1932

Abstract

Despite numerous findings on the sophisticated inferences that human infants draw from observing third-party helping interactions, currently there is no theoretical account of how infants come to understand such events in the first place. After reviewing the available evidence in infants, we describe an account of how human adults understand helping actions. According to this mature concept, helping is a second-order, goal-directed action aiming to increase the utility of another agent (the Helpee) via reducing the cost, or increasing the reward, of the Helpee's own goal-directed action. We then identify the cognitive prerequisites for conceiving helping in this way and ask whether these are available to infants in the interpretation of helping interactions. In contrast to the mature concept, we offer two simpler alternatives that may underlie the early understanding of helping actions: () helping as enabling, which requires second-order goal attribution but no utility calculus, and () helping as joint action, which requires efficiency (i.e., utility) evaluation without demanding second-order goal attribution. We evaluate the evidence supporting these accounts, derive unique predictions from them, and describe what developmental pathway toward the mature concept they envisage. We conclude the article by outlining further open questions that the developmental literature on the interpretation of helping interactions has not yet addressed.

Loading

Article metrics loading...

/content/journals/10.1146/annurev-devpsych-120321-033548
2023-12-11
2024-10-05
Loading full text...

Full text loading...

/deliver/fulltext/devpsych/5/1/annurev-devpsych-120321-033548.html?itemId=/content/journals/10.1146/annurev-devpsych-120321-033548&mimeType=html&fmt=ahah

Literature Cited

  1. Allen M, Perry C, Kaufman J. 2018.. Toddlers prefer to help familiar people. . J. Exp. Child. Psychol. 174::90102 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2018.05.009
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  2. Augusti E, Melinder A, Gredebäck G. 2010.. Look who's talking: pre-verbal infants’ perception of face-to-face and back-to-back social interactions. . Front. Psychol. 1::161. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2010.00161
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  3. Baker CL, Goodman ND, Tenenbaum JB. 2008.. Theory-based social goal inference. . In 30th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, pp. 135964 Austin, TX:: Cogn. Sci. Soc.
    [Google Scholar]
  4. Baker CL, Jara-Ettinger J, Saxe R, Tenenbaum JB. 2017.. Rational quantitative attribution of beliefs, desires and percepts in human mentalizing. . Nat. Hum. Behav. 1:(4):0064. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-017-0064
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  5. Baker CL, Saxe R, Tenenbaum JB. 2009.. Action understanding as inverse planning. . Cognition 113:(3):32949 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2009.07.005
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  6. Barragan RC, Dweck CS. 2014.. Rethinking natural altruism: simple reciprocal interactions trigger children's benevolence. . PNAS 111:(48):1707174 https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1419408111
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  7. Begus K, Curioni A, Knoblich G, Gergely G. 2020.. Infants understand collaboration: neural evidence for 9-month-olds’ attribution of shared goals to coordinated joint actions. . Complex Netw. Entropy 15:(6):65567 https://doi.org/10.1080/17470919.2020.1847730
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  8. Behne T, Carpenter M, Call J, Tomasello M. 2005.. Unwilling versus unable: infants’ understanding of intentional action. . Dev. Psychol. 41:(2):32837 https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.41.2.328
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  9. Beier JS, Spelke ES. 2012.. Infants’ developing understanding of social gaze. . Child Dev. 83:(2):48696 https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01702.x
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  10. Benton DT, Lapan C. 2022.. Moral masters or moral apprentices? A connectionist account of sociomoral evaluation in preverbal infants. . Cogn. Dev. 62::101164. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2022.101164
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  11. Blake PR, McAuliffe K, Warneken F. 2014.. The developmental origins of fairness: the knowledge–behavior gap. . Trends Cogn. Sci. 18:(11):55961 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2014.08.003
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  12. Brandone AC, Wellman HM. 2009.. You can't always get what you want: Infants understand failed goal-directed actions. . Psychol. Sci. 20:(1):8591 https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02246.x
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  13. Brownell CA. 2013.. Early development of prosocial behavior: current perspectives. . Infancy 18:(1):19 https://doi.org/10.1111/infa.12004
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  14. Chae JJK, Song H. 2018.. Negativity bias in infants’ expectations about agents’ dispositions. . Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 36:(4):62033 https://doi.org/10.1111/bjdp.12246
