1932

Abstract

The review first discusses componential explanations of automaticity, which specify non/automaticity features (e.g., un/controlled, un/conscious, non/efficient, fast/slow) and their interrelations. Reframing these features as factors that influence processes (e.g., goals, attention, and time) broadens the range of factors that can be considered (e.g., adding stimulus intensity and representational quality). The evidence reviewed challenges the view of a perfect coherence among goals, attention, and consciousness, and supports the alternative view that () these and other factors influence the quality of representations in an additive way (e.g., little time can be compensated by extra attention or extra stimulus intensity) and that () a first threshold of this quality is required for unconscious processing and a second threshold for conscious processing. The review closes with a discussion of causal explanations of automaticity, which specify factors involved in automatization such as repetition and complexity, and a discussion of mechanistic explanations, which specify the low-level processes underlying automatization.

Loading

Article metrics loading...

/content/journals/10.1146/annurev-psych-122414-033550
2016-01-04
2024-10-13
Loading full text...

Full text loading...

/deliver/fulltext/psych/67/1/annurev-psych-122414-033550.html?itemId=/content/journals/10.1146/annurev-psych-122414-033550&mimeType=html&fmt=ahah

Literature Cited

  1. Andersen SK, Hillyard SA, Müller MM. 2008. Attention facilitates multiple stimulus features in parallel in human visual cortex. Curr. Biol. 18:131006–9 [Google Scholar]
  2. Anderson JR. 1992. Automaticity and the ACT* theory. Am. J. Psychol. 105:165–80 [Google Scholar]
  3. Ansorge U, Neumann O. 2005. Intentions determine the effect of nonconsciously registered visual information: evidence for direct parameter specification in the metacontrast dissociation. J. Exp. Psychol.: Hum. Percept. Perform. 31:762–77 [Google Scholar]
  4. Atas A, Vermeiren A, Cleeremans A. 2013. Repeating a strongly masked stimulus increases priming and awareness. Conscious. Cogn. 22:1422–30 [Google Scholar]
  5. Awh E, Belopolsky AV, Theeuwes J. 2012. Top-down versus bottom-up attentional control: a failed theoretical dichotomy. Trends Cogn. Sci. 16:8437–43 [Google Scholar]
  6. Awh E, Vogel EK, Oh SH. 2006. Interactions between attention and working memory. Neuroscience 139:201–8 [Google Scholar]
  7. Baars BJ. 1988. A Cognitive Theory of Consciousness Cambridge, UK: Cambridge Univ. Press [Google Scholar]
  8. Bargh JA. 1992. The ecology of automaticity: toward establishing the conditions needed to produce automatic processing effects. Am. J. Psychol. 105:181–99 [Google Scholar]
  9. Bargh JA. 1994. The four horsemen of automaticity: awareness, intention, efficiency, and control in social cognition. Handbook of Social Cognition 1 RS Wyer, TK Srull 1–40 Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum [Google Scholar]
  10. Bechara A, Damasio H, Tranel D, Damasio AR. 1997. Deciding advantageously before knowing the advantageous strategy. Science 275:1293–95 [Google Scholar]
  11. Bechtel W. 2008. Mental Mechanisms: Philosophical Perspectives on Cognitive Neuroscience London, UK: Routledge [Google Scholar]
  12. Belopolsky AV, Schreij D, Theeuwes J. 2010. What is top-down about contingent capture?. Atten. Percept. Psychophys. 72:326–41 [Google Scholar]
  13. Bernstein A, Taylor K. 1979. The interaction of stimulus information with potential stimulus significance in eliciting the skin conductance orienting response. The Orienting Reflex in Humans H Kimmel, E van Olst, J Orlebeke 499–519 Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum [Google Scholar]
