1932

Abstract

Why has computational psychiatry yet to influence routine clinical practice? One reason may be that it has neglected context and temporal dynamics in the models of certain mental health problems. We develop three heuristics for estimating whether time and context are important to a mental health problem: Is it characterized by a core neurobiological mechanism? Does it follow a straightforward natural trajectory? And is intentional mental content peripheral to the problem? For many problems the answers are no, suggesting that modeling time and context is critical. We review computational psychiatry advances toward this end, including modeling state variation, using domain-specific stimuli, and interpreting differences in context. We discuss complementary network and complex systems approaches. Novel methods and unification with adjacent fields may inspire a new generation of computational psychiatry.

Loading

Article metrics loading...

/content/journals/10.1146/annurev-psych-021621-124910
2022-01-04
2024-04-26
Loading full text...

Full text loading...

/deliver/fulltext/psych/73/1/annurev-psych-021621-124910.html?itemId=/content/journals/10.1146/annurev-psych-021621-124910&mimeType=html&fmt=ahah

Literature Cited

  1. Agrawal M, Mattar MG, Cohen JD, Daw ND. 2020. The temporal dynamics of opportunity costs: A normative account of cognitive fatigue and boredom. bioRxiv 2877276. https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.09.08.287276
    [Crossref]
  2. Am. Psychiatr. Assoc 2013. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders Arlington, VA: Am. Psychiatr. Publ, 5th ed.. [DSM-5]
  3. Andrews PW, Thomson JA Jr. 2009. The bright side of being blue: depression as an adaptation for analyzing complex problems. Psychol. Rev. 116:3620–54Makes the case that depressed mood and rumination facilitate recovery and elicit needed social support.
    [Google Scholar]
  4. Armour C, Műllerová J, Elhai JD. 2015. A systematic literature review of PTSD's latent structure in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders: DSM-IV to DSM-5. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 44:60–74
    [Google Scholar]
  5. Baker CL, Jara-Ettinger J, Saxe R, Tenenbaum JB 2017. Rational quantitative attribution of beliefs, desires and percepts in human mentalizing. Nat. Hum. Behav. 1:0064
    [Google Scholar]
  6. Barlow DH, Hersen M. 1973. Single-case experimental designs: uses in applied clinical research. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 29:3319–25
    [Google Scholar]
  7. Beeler JA, Frank MJ, McDaid J, Alexander E, Turkson S et al. 2012. A role for dopamine-mediated learning in the pathophysiology and treatment of Parkinson's disease. Cell Rep 2:61747–61
    [Google Scholar]
  8. Beevers CG, Mullarkey MC, Dainer-Best J, Stewart RA, Labrada J et al. 2019. Association between negative cognitive bias and depression: a symptom-level approach. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 128:3212–27
    [Google Scholar]
  9. Beltz AM, Gates KM. 2017. Network mapping with GIMME. Multivar. Behav. Res. 52:6789–804
    [Google Scholar]
  10. Borsboom D. 2008. Psychometric perspectives on diagnostic systems. J. Clin. Psychol. 64:91089–108
    [Google Scholar]
  11. Boyd R. 1991. Realism, anti-foundationalism and the enthusiasm for natural kinds. Philos. Stud. 61:1–2127–48
    [Google Scholar]
  12. Brick C, Hood B, Ekroll V, de-Wit L. 2021. Illusory essences: a bias holding back theorizing in psychological science. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. In press
    [Google Scholar]
  13. Bringmann LF, Eronen MI. 2018. Don't blame the model: reconsidering the network approach to psychopathology. Psychol. Rev. 125:4606–15
    [Google Scholar]
  14. Bringmann LF, Vissers N, Wichers M, Geschwind N, Kuppens P et al. 2013. A network approach to psychopathology: new insights into clinical longitudinal data. PLOS ONE 8:4e60188
    [Google Scholar]
  15. Brown VM, Zhu L, Wang JM, Frueh BC, King-Casas B, Chiu PH 2018. Associability-modulated loss learning is increased in posttraumatic stress disorder. eLife 7:e30150
    [Google Scholar]
