required field
Please enter a valid email address
Approval was a Success
Invalid data
An Error Occurred
Approval was partially successful, following selected items could not be processed due to error