  14. Block N. 1995. On a confusion about a function of consciousness. Behav. Brain Sci. 18:227–87 [Google Scholar]
  15. Broadbent D. 1958. Perception and Communication London, UK: Pergamon Press [Google Scholar]
  16. Bronfman ZZ, Brezis N, Jacobson H, Usher M. 2014. We see more than we can report: “cost free” color phenomenality outside focal attention. Psychol. Sci. 25:1394–403 [Google Scholar]
  17. Brosch T, Pourtois G, Sander D, Vuilleumier P. 2011. Additive effects of emotional, endogenous, and exogenous attention: behavioral and electrophysiological evidence. Neuropsychologia 49:71779–87 [Google Scholar]
  18. Bruchmann M, Hintze P, Mota S. 2011. The effects of spatial and temporal cueing on metacontrast masking. Adv. Cogn. Psychol. 7:132–41 [Google Scholar]
  19. Carrasco M. 2011. Visual attention: the past 25 years. Vis. Res. 51:1484–525 [Google Scholar]
  20. Carlson RA, Lundy DH. 1992. Consistency and restructuring in cognitive procedural sequences. J. Exp. Psychol.: Learn. Mem. Cogn. 18:127–41 [Google Scholar]
  21. Cave KR, Wolfe JM. 1990. Modeling the role of parallel processing in visual search. Cogn. Psychol. 22:225–71 [Google Scholar]
  22. Charles L, Van Opstal F, Marti S, Dehaene S. 2013. Distinct brain mechanisms for conscious versus subliminal error detection. NeuroImage 73:80–94 [Google Scholar]
  23. Cleeremans A, Jiménez L. 2002. Implicit learning and consciousness: a graded, dynamic perspective. Implicit Learning and Consciousness RM French, A Cleeremans 1–40 Hove, UK: Psychol. Press [Google Scholar]
  24. Corbetta M, Schulman GL. 2002. Controls of goal-directed and S-driven attention in the brain. Nat. Neurosci. 3:201–15 [Google Scholar]
  25. Cunningham WA, Zelazo PD. 2007. Attitudes and evaluations: a social cognitive neuroscience perspective. Trends Cogn. Sci. 11:97–104 [Google Scholar]
  26. De Brigard F, Prinz J. 2010. Attention and consciousness. WIREs Cogn. Sci. 1:51–59 [Google Scholar]
  27. Dehaene S, Naccache L. 2001. Towards a cognitive neuroscience of consciousness: basic evidence and a workspace framework. Cognition 79:1–37 [Google Scholar]
  28. Deutsch JA, Deutsch D. 1963. Attention: some theoretical considerations. Psychol. Rev. 70:80–90 [Google Scholar]
  29. Dijksterhuis A, Aarts H. 2010. Goals, attention, and (un)consciousness. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 61:467–90 [Google Scholar]
  30. Eitam B, Higgins ET. 2010. Motivation in mental accessibility: relevance of a representation (ROAR) as a new framework. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Compass 4:951–67 [Google Scholar]
  31. Engelmann JB, Damaraju E, Padmala S, Pessoa L. 2009. Combined effects of attention and motivation on visual task performance: transient and sustained motivational effects. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 3:1–17 [Google Scholar]
  32. Enns JT, Austen EL, Di Lollo V, Rauschenberger R, Yantis S. 2001. New objects dominate luminance transients in setting attentional priority. J. Exp. Psychol.: Hum. Percept. Perform. 27:1287–302 [Google Scholar]
  33. Folk CL, Remington R. 1998. Selectivity in distraction by irrelevant featural singletons: evidence for two forms of attentional capture. J. Exp. Psychol.: Hum. Percept. Perform. 24:847–58 [Google Scholar]
  34. Folk CL, Remington RW, Johnston JC. 1992. Involuntary covert orienting is contingent on attentional control settings. J. Exp. Psychol.: Hum. Percept. Perform. 18:1030–44 [Google Scholar]
  35. Hahn U, Chater N. 1998. Similarity and rules: Distinct? Exhaustive? Empirically distinguishable?. Cognition 65:197–230 [Google Scholar]
  36. Hassin RR. 2013. Yes it can: on the functional abilities of the human unconscious. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 8:195–207 [Google Scholar]