  16. Browning M, Carter CS, Chatham C, Den Ouden H, Gillan CM et al. 2020. Realizing the clinical potential of computational psychiatry: report from the Banbury Center Meeting, February 2019. Biol. Psychiatry 88:2e5–10
    [Google Scholar]
  17. Burger J, van der Veen DC, Robinaugh DJ, Quax R, Riese H et al. 2020. Bridging the gap between complexity science and clinical practice by formalizing idiographic theories: a computational model of functional analysis. BMC Med 18:199
    [Google Scholar]
  18. Cai N, Choi KW, Fried EI. 2020. Reviewing the genetics of heterogeneity in depression: operationalizations, manifestations and etiologies. Hum. Mol. Genet. 29:R1R10–18
    [Google Scholar]
  19. Cavanagh JF, Wiecki TV, Cohen MX, Figueroa CM, Samanta J et al. 2011. Subthalamic nucleus stimulation reverses mediofrontal influence over decision threshold. Nat. Neurosci. 14:111462–67
    [Google Scholar]
  20. Cicchetti D, Rogosch FA. 1996. Equifinality and multifinality in developmental psychopathology. Dev. Psychopathol. 8:4597–600
    [Google Scholar]
  21. Cohen JD, Servan-Schreiber D. 1992. Context, cortex, and dopamine: a connectionist approach to behavior and biology in schizophrenia. Psychol. Rev. 99:145–77
    [Google Scholar]
  22. Cohen RT, Kahana MJ. 2020. A memory-based theory of emotional disorders. bioRxiv 817486. https://doi.org/10.1101/817486
    [Crossref]
  23. Collins AGE, Albrecht MA, Waltz JA, Gold JM, Frank MJ. 2017. Interactions among working memory, reinforcement learning, and effort in value-based choice: a new paradigm and selective deficits in schizophrenia. Biol. Psychiatry 82:6431–39
    [Google Scholar]
  24. Collins AGE, Frank MJ. 2014. Opponent actor learning (OpAL): modeling interactive effects of striatal dopamine on reinforcement learning and choice incentive. Psychol. Rev. 121:3337–66
    [Google Scholar]
  25. Cools R, Barker RA, Sahakian BJ, Robbins TW. 2001. Enhanced or impaired cognitive function in Parkinson's disease as a function of dopaminergic medication and task demands. Cereb. Cortex 11:121136–43
    [Google Scholar]
  26. Dasgupta I, Gershman SJ. 2021. Memory as a computational resource. Trends Cogn. Sci. 25:3240–51
    [Google Scholar]
  27. Daw ND, Gershman SJ, Seymour B, Dayan P, Dolan RJ 2011. Model-based influences on humans’ choices and striatal prediction errors. Neuron 69:61204–15
    [Google Scholar]
  28. Daw ND, Niv Y, Dayan P. 2005. Uncertainty-based competition between prefrontal and dorsolateral striatal systems for behavioral control. Nat. Neurosci. 8:121704–11
    [Google Scholar]
  29. Dayan P, Huys QJM. 2008. Serotonin, inhibition, and negative mood. PLOS Comput. Biol. 4:2e4
    [Google Scholar]
  30. Decker JH, Otto AR, Daw ND, Hartley CA. 2016. From creatures of habit to goal-directed learners: tracking the developmental emergence of model-based reinforcement learning. Psychol. Sci. 27:6848–58
    [Google Scholar]
  31. Dowd EC, Frank MJ, Collins A, Gold JM, Barch DM 2016. Probabilistic reinforcement learning in patients with schizophrenia: relationships to anhedonia and avolition. Biol. Psychiatry Cogn. Neurosci. Neuroimaging 1:5460–73
    [Google Scholar]
  32. Drysdale AT, Grosenick L, Downar J, Dunlop K, Mansouri F et al. 2017. Resting-state connectivity biomarkers define neurophysiological subtypes of depression. Nat. Med. 23:128–38
    [Google Scholar]
  33. Durstewitz D, Huys QJM, Koppe G. 2020. Psychiatric illnesses as disorders of network dynamics. Biol. Psychiatry Cogn. Neurosci. Neuroimaging 6:986576
    [Google Scholar]
  34. Durstewitz D, Seamans JK. 2008. The dual-state theory of prefrontal cortex dopamine function with relevance to catechol-O-methyltransferase genotypes and schizophrenia. Biol. Psychiatry 64:9739–49
    [Google Scholar]
  35. Egerton A, Valmaggia LR, Howes OD, Day F, Chaddock CA et al. 2016. Adversity in childhood linked to elevated striatal dopamine function in adulthood. Schizophr. Res. 176:2–3171–76
    [Google Scholar]
  36. Eldar E, Niv Y. 2015. Interaction between emotional state and learning underlies mood instability. Nat. Commun 6:6149Shows a coupling between affect and appraisal and links individual differences therein to hypomanic symptoms.
