1932

Abstract

Objects permeate human culture and saturate the imagination. This duality offers both opportunity and challenge. Here we ask how young human children learn to exploit the immense potential afforded by objects that can exist simultaneously in physical and imaginary realms. To this end, we advance a new framework that integrates the presently siloed literatures on manual skill and play development. We argue that developments in children's real and imagined use of objects are embodied, reciprocal, and intertwined. Advances in one plane of action influence and scaffold advances in the other. Consistent with this unified framework, we show how real and imagined interactions with objects are characterized by developmental parallels in how children () transcend the present to encompass future points in time and space, () extend beyond the self, and () gradually move beyond objects’ designed functions. In addition, we highlight bidirectional influences in children's real and imagined interactions with objects: Play engenders practice and skill in using objects, but just the same, practice using objects engenders advances in play. We close by highlighting the theoretical, empirical, and translational implications of this embodied and integrated account of manual skill and play development.

Loading

Article metrics loading...

/content/journals/10.1146/annurev-devpsych-050720-102538
2021-12-09
2025-02-11
Loading full text...

Full text loading...

/deliver/fulltext/devpsych/3/1/annurev-devpsych-050720-102538.html?itemId=/content/journals/10.1146/annurev-devpsych-050720-102538&mimeType=html&fmt=ahah

Literature Cited

  1. Adolph KE. 1997.. Learning in the development of infant locomotion. . Monogr. Soc. Res. Child Dev. 62:(3):1158. https://doi.org/10.2307/1166199
    [Google Scholar]
  2. Adolph KE, Hoch JE. 2019.. Motor development: embodied, embedded, enculturated, and enabling. . Annu. Rev. Psychol. 70::14164. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-010418-102836
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  3. Barrett TM, Davis EF, Needham A. 2007.. Learning about tools in infancy. . Dev. Psychol. 43:(2):35268
    [Google Scholar]
  4. Barrett TM, Needham A. 2008.. Developmental differences in infants' use of an object's shape to grasp it securely. . Dev. Psychobiol. 50:(1):97106
    [Google Scholar]
  5. Barsalou LW. 2008.. Grounded cognition. . Annu. Rev. Psychol. 59::61745. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093639
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  6. Bates E. 1979.. The biology of symbols: some concluding thoughts. . In The Emergence Of Symbols: Cognition and Communication in Infancy, pp. 31570. New York:: Academic
    [Google Scholar]
  7. Belsky J, Goode MK, Most RK. 1980.. Maternal stimulation and infant exploratory competence: cross-sectional, correlational, and experimental analyses. . Child Dev. 51:(4:116878. https://doi.org/10.2307/1129558
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  8. Belsky J, Most RK. 1981.. From exploration to play: a cross-sectional study of infant free play behavior. . Dev. Psychol. 17:(5):63039
    [Google Scholar]
  9. Bigelow AE, MacLean K, Proctor J. 2004.. The role of joint attention in the development of infants’ play with objects. . Dev. Sci. 7:(5):51826. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2004.00375.x
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  10. Bornstein MH, Tamis-LeMonda CS. 1995.. Parent-child symbolic play: three theories in search of an effect. . Dev. Rev. 15:(4):382400
    [Google Scholar]
  11. Bornstein MH, Tamis-LeMonda CS. 2007.. Infants at play: development, partners and functions. . In Introduction to Infant Development, ed. A Slater, M Lewis , pp. 25369. New York:: Oxford Univ. Press
    [Google Scholar]
  12. Bornstein MH, Tamis-LeMonda CS, Hahn CS, Haynes OM. 2008.. Maternal responsiveness to young children at three ages: longitudinal analysis of a multidimensional, modular, and specific parenting construct. . Dev. Psychol. 44:(3):86774