  37. Hayden BY, Gallant JL. 2009. Combined effects of spatial and feature-based attention on responses of V4 neurons. Vis. Res. 49:101182–87 [Google Scholar]
  38. Hélie S, Waldschmidt JG, Ash FG. 2010. Automaticity in rule-based and information-integration categorization. Atten. Percept. Psychophys. 72:1013–31 [Google Scholar]
  39. Higgins ET. 1996. Knowledge activation: accessibility, applicability, and salience. Social Psychology: Handbook of Basic Principles ET Higgins, AW Kruglanski 133–68 New York: Guilford [Google Scholar]
  40. Hommel B. 2010. Grounding attention in action control: the intentional control of selection. Effortless Attention: A New Perspective in the Cognitive Science of Attention and Action BJ Bruya 121–40 Cambridge, MA: MIT Press [Google Scholar]
  41. Irons JL, Folk CL, Remington RW. 2012. All set! Evidence of simultaneous attentional control settings for multiple target colors. J. Exp. Psychol.: Hum. Percept. Perform. 38:3758–75 [Google Scholar]
  42. Itti L. 2007. Visual salience. Scholarpedia 2:93327 [Google Scholar]
  43. Jáskowski P, Skalska B, Verleger R. 2003. How the self controls its “automatic pilot” when processing of subliminal information. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 15:911–20 [Google Scholar]
  44. Jiang Y, Costello P, Fang F, Huang M, He S. 2006. Gender- and sexual orientation–dependent attentional effect of invisible images. PNAS 103:4517048–52 [Google Scholar]
  45. Kanai R, Tsuchiya N, Verstraten FA. 2006. The scope and limits of top-down attention in unconscious visual processing. Curr. Biol. 16:2332–36 [Google Scholar]
  46. Karremans JC, Stroebe W, Claus J. 2006. Beyond Vicary's fantasies: the impact of subliminal priming and brand choice. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 42:6792–98 [Google Scholar]
  47. Kastner S, Ungerleider LG. 2000. Mechanisms of visual attention in the human cortex. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 23:315–41 [Google Scholar]
  48. Kentridge RW, Nijboer TC, Heywood CA. 2008. Attended but unseen: Visual attention is not sufficient for visual awareness. Neuropsychologia 46:864–69 [Google Scholar]
  49. Kiefer M. 2012. Executive control over unconscious cognition: attentional sensitization of unconscious information processing. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 6:61Reviews studies showing that preemptive control but not reactive control is possible on unconscious stimuli. [Google Scholar]
  50. Kiefer M, Ansorge U, Haynes J-D, Hamker F, Mattler U. et al. 2011. Neuro-cognitive mechanisms of conscious and unconscious visual perception: from a plethora of phenomena to general principles. Adv. Cogn. Psychol. 7:55–67 [Google Scholar]
  51. Kiefer M, Brendel D. 2006. Attentional modulation of unconscious “automatic” processes: evidence from event-related potentials in a masked priming paradigm. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 18:184–98 [Google Scholar]
  52. Kiesel A, Kunde W, Pohl C, Berner MP, Hoffmann J. 2009. Playing chess unconsciously. J. Exp. Psychol.: Learn. Mem. Cogn. 35:292–98 [Google Scholar]
  53. Kiesel A, Kunde W, Pohl C, Hoffmann J. 2006. Priming from novel masked stimuli depends on target set size. Adv. Cogn. Psychol. 2:37–45 [Google Scholar]
  54. Koch C, Tsuchiya N. 2007. Attention and consciousness: two distinct brain processes. Trends Cogn. Sci. 11:16–22 [Google Scholar]
  55. Konstantinou N, Lavie N. 2013. Dissociable roles of different types of working memory load in visual detection. J. Exp. Psychol.: Hum. Percept. Perform. 39:919–24 [Google Scholar]
  56. Kouider S, Dehaene S. 2007. Levels of processing during non-conscious perception: a critical review of visual masking. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B 362:857–75 [Google Scholar]