    [Google Scholar]
  37. Eldar E, Roth C, Dayan P, Dolan RJ 2018. Decodability of reward learning signals predicts mood fluctuations. Curr. Biol. 28:91433–39.e7
    [Google Scholar]
  38. Eronen MI. 2019. The levels problem in psychopathology. Psychol. Med 51:692733
    [Google Scholar]
  39. Eronen MI. 2020a. Causal discovery and the problem of psychological interventions. New Ideas Psychol 59:100785
    [Google Scholar]
  40. Eronen MI. 2020b. Interventionism for the intentional stance: true believers and their brains. Topoi 39:145–55
    [Google Scholar]
  41. Frank MJ. 2005. Dynamic dopamine modulation in the basal ganglia: a neurocomputational account of cognitive deficits in medicated and nonmedicated Parkinsonism. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 17:151–72
    [Google Scholar]
  42. Frank MJ, Claus ED. 2006. Anatomy of a decision: striato-orbitofrontal interactions in reinforcement learning, decision making, and reversal. Psychol. Rev. 113:2300–26
    [Google Scholar]
  43. Frank MJ, Gagne C, Nyhus E, Masters S, Wiecki TV et al. 2015. fMRI and EEG predictors of dynamic decision parameters during human reinforcement learning. J. Neurosci. 35:2485–94
    [Google Scholar]
  44. Frank MJ, Samanta J, Moustafa AA, Sherman SJ. 2007. Hold your horses: impulsivity, deep brain stimulation, and medication in parkinsonism. Science 318:58541309–12
    [Google Scholar]
  45. Frank MJ, Seeberger LC, O'Reilly RC. 2004. By carrot or by stick: cognitive reinforcement learning in parkinsonism. Science 306:57031940–43Shows a mechanistic learning difference in the high-essentiality problem Parkinson's disease.
    [Google Scholar]
  46. Frey A-L, Frank MJ, McCabe C 2021. Social reinforcement learning as a predictor of real-life experiences in individuals with high and low depressive symptomatology. Psychol. Med 51:3408–15
    [Google Scholar]
  47. Frey A-L, McCabe C 2020a. Effects of serotonin and dopamine depletion on neural prediction computations during social learning. Neuropsychopharmacology 45:91431–37
    [Google Scholar]
  48. Frey A-L, McCabe C 2020b. Impaired social learning predicts reduced real-life motivation in individuals with depression: a computational fMRI study. J. Affect. Disord. 263:698–706
    [Google Scholar]
  49. Fried EI, Cramer AOJ. 2017. Moving forward: challenges and directions for psychopathological network theory and methodology. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 12:6999–1020
    [Google Scholar]
  50. Fried EI, Nesse RM. 2015. Depression sum-scores don't add up: why analyzing specific depression symptoms is essential. BMC Med 13:72
    [Google Scholar]
  51. Geana A, Barch DM, Gold JM, Carter CS, MacDonald AW III et al. 2021. Using computational modelling to capture schizophrenia-specific reinforcement learning differences and their implications on patient classification. Biol. PsychiatryIn press. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bpsc.2021.03.017
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  52. Gelman SA. 2004. Psychological essentialism in children. Trends Cogn. Sci. 8:9404–9
    [Google Scholar]
  53. Gershman SJ, Horvitz EJ, Tenenbaum JB. 2015. Computational rationality: a converging paradigm for intelligence in brains, minds, and machines. Science 349:6245273–78
    [Google Scholar]
  54. Gillan CM, Fineberg NA, Robbins TW. 2017. A trans-diagnostic perspective on obsessive-compulsive disorder. Psychol. Med. 47:91528–48
    [Google Scholar]
  55. Gillan CM, Kosinski M, Whelan R, Phelps EA, Daw ND 2016. Characterizing a psychiatric symptom dimension related to deficits in goal-directed control. eLife 5:e11305
    [Google Scholar]
  56. Gillan CM, Otto AR, Phelps EA, Daw ND. 2015. Model-based learning protects against forming habits. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 15:3523–36
    [Google Scholar]
  57. Gillan CM, Rutledge RB 2021. Smartphones and the neuroscience of mental health. Annu. Rev. Neurosci 44:12951Two computational psychiatrists make the case for the importance of smartphones in advancing mental-health research.