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  15. Colaizzi JM. 2016.. Empathy and prosocial behaviors in infancy. PhD Thesis , Okla. State Univ., Stillwater, OK:
    [Google Scholar]
  16. Cowell JM, Decety J. 2015.. Precursors to morality in development as a complex interplay between neural, socioenvironmental, and behavioral facets. . PNAS 112:(41):1265762 https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1508832112
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  17. Csibra G. 2007.. Action mirroring and action interpretation: an alternative account. . In Sensorimotor Foundations of Higher Cognition: Attention and Performance XXII, ed. P Haggard, Y Rosetti, M Kawato , pp. 43559 Oxford, UK:: Oxford Univ. Press. http://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199231447.003.0020
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  18. Csibra G, Bíró S, Koós O, Gergely G. 2003.. One-year-old infants use teleological representations of actions productively. . Cogn. Sci. 27:(1):11133 https://doi.org/10.1207/s15516709cog2701_4
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  19. Csibra G, Gergely G, Bıró S, Koós O, Brockbank M. 1999.. Goal attribution without agency cues: the perception of ‘pure reason’ in infancy. . Cognition 72:(3):23767 https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-0277(99)00039-6
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  20. Dahl A. 2020.. Early developments in acts and evaluations of helping: when and how to help?. In The Oxford Handbook of Moral Development: An Interdisciplinary Perspective, ed. LA Jensen , pp. 288305 Oxford, UK:: Oxford Univ. Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780190676049.013.16
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  21. Dahl A, Paulus M. 2019.. From interest to obligation: the gradual development of human altruism. . Child Dev. Perspect. 13:(1):1014 https://doi.org/10.1111/cdep.12298
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  22. Dahl A, Satlof-Bedrick ES, Hammond SI, Drummond JK, Waugh WE, Brownell CA. 2017.. Explicit scaffolding increases simple helping in younger infants. . Dev. Psychol. 53:(3):40716 https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000244
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  23. Fawcett C, Gredebäck G. 2013.. Infants use social context to bind actions into a collaborative sequence. . Dev. Sci. 16:(6):84149 https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12074
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  24. Fawcett C, Gredebäck G. 2015.. Eighteen-month-olds, but not 14-month-olds, use social context to bind action sequences. . Infancy 20:(1):11525 https://doi.org/10.1111/infa.12062
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  25. Fawcett C, Liszkowski U. 2012.. Infants anticipate others’ social preferences: social preference anticipation. . Infant Child Dev. 21:(3):23949 https://doi.org/10.1002/icd.739
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  26. Fortin C. 2019.. L'évaluation sociale et l'apprentissage en contexte social des nourrissons. PsyD Thesis , Univ. Montr., Qué., Can.: https://papyrus.bib.umontreal.ca/xmlui/bitstream/handle/1866/23841/Fortin_Camille_2019_Essai_Doctoral.pdf
    [Google Scholar]
  27. Gao T, Newman GE, Scholl BJ. 2009.. The psychophysics of chasing: a case study in the perception of animacy. . Cogn. Psychol. 59:(2):15479 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogpsych.2009.03.001
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  28. Geraci A. 2020.. How do toddlers evaluate defensive actions toward third parties?. Infancy 25:(6):91026 https://doi.org/10.1111/infa.12367
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  29. Geraci A, Surian L. 2011.. The developmental roots of fairness: infants’ reactions to equal and unequal distributions of resources. . Dev. Sci. 14:(5):101220 https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01048.x
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  30. Gergely G, Csibra G. 2003.. Teleological reasoning in infancy: the naıve theory of rational action. . Trends Cogn. Sci. 7:(7):28792 https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(03)00128-1
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  31. Gergely G, Nádasdy Z, Csibra G, Bíró S. 1995.. Taking the intentional stance at 12 months of age. . Cognition 56:(2):16593 https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(95)00661-H
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  32. Hamlin JK. 2013a.. Moral judgment and action in preverbal infants and toddlers: evidence for an innate moral core. . Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 22:(3):18693 https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721412470687
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  33. Hamlin JK. 2013b.. Failed attempts to help and harm: intention versus outcome in preverbal infants’ social evaluations. . Cognition 128:(3):45174 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2013.04.004