    [Google Scholar]
  13. Boudreau JP, Bushnell EW. 2000.. Spilling thoughts: configuring attentional resources in infants’ goal-directed actions. . Infant. Behav. Dev. 23:(3–4):54366. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0163-6383(01)00059-5
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  14. Bourgeois KS, Khawar AW, Neal SA, Lockman JJ. 2005.. Infant manual exploration of objects, surfaces, and their interrelations. . Infancy 8:(3):23352
    [Google Scholar]
  15. Boyette AH. 2019.. Autonomy, cognitive development, and the socialisation of cooperation in foragers. . Hunter Gatherer Res. 3:(3):475501. https://doi.org/10.3828/hgr.2017.23
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  16. Brand RJ, Baldwin DA, Ashburn LA. 2002.. Evidence for ‘motionese’: modifications in mothers’ infant-directed action. . Dev. Sci. 5:(1):7283
    [Google Scholar]
  17. Bretherton I. 1984.. Representing the social world in symbolic play: reality and fantasy. . In Symbolic Play, ed. I Bretherton , pp. 341. Orlando, FL:: Academic
    [Google Scholar]
  18. Bretherton I, O'Connell B, Shore C, Bates E. 1984.. The effect of contextual variation on symbolic play development from 20 to 28 months. . In Symbolic Play, ed. I Bretherton , pp. 27198. Orlando, FL:: Academic
    [Google Scholar]
  19. Bruner J. 1978.. The role of dialogue in language acquisition. . In The Child's Conception of Language, ed. A Sinclair, RJ Jarvella, WJM Levelt , pp. 24156. Berlin:: Springer Verlag
    [Google Scholar]
  20. Bushnell EW, Boudreau JP. 1993.. Motor development and the mind: the potential role of motor abilities as a determinant of aspects of perceptual development. . Child Dev. 64:(4):100521
    [Google Scholar]
  21. Bushnell EW, Boudreau JP. 1998.. Exploring and exploiting objects with the hands during infancy. . In The Psychobiology of the Hand, ed. K Connolly , pp. 14461. Cambridge, UK:: Mac Keith Press
    [Google Scholar]
  22. Cameron CE, Cottone EA, Murrah WM, Grissmer DW. 2016.. How are motor skills linked to children's school performance and academic achievement?. Child Dev. Perspect. 10:(2):9398. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdep.12168
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  23. Campbell SB, Mahoney AS, Northrup J, Moore EL, Leezenbaum NB, Brownell CA. 2018.. Developmental changes in pretend play from 22- to 34-months in younger siblings of children with autism spectrum disorder. . J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 46:(3):63954
    [Google Scholar]
  24. Clearfield MW. 2019.. Play for success: an intervention to boost object exploration in infants from low-income households. . Infant. Behav. Dev. 55::11222
    [Google Scholar]
  25. Chu J, Schulz LE. 2020.. Play, curiosity, and cognition. . Annu. Rev. Dev. Psychol. 2::31743
    [Google Scholar]
  26. Damast AM, Tamis-LeMonda CS, Bornstein MH. 1996.. Mother-child play: sequential interactions and the relation between maternal beliefs and behaviors. . Child Dev. 67:(4):175266
    [Google Scholar]
  27. de Barbaro K, Johnson CM, Forster D, Deák GO. 2016.. Sensorimotor decoupling contributes to triadic attention: a longitudinal investigation of mother-infant-object interactions. . Child Dev. 87:(2):494512
    [Google Scholar]
  28. De Resende BD, Ottoni EB, Fragaszy DM. 2008.. Ontogeny of manipulative behavior and nut-cracking in young tufted capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella): a perception–action perspective. . Dev. Sci. 11:(6):82840. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00731.x
    [Google Scholar]
  29. Defeyter MA, German TP. 2003.. Acquiring an understanding of design: evidence from children's insight problem solving. . Cognition 89::13355
    [Google Scholar]
  30. Delgado-Gaitan C, Trueba H. 1991.. Crossing Cultural Borders: Education for Immigrant Families in America. Bristol, PA:: Falmer Press
    [Google Scholar]
  31. DeLoache JS. 2004.. Becoming symbol-minded. . Trends Cogn. Sci. 8:(2):6670