    [Google Scholar]
  58. Gillan CM, Seow TXF 2020. Carving out new transdiagnostic dimensions for research in mental health. Biol. Psychiatry Cogn. Neurosci. Neuroimaging 5:10932–34
    [Google Scholar]
  59. Gold JM, Waltz JA, Frank MJ. 2015. Effort cost computation in schizophrenia: a commentary on the recent literature. Biol. Psychiatry 78:11747–53
    [Google Scholar]
  60. Gold JM, Waltz JA, Matveeva TM, Kasanova Z, Strauss GP et al. 2012. Negative symptoms and the failure to represent the expected reward value of actions: behavioral and computational modeling evidence. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 69:2129–38
    [Google Scholar]
  61. Goldstein BL, Klein DN. 2014. A review of selected candidate endophenotypes for depression. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 34:5417–27
    [Google Scholar]
  62. Grahek I, Shenhav A, Musslick S, Krebs RM, Koster EH. 2019. Motivation and cognitive control in depression. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev 102:371–81Analyzes the possible sources of apparent cognitive control impairments in depression.
    [Google Scholar]
  63. Gu X, FitzGerald THB, Friston KJ 2019. Modeling subjective belief states in computational psychiatry: interoceptive inference as a candidate framework. Psychopharm 236:82405–12
    [Google Scholar]
  64. Gueguen MCM, Schweitzer EM, Konova AB. 2021. Computational theory-driven studies of reinforcement learning and decision-making in addiction: What have we learned?. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 38:40–48
    [Google Scholar]
  65. Hammen C. 2005. Stress and depression. Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 1:293–319
    [Google Scholar]
  66. Haslbeck JMB, Borsboom D, Waldorp LJ. 2021. Moderated network models. Multivar. Behav. Res 56:225687
    [Google Scholar]
  67. Haslbeck JMB, Fried EI. 2017. How predictable are symptoms in psychopathological networks? A reanalysis of 18 published datasets. Psychol. Med 47:162767–76
    [Google Scholar]
  68. Haslbeck JMB, Ryan O, Robinaugh D, Waldorp L, Borsboom D 2019. Modeling psychopathology: from data models to formal theories. PsyArXiv, Dec. 10: https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/jgm7f
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  69. Henry TR, Robinaugh D, Fried EI 2020. On the control of psychological networks. PsyArXiv, Apr 1 https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/7vpz2
    [Crossref]
  70. Herz DM, Zavala BA, Bogacz R, Brown P. 2016. Neural correlates of decision thresholds in the human subthalamic nucleus. Curr. Biol. 26:7916–20
    [Google Scholar]
  71. Hitchcock P, Forman E, Rothstein NJ, Zhang F, Kounios J et al. 2021. Rumination derails reinforcement learning with possible implications for ineffective behavior. Clin. Psychol. SciIn press Argues that rumination may impede effective behavior in specific situations by impairing trial-and-error learning.