  57. Kouider S, Dehaene S, Jobert A, Le Bihan D. 2007. Cerebral bases of subliminal and supraliminal priming during reading. Cereb. Cortex 17:2019–29 [Google Scholar]
  58. Kunde W, Kiesel A, Hoffmann J. 2003. Conscious control over the content of unconscious cognition. Cognition 88:223–42 [Google Scholar]
  59. Kunde W, Reuss H, Kiesel A. 2012. Consciousness and cognitive control. Adv. Cogn. Psychol. 8:9–18Reviews studies showing that unconscious stimuli can trigger a response if a stimulus-response association was previously formed. [Google Scholar]
  60. Lachter J, Forster KI, Ruthruff E. 2004. Forty-five years after Broadbent 1958: still no identification without attention. Psychol. Rev. 111:880–913 [Google Scholar]
  61. Lamme VAF, Roelfsema PR. 2000. The distinct modes of vision offered by feedforward and recurrent processing. Trends Neurosci. 23:571–79 [Google Scholar]
  62. Lamy DF, Kristjánsson A. 2013. Is goal-directed attentional guidance just intertrial priming? A review. J. Vis. 13:314Much but not all evidence for the influence of goals on attention can be explained by intertrial priming. [Google Scholar]
  63. Lau HC, Passingham RE. 2007. Unconscious activation of the cognitive control system in the human prefrontal cortex. J. Neurosci. 27:215805–11 [Google Scholar]
  64. Libera CD, Chelazzi L. 2006. Visual selective attention and the effects of monetary reward. Psychol. Sci. 17:3222–27 [Google Scholar]
  65. Lin Z, Murray SO. 2013. Visible propagation from invisible exogenous cueing. J. Vis. 13:1112 [Google Scholar]
  66. Ling S, Carrasco M. 2006. When sustained attention impairs perception. Nat. Neurosci. 9:101243–45 [Google Scholar]
  67. Lisman J, Grace AA, Duzel E. 2011. A neo-Hebbian framework for episodic memory; role of dopamine-dependent late LTP. Trends Neurosci. 34:10536–47 [Google Scholar]
  68. Logan GD. 1988. Toward an instance theory of automatization. Psychol. Rev. 95:492–527 [Google Scholar]
  69. Macdonald JS, Lavie N. 2008. Load induced blindness. J. Exp. Psychol.: Hum. Percept. Perform. 34:51078–91 [Google Scholar]
  70. Mack A, Rock I. 1998. Inattentional Blindness Cambridge, MA: MIT Press [Google Scholar]
  71. Macknik SL, Livingstone MS. 1998. Neuronal correlates of visibility and invisibility in the primate visual system. Nat. Neurosci. 1:144–49 [Google Scholar]
  72. Marr D. 1982. Vision: A Computational Investigation into the Human Representation and Processing of Visual Information New York: Freeman [Google Scholar]
  73. Martens U, Ansorge U, Kiefer M. 2011. Controlling the unconscious: attentional task sets modulate subliminal semantic and visuo-motor processes differentially. Psychol. Sci. 22:282–91 [Google Scholar]
  74. Martens U, Kiefer M. 2009. Specifying attentional top-down influences on subsequent unconscious semantic processing. Adv. Cogn. Psychol. 5:56–68 [Google Scholar]
  75. Mathewson KE, Gratton G, Fabiani M, Beck DM, Ro T. 2009. To see or not to see: Prestimulus alpha phase predicts visual awareness. J. Neurosci. 29:2725–32 [Google Scholar]
  76. Mattler U. 2003. Priming of mental operations by masked stimuli. Percept. Psychophys. 65:167–87 [Google Scholar]
  77. Maunsell JHR, Treue S. 2006. Feature-based attention in visual cortex. Trends Neurosci. 6:295–346 [Google Scholar]
  78. McCann RS, Folk CL, Johnston JC. 1992. The role of spatial attention in visual word processing. J. Exp. Psychol.: Hum. Percept. Perform. 18:1015–29 [Google Scholar]
  79. Milner AD, Goodale MA. 1995. The Visual Brain in Action Oxford, UK: Oxford Univ. Press [Google Scholar]