    [Google Scholar]
  32. Diamond A. 1990.. Developmental time course in human infants and infant monkeys, and the neural bases of, inhibitory control in reaching. . Ann. New York Acad. Sci. 608:(1):63776. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-6632.1990.tb48913.x
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  33. Duncker K. 1945.. On problem-solving. . Psychol. Monogr. 58:(5):i113
    [Google Scholar]
  34. Farver JM, Howes C. 1993.. Cultural differences in American and Mexican mother-child pretend play. . Merrill-Palmer Q. 39::34458
    [Google Scholar]
  35. Fein GG. 1981.. Pretend play in childhood: an integrative review. . Child Dev. 52::1095118
    [Google Scholar]
  36. Fenson L, Kagan J, Kearsley R, Zelazo P. 1976.. The developmental progression of manipulative play in the first two years. . Child Dev. 47:(1):23236
    [Google Scholar]
  37. Fenson L, Ramsay DS. 1980.. Decentration and integration of the child's play in the second year. . Child Dev. 51::17178
    [Google Scholar]
  38. Fletcher KK, Cates CB, Mendelsohn AL, Tamis-LeMonda CS. 2020.. Play in Mexican-American mothers and toddlers is frequent, multimodal, and rich in symbolic content. . Infancy 25:(5):53551. https://doi.org/10.1111/infa.12344
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  39. Fletcher K, West K, Wang Y, Tamis-LeMonda CS. 2021.. Characterizing the spaces and places of infants' everyday play. Poster presented at the Society for Research in Child Development, online, Apr
    [Google Scholar]
  40. Fontenelle SA, Kahrs BA, Neal SA, Newton AT, Lockman JJ. 2007.. Infant manual exploration of composite substrates. . J. Exp. Child Psychol. 98:(3):15367
    [Google Scholar]
  41. Garvey C. 1990.. Play. Cambridge, MA:: Harvard Univ. Press. , 2nd ed..
    [Google Scholar]
  42. Gesell A, Amatruda CS. 1941.. Developmental Diagnosis: Normal and Abnormal Child Development. New York:: Hoeber
    [Google Scholar]
  43. Gibson EJ. 1982.. The concept of affordances in development: the renascence of functionalism. . In The Concept of Development: The Minnesota Symposia on Child Psychology, Vol. 15, ed. WA Collins , pp. 5581. Hillsdale, NJ:: Erlbaum
    [Google Scholar]
  44. Gibson EJ, Pick AD. 2000.. An Ecological Approach to Perceptual Learning and Development. New York:: Oxford Univ. Press
    [Google Scholar]
  45. Gibson EJ, Riccio G, Schmuckler MA, Stoffregen TA, Rosenberg D, Taormina J. 1987.. Detection of the traversability of surfaces by crawling and walking infants. . J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 13:(4):53344. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.13.4.533
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  46. Gibson JJ. 1962.. Observations on active touch. . Psychol. Rev. 69:(6):47791. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0046962
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  47. Gibson JJ. 1979.. The Ecological Approach to Visual Perception. Boston, MA:: Houghton Mifflin
    [Google Scholar]
  48. Gillespie A, Zittoun T. 2010.. Using resources: conceptualizing the mediation and reflective use of tools and signs. . Cult. Psychol. 16:(1):3762
    [Google Scholar]
  49. Gopnik A, Walker CM. 2013.. Considering counterfactuals: the relationship between causal learning and pretend play. . Am. J. Play 6:(1):1528
    [Google Scholar]
  50. Groos K. 1898.. The Play of Animals. New York:: Appleton
    [Google Scholar]
  51. Hafford C. 2010.. Sibling caretaking in immigrant families: understanding cultural practices to inform child welfare practice and evaluation. . Eval. Program Plan. 33:(3):294302
    [Google Scholar]
  52. Haight W, Miller PJ. 1992.. The development of everyday pretend play: a longitudinal study of mothers' participation. . Merrill-Palmer Q. 38::33149
    [Google Scholar]
  53. Harris PL. 2021.. Early constraints on the imagination: the realism of young children. . Child Dev. 92::46683. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.13487