    [Google Scholar]
  72. Hofmann SG, Hayes SC. 2019. The future of intervention science: process-based therapy. Clin. Psychol. Sci. 7:137–50
    [Google Scholar]
  73. Hunter LE, Meer EA, Gillan CM, Hsu M, Daw ND. 2019. Excessive deliberation in social anxiety. bioRxiv 522433. https://doi.org/10.1101/522433
    [Crossref]
  74. Huys QJM, Browning M, Paulus MP, Frank MJ. 2021. Advances in the computational understanding of mental illness. Neuropsychopharmacology 46:13–19
    [Google Scholar]
  75. Huys QJM, Daw ND, Dayan P. 2015. Depression: a decision-theoretic analysis. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 38:1–23
    [Google Scholar]
  76. Huys QJM, Maia TV, Frank MJ 2016. Computational psychiatry as a bridge from neuroscience to clinical applications. Nat. Neurosci. 19:3404–13
    [Google Scholar]
  77. Huys QJM, Pizzagalli DA, Bogdan R, Dayan P 2013. Mapping anhedonia onto reinforcement learning: a behavioural meta-analysis. Biol. Mood Anxiety Disord. 3:12
    [Google Scholar]
  78. Kaiser RH, Andrews-Hanna JR, Wager TD, Pizzagalli DA. 2015. Large-scale network dysfunction in major depressive disorder: a meta-analysis of resting-state functional connectivity. JAMA Psychiatry 72:6603–11
    [Google Scholar]
  79. Kapur S. 2003. Psychosis as a state of aberrant salience: a framework linking biology, phenomenology, and pharmacology in schizophrenia. Am. J. Psychiatry 160:113–23
    [Google Scholar]
  80. Kendler KS. 2005. Toward a philosophical structure for psychiatry. Am. J. Psychiatry 162:3433–40
    [Google Scholar]
  81. Kendler KS, Gardner CO, Gatz M, Pedersen NL. 2007. The sources of co-morbidity between major depression and generalized anxiety disorder in a Swedish national twin sample. Psychol. Med. 37:3453–62
    [Google Scholar]
  82. Kendler KS, Hettema JM, Butera F, Gardner CO, Prescott CA 2003. Life event dimensions of loss, humiliation, entrapment, and danger in the prediction of onsets of major depression and generalized anxiety. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 60:8789–96Shows some specificity in the life events precipitating major depression and generalized anxiety (humiliation and endangerment, respectively).
    [Google Scholar]
  83. Kendler KS, Thornton LM, Gardner CO 2000. Stressful life events and previous episodes in the etiology of major depression in women: an evaluation of the “kindling” hypothesis. Am. J. Psychiatry 157:81243–51
    [Google Scholar]
  84. Kendler KS, Zachar P, Craver C. 2011. What kinds of things are psychiatric disorders?. Psychol. Med. 41:61143–50
    [Google Scholar]
  85. Keramati M, Smittenaar P. 2016. Adaptive integration of habits into depth-limited planning defines a habitual-goal-directed spectrum. PNAS 113:4512868–73
    [Google Scholar]
  86. Keren H, O'Callaghan G, Vidal-Ribas P, Buzzell GA, Brotman MA et al. 2018. Reward processing in depression: a conceptual and meta-analytic review across fMRI and EEG studies. Am. J. Psychiatry 175:111111–20
    [Google Scholar]
  87. Kievit RA, Frankenhuis WE, Waldorp LJ, Borsboom D. 2013. Simpson's paradox in psychological science: a practical guide. Front. Psychol. 4:513
    [Google Scholar]
  88. Konova AB, Lopez-Guzman S, Urmanche A, Ross S, Louie K et al. 2020. Computational markers of risky decision-making for identification of temporal windows of vulnerability to opioid use in a real-world clinical setting. JAMA Psychiatry 77:4368–77Remarkable study showing that variation in the propensity to take ambiguous risk predicts prospective drug use.
    [Google Scholar]
  89. Kotov R, Krueger RF, Watson D, Achenbach TM, Althoff RR et al. 2017. The Hierarchical Taxonomy of Psychopathology (HiTOP): a dimensional alternative to traditional nosologies. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 126:4454–77
    [Google Scholar]
  90. Kozak MJ, Cuthbert BN. 2016. The NIMH research domain criteria initiative: background, issues, and pragmatics. Psychophysiology 53:3286–97
    [Google Scholar]
  91. Lamba A, Frank MJ, FeldmanHall O. 2020. Anxiety impedes adaptive social learning under uncertainty. Psychol. Sci. 31:5592–603
    [Google Scholar]
  92. Leising D, Burger J, Zimmermann J, Bäckström M, Oltmanns JR, Connelly BS. 2020. Why do items correlate with one another? A conceptual analysis with relevance for general factors and network models. PsyArXiv, Aug 8 https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/7c895
    [Crossref]
  93. Lieder F, Goodman ND, Huys QJM. 2013. Learned helplessness and generalization. In Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science SocietyVol 35:pp. 9005 Austin, TX: Cogn. Sci. Soc.