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  34. Hamlin JK. 2014.. Context-dependent social evaluation in 4.5-month-old human infants: the role of domain-general versus domain-specific processes in the development of social evaluation. . Front. Psychol. 5::614. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00614
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  35. Hamlin JK. 2015.. The case for social evaluation in preverbal infants: Gazing toward one's goal drives infants’ preferences for Helpers over Hinderers in the hill paradigm. . Front. Psychol. 5::1563. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01563
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  36. Hamlin JK, Hallinan EV, Woodward AL. 2008.. Do as I do: 7-month-old infants selectively reproduce others’ goals. . Dev. Sci. 11:(4):48794 https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00694.x
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  37. Hamlin JK, Newman GE, Wynn K. 2009.. Eight-month-old infants infer unfulfilled goals, despite ambiguous physical evidence. . Infancy 14:(5):57990 https://doi.org/10.1080/15250000903144215
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  38. Hamlin JK, Ullman T, Tenenbaum J, Goodman N, Baker C. 2013.. The mentalistic basis of core social cognition: experiments in preverbal infants and a computational model. . Dev. Sci. 16:(2):20926 https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12017
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  39. Hamlin JK, Wynn K. 2011.. Young infants prefer prosocial to antisocial others. . Cogn. Dev. 26:(1):3039 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2010.09.001
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  40. Hamlin JK, Wynn K, Bloom P. 2007.. Social evaluation by preverbal infants. . Nature 450:(7169):55759 https://doi.org/10.1038/nature06288
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  41. Hamlin JK, Wynn K, Bloom P, Mahajan N. 2011.. How infants and toddlers react to antisocial others. . PNAS 108:(50):1993136 https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1110306108
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  42. He X, Yang Y, Lin J, Wu X, Yin J. 2020.. Attributions of social interaction depend on the integration of the actor's simple goal and the influence on recipients. . Soc. Cogn. 38:(3):26686 https://doi.org/10.1521/soco.2020.38.3.266
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  43. Henderson AME, Wang Y, Matz LE, Woodward AL. 2013.. Active experience shapes 10-month-old infants’ understanding of collaborative goals. . Infancy 18:(1):1039 https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1532-7078.2012.00126.x
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  44. Henderson AME, Woodward AL. 2011. Let's work together”: What do infants understand about collaborative goals?. Cognition 121:(1):1221 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2011.05.008
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  45. Hepach R, Vaish A, Grossmann T, Tomasello M. 2016.. Young children want to see others get the help they need. . Child Dev. 87:(6):170314 https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12633
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  46. Hepach R, Vaish A, Tomasello M. 2012.. Young children are intrinsically motivated to see others helped. . Psychol. Sci. 23:(9):96772 https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797612440571
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  47. Hepach R, Warneken F. 2018.. Editorial overview: early development of prosocial behavior: revealing the foundation of human prosociality. . Curr. Opin. Psychol. 20::ivviii https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2018.02.001
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  48. Hobbs K, Spelke E. 2015.. Goal attributions and instrumental helping at 14 and 24 months of age. . Cognition 142::4459 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2015.03.014
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  49. Holvoet C, Scola C, Arciszewski T, Picard D. 2019.. Infants’ social evaluation abilities: testing their preference for prosocial agents at 6, 12 and 18 months with different social scenarios. . Early Child Dev. Care 189:(6):101831 https://doi.org/10.1080/03004430.2017.1361415
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  50. Hudson M, McDonough KL, Edwards R, Bach P. 2018.. Perceptual teleology: expectations of action efficiency bias social perception. . Proc. R. Soc. B 285::20180638. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2018.0638
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  51. Jara-Ettinger J, Floyd S, Huey H, Tenenbaum JB, Schulz LE. 2020.. Social pragmatics: Preschoolers rely on commonsense psychology to resolve referential underspecification. . Child Dev. 91:(4):113549 https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.13290
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  52. Jara-Ettinger J, Gweon H, Schulz LE, Tenenbaum JB. 2016.. The naïve utility calculus: computational principles underlying commonsense psychology. . Trends Cogn. Sci. 20::589604 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2016.05.011