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  54. Heathcock JC, Lockman JJ. 2019.. Infant and child development: innovations and foundations for rehabilitation. . Phys. Ther. 99::64346
    [Google Scholar]
  55. Herzberg O, Fletcher K, Schatz J, Adolph K, Tamis-LeMonda CS. 2021.. Infant exuberance object play at home: immense amounts of time-distributed, variable practice. . Child Dev. In press
    [Google Scholar]
  56. Hoff E. 2013.. Language Development. Belmont, CA:: Wadsworth Cengage Learn
    [Google Scholar]
  57. Hopkins EJ, Smith ED, Weisberg DS, Lillard AS. 2016.. The development of substitute object pretense: the differential importance of form and function. . J. Cogn. Dev. 17:(2):197220
    [Google Scholar]
  58. Inoue-Nakamura N, Matsuzawa T. 1997.. Development of stone tool use by wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). . J. Comp. Psychol. 111:(2):15973. https://doi.org/10.1037/0735-7036.111.2.159
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  59. Jayaraman S, Fausey CM, Smith LB. 2013.. Visual statistics of infants’ ordered experiences. . J. Vis. 13::735
    [Google Scholar]
  60. Jung WP, Kahrs BA, Lockman JJ. 2015.. Manual action, fitting, and spatial planning: relating objects by young children. . Cognition 134::12839
    [Google Scholar]
  61. Jung WP, Kahrs BA, Lockman JJ. 2018.. Fitting handled objects into apertures by 17- to 36-month-old children: the dynamics of spatial coordination. . Dev. Psychol. 54:(2):22839
    [Google Scholar]
  62. Kahrs BA, Jung WP, Lockman JJ. 2012.. What is the role of infant banging in the development of tool use?. Exp. Brain Res. 218:(2):31520
    [Google Scholar]
  63. Kahrs BA, Jung WP, Lockman JJ. 2013.. Motor origins of tool use. . Child Dev. 84:(3):81016
    [Google Scholar]
  64. Kahrs BA, Jung WP, Lockman JJ. 2014.. When does tool use become distinctively human? Hammering in young children. . Child Dev. 85:(3):105061
    [Google Scholar]
  65. Kahrs BA, Lockman JJ. 2014.. Building tool use from object manipulation: a perception-action perspective. . Ecol. Psychol. 26:(1–2):8897. https://doi.org/10.1080/10407413.2014.874908
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  66. Kaplan B, Rachwani J, Tamis-LeMonda CS, Adolph KE. 2021.. Deceptively simple: the perceptual-motor and social factors in children's learning of the designed actions of objects. Paper presented at the Society for Research in Child Development, online, Apr
    [Google Scholar]
  67. Karasik LB, Schneider JL, Kuchirko YA, Tamis-LeMonda CS. 2018.. Not so WEIRD object play in Tajikistan. Paper presented at the Meeting of the International Congress on Infant Studies, Philadelphia, PA:, June
    [Google Scholar]
  68. Kenward B, Rutz C, Weir AA, Kacelnik A. 2006.. Development of tool use in New Caledonian crows: inherited action patterns and social influences. . Anim. Behav. 72:(6):132943. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2006.04.007
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  69. Kimmerle M, Ferre CL, Kotwica KA, Michel GF. 2010.. Development of role-differentiated bimanual manipulation during the infant's first year. . Dev. Psychobiol. 52:(2):16880. https://doi.org/10.1002/dev.20428
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  70. Köhler W. 1929.. An old pseudoproblem. . Naturwissenschaften 17::395401
    [Google Scholar]
  71. Koterba EA, Iverson JM. 2009.. Investigating motionese: the effect of infant-directed action on infants’ attention and object exploration. . Infant. Behav. Dev. 32:(4):43744
    [Google Scholar]
  72. Kretch KS, Adolph KE. 2017.. The organization of exploratory behaviors in infant locomotor planning. . Dev. Sci. 20:(4):e12421. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12421
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  73. Lakoff G, Johnson M. 1980.. The metaphorical structure of the human conceptual system. . Cogn. Sci. 4:(2):195208
    [Google Scholar]
  74. Lancy DF. 2016.. Ethnographic perspectives on culture acquisition. . In Childhood: Origins, Evolution, and Implications, ed. CL Meehan, AN Crittenden , pp. 17395. Albuquerque:: Univ. N. M. Press