  80. Mogg K, Bradley BP, Hyare H, Lee S. 1998. Selective attention to food-related stimuli in hunger: Are attentional biases specific to emotional and psychopathological states, or are they also found in normal drive states?. Behav. Res. Ther. 36:227–37 [Google Scholar]
  81. Moors A. 2014. Examining the mapping problem in dual process models. Dual Process Theories of the Social Mind JW Sherman, B Gawronski, Y Trope 20–34 New York: Guilford [Google Scholar]
  82. Moors A, De Houwer J. 2006a. Automaticity: a theoretical and conceptual analysis. Psychol. Bull. 132:297–326In-depth analysis of the concepts un/intentional, un/controlled, goal in/dependent, non/autonomous, not/purely stimulus driven, non/efficient, un/conscious, and fast/slow. [Google Scholar]
  83. Moors A, De Houwer J. 2006b. Problems with dividing the realm of processes. Psychol. Inq. 17:199–204 [Google Scholar]
  84. Moors A, Spruyt A, De Houwer J. 2010. In search of a measure that qualifies as implicit: recommendations based on a decompositional view of automaticity. Handbook of Implicit Social Cognition: Measurement, Theory, and Applications B Gawronski, KB Payne 19–37 New York: Guilford [Google Scholar]
  85. Moutoussis K, Zeki S. 2002. The relationship between cortical activation and perception investigated with invisible stimuli. PNAS 99:9527–32 [Google Scholar]
  86. Mudrik L, Breska A, Lamy D, Deouell LY. 2011. Integration without awareness: expanding the limits of unconscious processing. Psychol. Sci. 22:6764–70 [Google Scholar]
  87. Mudrik L, Faivre N, Koch C. 2014. Information integration without awareness. Trends Cogn. Sci. 18:9488–96Reviews evidence for unconscious information integration and suggests potential moderators such as distance and novelty. [Google Scholar]
  88. Mulckhuyse M, Theeuwes J. 2010. Unconscious attentional orienting to exogenous cues: a review of the literature. Acta Psychol. 134:3299–309 [Google Scholar]
  89. Müller NG, Strumpf H, Scholz M, Baier B, Melloni L. 2013. Repetition suppression versus enhancement—it's quantity that matters. Cereb. Cortex 23:315–22 [Google Scholar]
  90. Müsseler J, Hommel B. 1997. Blindness to response-compatible stimuli. J. Exp. Psychol.: Hum. Percept. Perform. 23:861–72 [Google Scholar]
  91. Nieuwenhuis S, De Kleijn R. 2011. Consciousness of targets during the attentional blink: a gradual or all-or-none dimension?. Atten. Percept. Psychophys. 73:364–73 [Google Scholar]
  92. Öhman A. 1992. Orienting and attention: preferred preattentive processing of potentially phobic stimuli. Attention and Information Processing in Infants and Adults BA Campbell, H Hayne, R Richardson 263–95 Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum [Google Scholar]
  93. O'Regan JK, Noë A. 2001. A sensorimotor account of vision and visual consciousness. Behav. Brain Sci. 24:939–1031 [Google Scholar]
  94. Pessoa L, McKenna M, Gutierrez E, Ungerleider LG. 2002. Neural processing of emotional faces requires attention. PNAS 99:11458–63 [Google Scholar]
  95. Posner MI. 1980. Orienting of attention. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 32:3–25 [Google Scholar]
  96. Posner MI, Snyder CR, Davidson BJ. 1980. Attention and the detection of signals. J. Exp. Psychol. 109:2160–74 [Google Scholar]
  97. Posner MI, Snyder CRR. 1975. Attention and cognitive control. Information Processing and Cognition: The Loyola Symposium RL Solso 153–75 Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum [Google Scholar]
  98. Pourtois G, Schettino A, Vuilleumier P. 2013. Brain mechanisms for emotional influence on perception and attention: what is magic and what is not. Biol. Psychol. 92:3492–512 [Google Scholar]
  99. Prinz J. 2004. Gut Reactions: A Perceptual Theory of Emotion Oxford, UK: Oxford Univ. Press [Google Scholar]
  100. Prinz J. 2010. When is perception conscious?. Perceiving the World: New Essays on Perception B Nanay 310–32 Oxford, UK: Oxford Univ. Press [Google Scholar]