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  53. Jin K-S, Baillargeon R. 2017.. Infants possess an abstract expectation of ingroup support. . PNAS 114:(31):8199204 https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1706286114
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  54. Kanakogi Y, Inoue Y, Matsuda G, Butler D, Hiraki K, Myowa-Yamakoshi M. 2017.. Preverbal infants affirm third-party interventions that protect victims from aggressors. . Nat. Hum. Behav. 1:(2):0037. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-016-0037
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  55. Kanakogi Y, Okumura Y, Inoue Y, Kitazaki M, Itakura S. 2013.. Rudimentary sympathy in preverbal infants: preference for others in distress. . PLOS ONE 8:(6):e65292. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0065292
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  56. Kleiman-Weiner M, Ho MK, Austerweil JL, Littman ML, Tenenbaum JB. 2016.. Coordinate to cooperate or compete: abstract goals and joint intentions in social interaction. . In Proceedings of the 38th Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society, ed. A Papafragou, D Grodner, D Mirman, JC Trueswell , pp. 167984 Austin, TX:: Cogn. Sci. Soc.
    [Google Scholar]
  57. Kominsky JF, Lucca K, Thomas AJ, Frank MC, Hamlin K. 2020.. Simplicity and validity in infant research. . PsyArXiv. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/6j9p3
    [Crossref]
  58. Köster M, Itakura S, Omori M, Kärtner J. 2019.. From understanding others’ needs to prosocial action: Motor and social abilities promote infants’ helping. . Dev. Sci. 22:(6):e12804. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12804
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  59. Köster M, Ohmer X, Nguyen TD, Kärtner J. 2016.. Infants understand others’ needs. . Psychol. Sci. 27:(4):54248 https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797615627426
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  60. Kudrnova V, Spelke E, Thomas AJ. 2023.. Infants infer social relationships between individuals who engage in imitative social interactions. . PsyArXiv. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/zuwpc
    [Crossref]
  61. Kuhlmeier V, Wynn K, Bloom P. 2003.. Attribution of dispositional states by 12-month-olds. . Psychol. Sci. 14:(5):4028 https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.01454
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  62. Lancy DF. 2020.. Child Helpers: A Multidisciplinary Perspective. Cambridge, UK:: Cambridge Univ. Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108769204
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  63. Lee W, Kim EY, Song H. 2020.. Do infants expect others to be helpful?. Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 38:(3):47890 https://doi.org/10.1111/bjdp.12331
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  64. Lee Y, Yun JE, Kim EY, Song H. 2015.. The development of infants’ sensitivity to behavioral intentions when inferring others’ social preferences. . PLOS ONE 10:(9):e0135588. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0135588
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  65. Liu S, Brooks NB, Spelke ES. 2019.. Origins of the concepts cause, cost, and goal in prereaching infants. . PNAS 116:(36):1774752 https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1904410116
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  66. Liu S, Spelke ES. 2017.. Six-month-old infants expect agents to minimize the cost of their actions. . Cognition 160::3542 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2016.12.007
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  67. Liu S, Ullman TD, Tenenbaum JB, Spelke ES. 2017.. Ten-month-old infants infer the value of goals from the costs of actions. . Science 358:(6366):103841 https://doi.org/10.1126/science.aag2132
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  68. Mascaro O, Csibra G. 2012.. Representation of stable dominance relations by human infants. . PNAS 109:(18):686267 https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1113194109
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  69. Mascaro O, Csibra G. 2022.. Infants expect agents to minimize the collective cost of collaborative actions. . Sci. Rep. 12::17088. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-022-21452-5
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  70. Maxwell L, Rafetseder E. 2015.. Do toddlers have an innate moral core? An investigation into whether toddlers reward a good outcome over a good goal. . Poster presented at the 2015 Budapest CEU Conference on Cognitive Development, Budapest, Hung:., Jan. 9
  71. Meltzoff AN. 1995.. Understanding the intentions of others: re-enactment of intended acts by 18-month-old children. . Dev. Psychol. 31:(5):83850 https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.31.5.838
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  72. Michael J, Székely M. 2017.. Goal slippage: a mechanism for spontaneous instrumental helping in infancy?. Topoi 38::17383 https://doi.org/10.1007/s11245-017-9485-5
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  73. Mikhail J. 2011.. Elements of Moral Cognition: Rawls’ Linguistic Analogy and the Cognitive Science of Moral and Legal Judgment. Cambridge, UK:: Cambridge Univ. Press