    [Google Scholar]
  75. Lederman SJ, Klatzky RL. 1987.. Hand movements: a window into haptic object recognition. . Cogn. Psychol. 19:(3):34268
    [Google Scholar]
  76. Leslie AM. 1987.. Pretense and representation: the origins of “theory of mind. Psychol. Rev. 94::41226
    [Google Scholar]
  77. Libertus K, Joh AS, Needham AW. 2016.. Motor training at 3 months affects object exploration 12 months later. . Dev. Sci. 19:(3):105866. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12370
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  78. Lillard A, Nishida T, Massaro D, Vaish A, Ma L, McRoberts G. 2007.. Signs of pretense across age and scenario. . Infancy 11:(1):130
    [Google Scholar]
  79. Lillard AS. 1993.. Pretend play skills and the child's theory of mind. . Child Dev. 64:(2):34871
    [Google Scholar]
  80. Lillard AS. 2011.. Mother-child fantasy play. . In The Oxford Handbook of Development of Play, ed. P Nathan, AD Pelligrini , pp. 28495. New York:: Oxford Univ. Press
    [Google Scholar]
  81. Lillard AS. 2013.. Playful learning and Montessori education. . NAMTA J. 38:(2):13774
    [Google Scholar]
  82. Lillard AS. 2015.. The development of play. . In Handbook of Child Psychology and Developmental Science, Vol. 2, ed. RM Lerner , pp. 42568. Hoboken, NJ:: Wiley-Blackwell. , 7th ed..
    [Google Scholar]
  83. Lillard AS, Pinkham AM, Smith E. 2011.. Pretend play and cognitive development. . In The Wiley-Blackwell Handbook of Childhood Cognitive Development, U Goswami , pp. 285311. New York:: Wiley-Blackwell. , 2nd ed..
    [Google Scholar]
  84. Little EE, Carver LJ, Legare CH. 2016.. Cultural variation in triadic infant–caregiver object exploration. . Child Dev. 87:(4):113045
    [Google Scholar]
  85. Lobo MA, Hall ML, Greenspan B, Rohloff P, Prosser LA, Smith BA. 2019.. Wearables for pediatric rehabilitation: how to optimally design and use products to meet the needs of users. . Phys. Ther. 99::64757
    [Google Scholar]
  86. Lockman JJ. 1984.. The development of detour ability during infancy. . Child Dev. 55::48291
    [Google Scholar]
  87. Lockman JJ. 2000.. A perception–action perspective on tool use development. . Child Dev. 71:(1):13744
    [Google Scholar]
  88. Lockman JJ, Adams CD. 2001.. Going around transparent and grid-like barriers: detour ability as a perception–action skill. . Dev. Sci. 4:(4):46371. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-7687.00188
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  89. Lockman JJ, Kahrs BA. 2017.. New insights into the development of human tool use. . Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 26:(4):33034
    [Google Scholar]
  90. Lockman JJ, McHale JP. 1989.. Object manipulation in infancy. . In Action in Social Context, ed. JJ Lockman, NL Hazen , pp. 12967. New York:: Plenum
    [Google Scholar]
  91. Matthews A, Ellis AE, Nelson CA. 1996.. Development of preterm and full-term infant ability on AB, recall memory, transparent barrier detour, and means-end tasks. . Child Dev. 67:(6):265876. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.1996.tb01881.x
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  92. Maynard AE, Greenfield PM. 2003.. Implicit cognitive development in cultural tools and children: lessons from Maya Mexico. . Cogn. Dev. 18:(4):489510
    [Google Scholar]
  93. McCarty ME, Clifton RK, Collard RR. 2001.. The beginnings of tool use by infants and toddlers. . Infancy 2:(2):23356
    [Google Scholar]
  94. McCune L. 1995.. A normative study of representational play in the transition to language. . Dev. Psychol. 31:(2):198206
    [Google Scholar]
  95. McCune-Nicolich L. 1981.. Toward symbolic functioning: structure of early pretend games and potential parallels with language. . Child Dev. 52::78597
    [Google Scholar]
  96. Morgante JD, Keen R. 2008.. Vision and action: the effect of visual feedback on infants’ exploratory behaviors. . Infant. Behav. Dev. 31:(4):72933
    [Google Scholar]