  101. Prinz J. 2011. Is attention necessary or sufficient for consciousness?. Attention: Philosophical and Psychological Essays C Mole, D Smithies, W Wu 174–204 Oxford, UK: Oxford Univ. Press [Google Scholar]
  102. Ranganath C, Rainer G. 2003. Neural mechanisms for detecting and remembering novel events. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 4:193–202 [Google Scholar]
  103. Raymond JE, Shapiro KL, Arnell KM. 1992. Temporary suppression of visual processing in an RSVP task: an attentional blink?. J. Exp. Psychol.: Hum. Percept. Perform. 18:849–60 [Google Scholar]
  104. Rensink RA, O'Regan JK, Clark JJ. 1997. To see or not to see: the need for attention to perceive changes in scenes. Psychol. Sci. 8:5368–73 [Google Scholar]
  105. Reynolds JH, Pasternak T, Desimone R. 2000. Attention increases sensitivity of V4 neurons. Neuron 37:853–63 [Google Scholar]
  106. Ric F, Muller D. 2012. Unconscious addition: when we unconsciously initiate and follow arithmetic rules. J. Exp. Psychol.: Gen. 141:222–26 [Google Scholar]
  107. Risko EF, Stolz JA, Besner D. 2011. Basic processes in reading: on the relation between spatial attention and familiarity. Lang. Cogn. Process. 26:47–62 [Google Scholar]
  108. Sander D, Grafman J, Zalla T. 2003. The human amygdala: an evolved system for relevance detection. Rev. Neurosci. 14:303–16 [Google Scholar]
  109. Schmidt F, Haberkamp A, Schmidt T. 2011. Dos and don'ts in response priming research. Adv. Cogn. Psychol. 7:120–31 [Google Scholar]
  110. Schmidt F, Schmidt T. 2010. Feature-based attention to unconscious shapes and colors. Atten. Percept. Psychophys. 72:1480–94 [Google Scholar]
  111. Schmidt T, Niehaus S, Nagel A. 2006. Primes and targets in rapid chases: tracing sequential waves of motor activation. Behav. Neurosci. 120:1005–16 [Google Scholar]
  112. Serences JT, Boynton GM. 2007. Feature-based attentional modulations in the absence of direct visual stimulation. Neuron 55:301–12 [Google Scholar]
  113. Sergent C, Dehaene S. 2004. Is consciousness a gradual phenomenon? Evidence for an all-or-none bifurcation during the attentional blink. Psychol. Sci. 15:720–28 [Google Scholar]
  114. Shallice T. 1988. Information-processing models of consciousness: possibilities and problems. Consciousness in Contemporary Science AJ Marcel, E Bisiach 305–33 Oxford, UK: Oxford Univ. Press [Google Scholar]
  115. Shiffrin RM. 1988. Attention. Stevens' Handbook of Experimental Psychology 2 RC Atkinson, RJ Hernstein, G Lindzey, RD Luce 739–11 New York: Wiley [Google Scholar]
  116. Sloman SA. 1996. The empirical case for two systems of reasoning. Psychol. Bull. 119:3–22 [Google Scholar]
  117. Smith ER, Lerner M. 1986. Development of automatism of social judgements. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 50:246–59 [Google Scholar]
  118. Spelke ES, Hirst WC, Neisser U. 1976. Skills of divided attention. Cognition 4:215–30 [Google Scholar]
  119. Sperling G. 1960. The information available in brief visual presentations. Psychol. Monogr. 74:1–29 [Google Scholar]
  120. Spruyt A, De Houwer J, Everaert T, Hermans D. 2012. Unconscious semantic activation depends on feature-specific attention allocation. Cognition 122:91–95 [Google Scholar]
  121. Sumner P, Tsai PC, Yu K, Nachev P. 2006. Attentional modulation of sensorimotor processes in the absence of perceptual awareness. PNAS 103:10520–25 [Google Scholar]
  122. Tapia E, Breitmeyer BG, Broyles EC. 2011. Properties of spatial attention in conscious and nonconscious visual information processing. Conscious. Cogn. 20:426–31 [Google Scholar]