    [Google Scholar]
  74. Netanyahu A, Shu T, Katz B, Barbu A, Tenenbaum JB. 2021.. PHASE: PHysically-grounded Abstract Social Events for machine social perception. . arXiv:2103.01933 [cs.AI]. https://doi.org/10.48550/arXiv.2103.01933
    [Crossref]
  75. Nighbor T, Kohn C, Normand M, Schlinger H. 2017.. Stability of infants’ preference for prosocial others: implications for research based on single-choice paradigms. . PLOS ONE 12:(6):e0178818. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0178818
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  76. Pepe B, Powell LJ. 2023.. Infants’ expectations for prosociality in imitators. . OSF Preprints. https://osf.io/cjnkb/
  77. Pettygrove DM, Hammond SI, Karahuta EL, Waugh WE, Brownell CA. 2013.. From cleaning up to helping out: parental socialization and children's early prosocial behavior. . Infant Behav. Dev. 36:(4):84346 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2013.09.005
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  78. Pomiechowska B, Csibra G. 2020.. Ten-month-olds infer relative costs of different goal-directed actions. . In Proceedings of the 42nd Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, pp. 39196 Austin, TX:: Cogn. Sci. Soc.
    [Google Scholar]
  79. Pomiechowska B, Csibra G. 2022.. Infants infer motor competence from differences in agent-specific relative action costs. . In Proceedings of the 44th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, pp. 333945 Austin, TX:: Cogn. Sci. Soc.
    [Google Scholar]
  80. Powell LJ. 2022.. Adopted utility calculus: origins of a concept of social affiliation. . Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 17:(5):121533 https://doi.org/10.1177/17456916211048487
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  81. Powell LJ, Spelke ES. 2013.. Preverbal infants expect members of social groups to act alike. . PNAS 110:(41):E396572 https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1304326110
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  82. Powell LJ, Spelke ES. 2018a.. Human infants’ understanding of social imitation: inferences of affiliation from third party observations. . Cognition 170::3148 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2017.09.007
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  83. Powell LJ, Spelke ES. 2018b.. Third-party preferences for imitators in preverbal infants. . Open Mind 2:(2):6171 https://doi.org/10.1162/opmi_a_00018
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  84. Premack D, Premack AJ. 1997.. Infants attribute value± to the goal-directed actions of self-propelled objects. . J. Cogn. Neurosci. 9:(6):84856 https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.1997.9.6.848
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  85. Pun A, Birch SAJ, Baron AS. 2021.. The power of allies: infants’ expectations of social obligations during intergroup conflict. . Cognition 211::104630. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2021.104630
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  86. Revencu B, Csibra G. 2020.. Opening the black box of early depiction interpretation: from whether to how in the theory-of-puppets debate. . Commentary on Kominsky, Lucca, Thomas, Frank, & Hamlin. PsyArXiv. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/u3em8
    [Crossref]
  87. Rhodes M, Hetherington C, Brink K, Wellman HM. 2015.. Infants’ use of social partnerships to predict behavior. . Dev. Sci. 18:(6):90916 https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12267
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  88. Salvadori E, Blazsekova T, Volein A, Karap Z, Tatone D, Mascaro O, Csibra G. 2015.. Probing the strength of infants’ preference for helpers over hinderers: two replication attempts of Hamlin and Wynn. 2011.. PLOS ONE 10:(11):e0140570. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0140570
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  89. Schlingloff L, Csibra G, Tatone D. 2020.. Do 15-month-old infants prefer helpers? A replication of Hamlin et al. 2007. . R. Soc. Open Sci. 7:(4):191795. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsos.191795
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  90. Schuhmacher N, Köster M, Kärtner J. 2018.. Modeling prosocial behavior increases helping in 16-month-olds. . Child Dev. 90:(5):1789801 https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.13054
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  91. Sebanz N, Bekkering H, Knoblich G. 2006.. Joint action: bodies and minds moving together. . Trends Cogn. Sci. 10:(2):7076 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2005.12.009
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  92. Shimizu Y, Senzaki S, Uleman JS. 2018.. The influence of maternal socialization on infants’ social evaluation in two cultures. . Infancy 23:(5):74866 https://doi.org/10.1111/infa.12240
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  93. Shu T, Kryven M, Ullman TD, Tenenbaum JB. 2020.. Adventures in Flatland: perceiving social interactions under physical dynamics. . In Proceedings of the 42nd Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society, pp. 29017 Austin, TX:: Cogn. Sci. Soc.