  97. Müller U, Yeung E, Hutchison SM. 2013.. The role of distancing in Werner and Kaplan's account of symbol formation and beyond. . Cult. Psychol. 19:(4):46383. https://doi.org/10.1177/1354067X13500323
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  98. Musgrave S, Lonsdorf E, Morgan D, Sanz C. 2021.. The ontogeny of termite gathering among chimpanzees in the Guaolougo Triangle, Republic of Congo. . Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 174::187200
    [Google Scholar]
  99. Needham AW. 2016.. Learning About Objects in Infancy. New York:: Routledge
    [Google Scholar]
  100. Norman DA. 2013.. The Design of Everyday Things. New York:: Basic Books. Revis
    [Google Scholar]
  101. O'Connell B, Bretherton I. 1984.. Toddler's play, alone and with mother: the role of maternal guidance. . In Symbolic Play, ed. I Bretherton , pp. 33768. Orlando, FL:: Academic
    [Google Scholar]
  102. Orr E, Geva R. 2015.. Symbolic play and language development. . Infant. Behav. Dev. 38::14761
    [Google Scholar]
  103. Palmer CF. 1989.. The discriminating nature of infants' exploratory actions. . Dev. Psychol. 25:(6):88593
    [Google Scholar]
  104. Piaget J. 1945.. Play, Dreams, and Imitation in Childhood. New York:: Norton
    [Google Scholar]
  105. Piaget J. 1952.. The Origins of Intelligence in Children. New York:: W.W. Norton. https://doi.org/10.1037/11494-000
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  106. Quinn S, Donnelly S, Kidd E. 2018.. The relationship between symbolic play and language acquisition: a meta-analytic review. . Dev. Rev. 49::12135
    [Google Scholar]
  107. Quinn S, Kidd E. 2018.. Symbolic play promotes non-verbal communicative exchanges in infant–caregiver dyads. . Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 37::3350. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjdp.12251
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  108. Rachwani J, Kaplan BE, Tamis-LeMonda CS, Adolph KE. 2021.. Children's use of everyday artifacts: learning the hidden affordances of zipping. . Dev. Psychobiol. 63::79399. https://doi.org/10.1002/dev.22049
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  109. Rachwani J, Tamis-LeMonda CS, Lockman JJ, Karasik LB, Adolph KE. 2020.. Learning the designed actions of everyday objects. . J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 149:(1):6778. https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0000631
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  110. Rips LJ, Hespos SJ. 2015.. Divisions of the physical world: concepts of objects and substances. . Psychol. Bull. 141:(4):786811
    [Google Scholar]
  111. Rogoff B. 1990.. Apprenticeship in Thinking: Cognitive Development in Social Context. New York:: Oxford Univ. Press
    [Google Scholar]
  112. Rogoff B. 2003.. The Cultural Nature of Human Development. New York:: Oxford Univ. Press
    [Google Scholar]
  113. Rogoff B, Mistry J, Göncü A, Mosier C. 1993.. Guided participation in cultural activity by toddlers and caregivers. . Monogr. Soc. Res. Child Dev. 58::1179
    [Google Scholar]
  114. Ruff HA. 1984.. Infants' manipulative exploration of objects: effects of age and object characteristics. . Dev. Psychol. 20:(1):920
    [Google Scholar]
  115. Rutz C, Klump BC, Komarczyk L, Leighton R, Kramer J, et al. 2016.. Discovery of species-wide tool use in the Hawaiian crow. . Nature 537::4037
    [Google Scholar]
  116. Slade A. 1987.. Quality of attachment and early symbolic play. . Dev. Psychol. 23:(1):7885
    [Google Scholar]
  117. Smith LB, Yu C, Yoshida H, Fausey KM. 2015.. Contributions of head-mounted cameras to studying the visual environments of infants and young children. . J. Cogn. Dev. 16:(3):40719
    [Google Scholar]
  118. Stahl AE, Feigenson L. 2015.. Observing the unexpected enhances infants’ learning and exploration. . Science 348:(6230):9194
    [Google Scholar]
  119. Stahl AE, Feigenson L. 2018.. Infants use linguistic group distinctions to chunk items in memory. . J. Exp. Child Psychol. 172::14967. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2018.03.005