    [Google Scholar]
  94. Litz BT, Stein N, Delaney E, Lebowitz L, Nash WP et al. 2009. Moral injury and moral repair in war veterans: a preliminary model and intervention strategy. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 29:8695–706
    [Google Scholar]
  95. Lydon-Staley DM, Cornblath EJ, Blevins AS, Bassett DS. 2021. Modeling brain, symptom, and behavior in the winds of change. Neuropsychopharmacology 46:120–32
    [Google Scholar]
  96. Maia TV, Frank MJ 2011. From reinforcement learning models to psychiatric and neurological disorders. Nat. Neurosci. 14:2154–62
    [Google Scholar]
  97. Maia TV, Frank MJ 2017. An integrative perspective on the role of dopamine in schizophrenia. Biol. Psychiatry 81:152–66
    [Google Scholar]
  98. Mason L, Eldar E, Rutledge RB 2017. Mood instability and reward dysregulation—a neurocomputational model of bipolar disorder. JAMA Psychiatry 74:121275–76
    [Google Scholar]
  99. Matar E, Shine JM, Gilat M, Ehgoetz Martens KA, Ward PB et al. 2019. Identifying the neural correlates of doorway freezing in Parkinson's disease. Hum. Brain Mapp. 40:72055–64
    [Google Scholar]
  100. McCabe C. 2018. Linking anhedonia symptoms with behavioural and neural reward responses in adolescent depression. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 22:143–51
    [Google Scholar]
  101. McClure SM, Laibson DI, Loewenstein G, Cohen JD. 2004. Separate neural systems value immediate and delayed monetary rewards. Science 306:5695503–7
    [Google Scholar]
  102. McCutcheon RA, Marques TR, Howes OD. 2020. Schizophrenia—an overview. JAMA Psychiatry 77:2201–10
    [Google Scholar]
  103. McNally RJ. 2001. On Wakefield's harmful dysfunction analysis of mental disorder. Behav. Res. Ther. 39:3309–14
    [Google Scholar]
  104. McNally RJ. 2011. What Is Mental Illness? Cambridge, MA: Harvard Univ. Press
    [Google Scholar]
  105. McNally RJ. 2021. Network analysis of psychopathology: controversies and challenges. Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 17:31–53
    [Google Scholar]
  106. Michely J, Eldar E, Martin IM, Dolan RJ 2020. A mechanistic account of serotonin's impact on mood. Nat. Commun. 11:12335
    [Google Scholar]
  107. Minsky M. 2007. The Emotion Machine: Commonsense Thinking, Artificial Intelligence, and the Future of the Human Mind New York: Simon & Schuster
  108. Mobbs D, Headley DB, Ding W, Dayan P. 2020. Space, time, and fear: survival computations along defensive circuits. Trends Cogn. Sci. 24:3228–41
    [Google Scholar]
  109. Moutoussis M, Eldar E, Dolan RJ 2017. Building a new field of computational psychiatry. Biol. Psychiatry 82:6388–90
    [Google Scholar]
  110. Moutoussis M, Shahar N, Hauser TU, Dolan RJ. 2018. Computation in psychotherapy, or how computational psychiatry can aid learning-based psychological therapies. Comput. Psychiatry 2:50–73
    [Google Scholar]
  111. Nelson B, McGorry PD, Wichers M, Wigman JTW, Hartmann JA. 2017. Moving from static to dynamic models of the onset of mental disorder: a review. JAMA Psychiatry 74:5528–34
    [Google Scholar]
  112. Niv Y, Hitchcock P, Berwian IM, Schoen G. 2021. Toward precision cognitive behavioral therapy via reinforcement learning theory. Precision Psychiatry LM Williams, LM Hack Washington, DC: Am. Psychiatr. Assoc. In press
    [Google Scholar]
  113. Nolen-Hoeksema S, Watkins ER 2011. A heuristic for developing transdiagnostic models of psychopathology: explaining multifinality and divergent trajectories. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 6:6589–609How to think about multifinality by distinguishing the factors that contribute to mental health problems.