    [Google Scholar]
  94. Shum M, Kleiman-Weiner M, Littman ML, Tenenbaum JB. 2019.. Theory of minds: understanding behavior in groups through inverse planning. . arXiv:1901.06085v1 [cs.AI]. https://doi.org/10.48550/arXiv.1901.06085
    [Crossref]
  95. Singh L. 2020.. Bilingual infants are more sensitive to morally relevant social behavior than monolingual infants. . J. Cogn. Dev. 21:(5):63150 https://doi.org/10.1080/15248372.2020.1807987
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  96. Siposova B, Tomasello M, Carpenter M. 2018.. Communicative eye contact signals a commitment to cooperate for young children. . Cognition 179::192201 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2018.06.010
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  97. Sommerville JA. 2018.. Infants' understanding of distributive fairness as a test case for identifying the extents and limits of infants' sociomoral cognition and behavior. . Child Dev. Perspect. 12:(3):14145 https://doi.org/10.1111/cdep.12283
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  98. Sommerville JA, Enright EA. 2018.. The origins of infants' fairness concerns and links to prosocial behavior. . Curr. Opin. Psychol. 20::11721 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2018.01.005
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  99. Sommerville JA, Enright EA, Horton RO, Lucca K, Sitch MJ, Kirchner-Adelhart S. 2018.. Infants’ prosocial behavior is governed by cost-benefit analyses. . Cognition 177::1220 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2018.03.021
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  100. Spelke ES. 2016.. Core knowledge and conceptual change: a perspective on social cognition. . In Core Knowledge and Conceptual Change, ed. D Barner, AS Baron , pp. 279300 Oxford, UK:: Oxford Univ. Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780190467630.003.0016
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  101. Steckler CM, Liberman Z, Van de Vondervoort JW, Slevinsky J, Le DT, Hamlin JK. 2018.. Feeling out a link between feeling and infant sociomoral evaluation. . Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 36:(3):482500 https://doi.org/10.1111/bjdp.12232
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  102. Steckler CM, Woo BM, Hamlin JK. 2017.. The limits of early social evaluation: 9-month-olds fail to generate social evaluations of individuals who behave inconsistently. . Cognition 167::25565 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2017.03.018
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  103. Tatone D, Geraci A, Csibra G. 2015.. Giving and taking: representational building blocks of active resource-transfer events in human infants. . Cognition 137::4762 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2014.12.007
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  104. Tatone D, Hernik M, Csibra G. 2019.. Minimal cues of possession transfer compel infants to ascribe the goal of giving. . Open Mind 3::3140 https://doi.org/10.1162/opmi_a_00024
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  105. Tauzin T, Gergely G. 2018.. Communicative mind-reading in preverbal infants. . Sci. Rep. 8:(1):9534. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-018-27804-4
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  106. Thiele M, Hepach R, Michel C, Gredebäck G, Haun DBM. 2021.. Social interaction targets enhance 13-month-old infants' associative learning. . Infancy 26:(3):40822 https://doi.org/10.1111/infa.12393
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  107. Thomas AJ, Thomse L, Lukowski AF, Abramyan M, Sarnecka BW. 2018.. Toddlers prefer those who win but not when they win by force. . Nat. Hum. Behav. 2::66269 https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-018-0415-3
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  108. Török G, Pomiechowska B, Csibra G, Sebanz N. 2019.. Rationality in joint action: maximizing coefficiency in coordination. . Psychol. Sci. 30:(6):93041 https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797619842550
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  109. Török G, Stanciu O, Sebanz N, Csibra G. 2021.. Computing joint action costs: co-actors minimize the aggregate individual costs in an action sequence. . Open Mind 5::10012 https://doi.org/10.1162/opmi_a_00045
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  110. Ullman TD, Baker CL, Macindoe O, Evans O, Goodman ND, Tenenbaum JB. 2009.. Help or hinder: Bayesian models of social goal inference. . In Advances in Neural Information Processing Systems 22, ed. Y Bengio, D Schuurmans, J Lafferty, C Williams, A Culotta , pp. 187482 La Jolla, CA:: Neural Inf. Proc. Syst.