  123. Tapia E, Breitmeyer BG, Jacob J, Broyles EC. 2013. Spatial attention effects during conscious and nonconscious processing of visual features and objects. J. Exp. Psychol.: Hum. Percept. Perform. 39:745–56 [Google Scholar]
  124. Tapia E, Breitmeyer BG, Schooner CR. 2010. Role of task-directed attention in nonconscious and conscious response priming by form and color. J. Exp. Psychol.: Hum. Percept. Perform. 36:74–87 [Google Scholar]
  125. Theeuwes J. 2010. Top-down and bottom-up control of visual selection. Acta Psychol. 135:77–99 [Google Scholar]
  126. Treisman A, Vieira A, Hayes A. 1992. Automaticity and preattentive processing. Am. J. Psychol. 105:341–62 [Google Scholar]
  127. Tzelgov J, Yehene V, Kotler L, Alon A. 2000. Automatic comparisons of artificial digits never compared: learning linear ordering relations. J. Exp. Psychol.: Learn. Mem. Cogn. 26:103–20 [Google Scholar]
  128. Tzur B, Frost R. 2007. SOA does not reveal the absolute time course of cognitive processing in fast priming experiments. J. Mem. Lang. 56:321–35 [Google Scholar]
  129. Uleman JS. 1989. A framework for thinking intentionally about unintended thoughts. Unintended Thought JS Uleman, JA Bargh 425–49 New York: Guilford [Google Scholar]
  130. van Boxtel JJ, Tsuchiya N, Koch C. 2010. Consciousness and attention: on sufficiency and necessity. Front. Psychol. 1:217 [Google Scholar]
  131. van Gaal S, Ridderinkhof KR, Fahrenfort JJ, Scholte HS, Lamme VAF. 2008. Frontal cortex mediates unconsciously triggered inhibitory control. J. Neurosci. 28:8053–62 [Google Scholar]
  132. van Gaal S, Ridderinkhof KR, van den Wildenberg WPM, Lamme VAF. 2009. Dissociating consciousness from inhibitory control: evidence for unconsciously triggered response inhibition in the stop-signal task. J. Exp. Psychol.: Hum. Percept. Perform. 35:1129–39 [Google Scholar]
  133. Van Opstal F, Gevers W, Osman M, Verguts T. 2010. Unconscious task application. Conscious. Cogn. 19:999–1006 [Google Scholar]
  134. Verwijmeren T, Karremans JC, Benritte SF, Stroebe W, Wigboldus DHJ. 2013. Warning: You are being primed! The effect of a warning on the impact of subliminal ads. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 49:1124–29 [Google Scholar]
  135. Vogt J, De Houwer J, Moors A, Van Damme S, Crombez G. 2010. The automatic orienting of attention to goal-relevant stimuli. Acta Psychol. 134:61–69 [Google Scholar]
  136. Vorberg D, Mattler U, Heinecke A, Schmidt T, Schwarzbach J. 2003. Invariant time course of priming with and without awareness. Psychophysics Beyond Sensation: Laws and Invariants of Human Cognition C Kaernbach, E Schröger, H Müller 271–88 Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum [Google Scholar]
  137. Wolfe JM. 1999. Inattentional amnesia. Fleeting Memories V Coltheart 71–94 Cambridge, MA: MIT Press [Google Scholar]
  138. Wolfe JM, Butcher SJ, Lee C, Hyle M. 2003. Changing your mind: on the contributions of top-down and bottom-up guidance in visual search for feature singletons. J. Exp. Psychol.: Hum. Percept. Perform 29:483–502 [Google Scholar]
  139. Wolfe JM, Horowitz TS. 2004. What attributes guide the deployment of attention and how do they do it?. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 5:6495–501 [Google Scholar]
  140. Wu S-C, Remington RW, Folk CL. 2014. Onsets do not override top-down goals but they are responded to more quickly. Atten. Percept. Psychophys. 76:649–54 [Google Scholar]
/content/journals/10.1146/annurev-psych-122414-033550
Loading
/content/journals/10.1146/annurev-psych-122414-033550
Loading

Data & Media loading...

  • Article Type: Review Article
This is a required field
Please enter a valid email address
Approval was a Success
Invalid data
An Error Occurred
Approval was partially successful, following selected items could not be processed due to error