    [Google Scholar]
  114. Nolen-Hoeksema S, Wisco BE, Lyubomirsky S. 2008. Rethinking rumination. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 3:5400–24
    [Google Scholar]
  115. Norman SB, Wilkins KC, Myers US, Allard CB. 2014. Trauma informed guilt reduction therapy with combat veterans. Cogn. Behav. Pract. 21:178–88
    [Google Scholar]
  116. Patzelt EH, Kool W, Millner AJ, Gershman SJ. 2019. Incentives boost model-based control across a range of severity on several psychiatric constructs. Biol. Psychiatry 85:5425–33
    [Google Scholar]
  117. Perivoliotis D, Morrison AP, Grant PM, French P, Beck AT 2009. Negative performance beliefs and negative symptoms in individuals at ultra-high risk of psychosis: a preliminary study. Psychopathology 42:6375–79
    [Google Scholar]
  118. Podsakoff PM, MacKenzie SB, Podsakoff NP 2012. Sources of method bias in social science research and recommendations on how to control it. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 63:539–69
    [Google Scholar]
  119. Poldrack RA, Yarkoni T. 2016. From brain maps to cognitive ontologies: informatics and the search for mental structure. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 67:587–612
    [Google Scholar]
  120. Pulcu E, Browning M. 2017. Using computational psychiatry to rule out the hidden causes of depression. JAMA Psychiatry 74:8777–78
    [Google Scholar]
  121. Radulescu A, Niv Y. 2019. State representation in mental illness. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 55:160–66
    [Google Scholar]
  122. Redish AD, Gordon JA. 2016. Computational Psychiatry: New Perspectives on Mental Illness Cambridge, MA: MIT Press
  123. Robinaugh D, Haslbeck J, Waldorp L, Kossakowski J, Fried EI et al. 2019. Advancing the network theory of mental disorders: a computational model of panic disorder.. PsyArXiv, May 29 . https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/km37w Empirically informed, quantitative representation of panic disorder as a network of interacting elements.
    [Crossref]
  124. Rodebaugh TL, Scullin RB, Langer JK, Dixon DJ, Huppert JD et al. 2016. Unreliability as a threat to understanding psychopathology: the cautionary tale of attentional bias. J. Abnorm. Psychol 125:6840–51
    [Google Scholar]
  125. Rouault M, Seow T, Gillan CM, Fleming SM 2018. Psychiatric symptom dimensions are associated with dissociable shifts in metacognition but not task performance. Biol. Psychiatry 84:6443–51
    [Google Scholar]
  126. Russek EM, Moran R, McNamee D, Reiter A, Liu Y et al. 2020. Opportunities for emotion and mental health research in the resource-rationality framework. Behav. Brain Sci. 43:e21
    [Google Scholar]
  127. Rutledge RB, Chekroud AM, Huys QJ. 2019. Machine learning and big data in psychiatry: toward clinical applications. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 55:152–59
    [Google Scholar]
  128. Salmi J, Ritakallio L, Fellman D, Ellfolk U, Rinne JO, Laine M. 2020. Disentangling the role of working memory in Parkinson's disease. Front. Aging Neurosci. 12:572037
    [Google Scholar]
  129. Scholl J, Klein-Flügge M. 2018. Understanding psychiatric disorder by capturing ecologically relevant features of learning and decision-making. Behav. Brain Res. 355:56–75
    [Google Scholar]
  130. Shenhav A, Botvinick MM, Cohen JD. 2013. The expected value of control: an integrative theory of anterior cingulate cortex function. Neuron 79:2217–40
    [Google Scholar]
  131. Simon HA. 1990. Invariants of human behavior. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 41:1–19
    [Google Scholar]
  132. Snyder HR. 2013. Major depressive disorder is associated with broad impairments on neuropsychological measures of executive function: a meta-analysis and review. Psychol. Bull. 139:181–132
    [Google Scholar]
  133. Snyder HR, Hankin BL. 2016. Spiraling out of control: stress generation and subsequent rumination mediate the link between poorer cognitive control and internalizing psychopathology. Clin. Psychol. Sci. 4:61047–64