    [Google Scholar]
  111. Van de Vondervoort JW, Hamlin JK. 2016.. Evidence for intuitive morality: preverbal infants make sociomoral evaluations. . Child Dev. Perspect. 10:(3):14348 https://doi.org/10.1111/cdep.12175
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  112. Vaporova E, Zmyj N. 2020.. Social evaluation and imitation of prosocial and antisocial agents in infants, children, and adults. . PLOS ONE 15:(9):e0235595. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0235595
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  113. Vesper C, Sevdalis V. 2020.. Informing, coordinating, and performing: a perspective on functions of sensorimotor communication. . Front. Hum. Neurosci. 14::168. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2020.00168
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  114. Wang RE, Wu SA, Evans JA, Tenenbaum JB, Parkes DC, Kleiman-Weiner M. 2020.. Too many cooks: Bayesian inference for coordinating multi-agent collaboration. . arXiv:2003.11778 [cs.AI]. http://arxiv.org/abs/2003.11778
  115. Wang Y, Henderson AME. 2018.. We cooperated so…now what? Infants expect cooperative partners to share resources. . Infant Behav. Dev. 52::913 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2018.04.005
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  116. Warneken F. 2018.. How children solve the two challenges of cooperation. . Annu. Rev. Psychol. 69::20529 https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-122216-011813
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  117. Warneken F, Tomasello M. 2010.. Helping and cooperation at 14 months of age. . Infancy 11:(3):27194 https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1532-7078.2007.tb00227.x
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  118. Woo BM, Spelke ES. 2020.. How to help best: Infants’ changing understanding of multistep actions informs their evaluations of helping. . In Proceedings of the 42nd Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, pp. 38490 Austin, TX:: Cogn. Sci. Soc.
    [Google Scholar]
  119. Woo BM, Spelke ES. 2022.. Infants’ social evaluations of agents who act on false beliefs. . Dev. Sci. 26:(2):e13314. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.13314
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  120. Woo BM, Steckler CM, Le DT, Hamlin JK. 2017.. Social evaluation of intentional, truly accidental, and negligently accidental helpers and harmers by 10-month-old infants. . Cognition 168::15463 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2017.06.029
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  121. Woo BM, Tan E, Hamlin JK. 2022.. Human morality is based on an early-emerging moral core. . Annu. Rev. Dev. Psychol. 4::4161 https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-devpsych-121020-023312
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  122. Wyman E, Rakoczy H, Tomasello M. 2013.. Non-verbal communication enables children's coordination in a “Stag Hunt” game. . Eur. J. Dev. Psychol. 10:(5):597610 https://doi.org/10.1080/17405629.2012.726469
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  123. Wynn K. 2009.. Constraints on natural altruism. . Br. J. Psychol. 100:(3):48190 https://doi.org/10.1348/000712609X441312
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  124. Yin J, Csibra G, Tatone D. 2022.. Structural asymmetries in the representation of giving and taking events. . Cognition 229::105248. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2022.105248
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
/content/journals/10.1146/annurev-devpsych-120321-033548
Loading
/content/journals/10.1146/annurev-devpsych-120321-033548
Loading

Data & Media loading...

  • Article Type: Review Article
This is a required field
Please enter a valid email address
Approval was a Success
Invalid data
An Error Occurred
Approval was partially successful, following selected items could not be processed due to error