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  120. Sutherland SL, Friedman O. 2013.. Just pretending can be really learning: Children use pretend play as a source for acquiring generic knowledge. . Dev. Psychol. 49:(9):166068
    [Google Scholar]
  121. Sweller J. 2011.. Cognitive load theory. . In Psychology of Learning and Motivation, Vol. 55, ed. JP Mestre, BH Ross , pp. 3776. Cambridge, UK:: Elsevier. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-387691-1.00002-8
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  122. Tamis-LeMonda C, Bornstein MH. 1996.. Variation in children's exploratory, nonsymbolic, and symbolic play: an explanatory multidimensional framework. . In Advances in Infancy Research, ed. C Rovee-Collier, L Lipsitt , pp. 3778. New York:: Ablex Publishers
    [Google Scholar]
  123. Tamis-LeMonda CS, Bornstein MH. 1991.. Individual variation, correspondence, stability, and change in mother and toddler play. . Infant. Behav. Dev. 14:(2):14362
    [Google Scholar]
  124. Tamis-LeMonda CS, Bornstein MH. 1993.. Play and its relations to other mental functions in the child. . New Dir. Child Adolesc. Dev. 1993.(59):1728
    [Google Scholar]
  125. Tamis-LeMonda CS, Kuchirko Y, Tafuro L. 2013.. From action to interaction: Infant object exploration and mothers' contingent responsiveness. . IEEE Trans. Auton. Ment. Dev. 5:(3):2029
    [Google Scholar]
  126. Tamis-LeMonda CS, Lockman JJ. 2020.. Infant object manipulation and play. . In The Cambridge Handbook of Infant Development, ed. JJ Lockman, CS Tamis-LeMonda , pp. 52048. New York:: Cambridge Univ. Press
    [Google Scholar]
  127. Tamis-LeMonda CS, Schatz J. 2019.. Learning language in the context of play. . In International Handbook of Language Development, ed. J Horst, J von Koss, K Torkildsen , pp. 44261. London:: Routledge
    [Google Scholar]
  128. Tan AWY. 2017.. From play to proficiency: the ontogeny of stone-tool use in coastal-foraging long-tailed macaques (Macaca fascicularis) from a comparative perception-action perspective. . J. Comp. Psychol. 131:(2):89114
    [Google Scholar]
  129. Thelen E. 1981.. Rhythmical behavior in infancy: an ethological perspective. . Dev. Psychol. 17:(3):23757. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.17.3.237
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  130. Turvey MT. 1996.. Dynamic touch. . Am. Psychol. 51::113452
    [Google Scholar]
  131. Uzgiris IC. 1976.. Organization of sensorimotor intelligence. . In Origins of Intelligence, ed. M Lewis , pp. 12363. Boston, MA:: Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4684-6961-5_5
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  132. von Hofsten C. 1993.. Prospective control: a basic aspect of action development. . Hum. Dev. 36::25370
    [Google Scholar]
  133. Vygotsky LS. 1967.. Play and its role in the mental development of the child. . Sov. Psychol. 5:(3):618
    [Google Scholar]
  134. Weisberg DS, Bloom P. 2009.. Young children separate multiple pretend worlds. . Dev. Sci. 12::699705
    [Google Scholar]
  135. Weisberg DS, Hirsh-Pasek K, Golinkoff RM, Kittredge AK, Klahr D. 2016.. Guided play: principles and practices. . Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 25::17782
    [Google Scholar]
  136. Werner H, Kaplan B. 1963.. Symbol Formation. Oxford, UK:: Wiley
    [Google Scholar]
  137. Zukow PG. 1989.. Siblings as effective socializing agents: evidence from Central Mexico. . In Sibling Interaction Across Cultures, ed. PG Zukow , pp. 79105. New York:: Springer
    [Google Scholar]
/content/journals/10.1146/annurev-devpsych-050720-102538
Loading
/content/journals/10.1146/annurev-devpsych-050720-102538
Loading

Data & Media loading...

  • Article Type: Review Article
This is a required field
Please enter a valid email address
Approval was a Success
Invalid data
An Error Occurred
Approval was partially successful, following selected items could not be processed due to error