    [Google Scholar]
  134. Spitzer RL, First MB, Wakefield JC. 2007. Saving PTSD from itself in DSM-V. J. Anxiety Disord. 21:2233–41
    [Google Scholar]
  135. Treadway MT, Zald DH. 2011. Reconsidering anhedonia in depression: lessons from translational neuroscience. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 35:3537–55
    [Google Scholar]
  136. Valton V, Romaniuk L, Douglas Steele J, Lawrie S, Seriès P 2017. Comprehensive review: computational modelling of schizophrenia. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 83:631–46
    [Google Scholar]
  137. van de Leemput IA, Wichers M, Cramer AOJ, Borsboom D, Tuerlinckx F et al. 2014. Critical slowing down as early warning for the onset and termination of depression. PNAS 111:187–92
    [Google Scholar]
  138. Van Vugt MK, Hitchcock P, Shahar B. 2012. The effects of mindfulness-based cognitive therapy on affective memory recall dynamics in depression: a mechanistic model of rumination. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 6:257
    [Google Scholar]
  139. Wachbroit R. 1994. Normality as a biological concept. Philos. Sci. 61:4579–91
    [Google Scholar]
  140. Wakefield JC. 1992a. Disorder as harmful dysfunction: a conceptual critique of DSM-III-R's definition of mental disorder. Psychol. Rev. 99:2232–47
    [Google Scholar]
  141. Wakefield JC. 1992b. The concept of mental disorder: on the boundary between biological facts and social values. Am. Psychol. 47:3373–88
    [Google Scholar]
  142. Wang X-J, Krystal JH. 2014. Computational psychiatry. Neuron 84:3638–54
    [Google Scholar]
  143. Watkins ER. 2008. Constructive and unconstructive repetitive thought. Psychol. Bull. 134:2163–206
    [Google Scholar]
  144. Watts AL, Boness CL, Loeffelman JE, Steinley D. 2020. Does crude measurement contribute to observed unidimensionality of psychological constructs? An example with DSM-5 alcohol use disorder. PsyArXiv, June 28. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/paxd4
    [Crossref]
  145. Webb C, Trivedi M, Bruder G, Pizzagalli DA 2016. Neural correlates of three promising endophenotypes of depression: evidence from the EMBARC study. Neuropsychopharmacology 41:454–63
    [Google Scholar]
  146. Weiner WJ. 2008. There is no Parkinson disease. Arch. Neurol. 65:6705–8
    [Google Scholar]
  147. Whitmer AJ, Frank MJ, Gotlib IH. 2012. Sensitivity to reward and punishment in major depressive disorder: effects of rumination and of single versus multiple experiences. Cogn. Emot. 26:81475–85
    [Google Scholar]
  148. Wiecki TV, Antoniades CA, Stevenson A, Kennard C, Borowsky B et al. 2016. A computational cognitive biomarker for early-stage Huntington's disease. PLOS ONE 11:2e0148409
    [Google Scholar]
  149. Wiecki TV, Frank MJ. 2010. Neurocomputational models of motor and cognitive deficits in Parkinson's disease. Prog. Brain Res. 183:275–97
    [Google Scholar]
  150. Wiecki TV, Poland J, Frank MJ. 2015. Model-based cognitive neuroscience approaches to computational psychiatry: clustering and classification. Clin. Psychol. Sci. 3:3378–99
    [Google Scholar]
  151. Williams LM. 2016. Precision psychiatry: a neural circuit taxonomy for depression and anxiety. Lancet Psychiatry 3:5472–80
    [Google Scholar]
  152. Wright AGC, Woods WC. 2020. Personalized models of psychopathology. Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 16:49–74
    [Google Scholar]
  153. Zorowitz S, Momennejad I, Daw ND. 2020. Anxiety, avoidance, and sequential evaluation. Comput. Psychiatry 4:1–17
    [Google Scholar]
/content/journals/10.1146/annurev-psych-021621-124910
Loading
/content/journals/10.1146/annurev-psych-021621-124910
Loading

Data & Media loading...

Supplemental Material

Supplementary Data

  • Article Type: Review Article
This is a required field
Please enter a valid email address
Approval was a Success
Invalid data
An Error Occurred
Approval was partially successful, following selected items could not be processed due to error