1932

Abstract

All human societies are permeated by collectively shared entities that govern daily social interactions and promote coordination and cooperation: norms. While the study of norm development is not new to developmental psychology, it has only recently been the target of an interdisciplinary wave of research using new methodologies and (often) complementary theoretical accounts to describe and explain the origins and potentially species-unique aspects of human norm psychology. Here we review recent developmental research showing that young children swiftly acquire and infer norms in a variety of social contexts. Moreover, children actively enforce these norms, even as unaffected bystanders, when third parties do things the wrong way. This research suggests that the foundations of human norm psychology can be found in early childhood. Deeper insights into the ontogenetic roots of norm psychology may contribute to understanding the evolutionary emergence of human cooperation and its maintenance in the contemporary world.

Loading

Article metrics loading...

/content/journals/10.1146/annurev-devpsych-120621-034731
2023-12-11
2024-05-03
Loading full text...

Full text loading...

/deliver/fulltext/devpsych/5/1/annurev-devpsych-120621-034731.html?itemId=/content/journals/10.1146/annurev-devpsych-120621-034731&mimeType=html&fmt=ahah

Literature Cited

  1. Bayertz K, Roughley N, eds. 2019. The Normative Animal? On the Anthropological Significance of Social, Moral and Linguistic Norms New York: Oxford Univ. Press
  2. Blake PR, McAuliffe K, Corbit J, Callaghan TC, Barry O et al. 2015. The ontogeny of fairness in seven societies. Nature 528:758125861
    [Google Scholar]
  3. Bloom P. 2013. Just Babies: The Origins of Good and Evil New York: Crown Publ.
  4. Boeckle M, Schiestl M, Frohnwieser A, Gruber R, Miller R et al. 2020. New Caledonian crows plan for specific future tool use. Proc. R. Soc. B 287(1938):20201490
    [Google Scholar]
  5. Boesch C. 1994. Cooperative hunting in wild chimpanzees. Anim. Behav. 48:365367
    [Google Scholar]
  6. Boesch C. 2012. Wild Cultures: A Comparison Between Chimpanzee and Human Cultures Cambridge, MA: Cambridge Univ. Press
  7. Boyd R, Richerson PJ. 2005. The Origin and Evolution of Cultures New York: Oxford Univ. Press
  8. Brandom RB. 1994. Making It Explicit Cambridge, MA: Harvard Univ. Press
  9. Bräuer J, Call J, Tomasello M. 2009. Are apes inequity averse? New data on the token-exchange paradigm. Am. J. Primatol. 71:217581
    [Google Scholar]
  10. Bräuer J, Hanus D. 2012. Fairness in non-human primates?. Soc. Justice Res. 25:325676
    [Google Scholar]
  11. Brosnan SF, de Waal FBM. 2003. Monkeys reject unequal pay. Nature 425:695529799
    [Google Scholar]
  12. Brosnan SF, de Waal FBM. 2014. Evolution of responses to (un)fairness. Science 346:62071251776
    [Google Scholar]
  13. Brosnan SF, Schiff HC, de Waal FBM. 2005. Tolerance for inequity may increase with social closeness in chimpanzees. Proc. R. Soc. B 272:156025358
    [Google Scholar]
  14. Brosnan SF, Talbot C, Ahlgren M, Lambeth SP, Schapiro SJ. 2010. Mechanisms underlying responses to inequitable outcomes in chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes. Anim. Behav. 79:6122937
    [Google Scholar]
  15. Buresh JS, Woodward AL. 2007. Infants track action goals within and across agents. Cognition 104:2287314
    [Google Scholar]
  16. Burge T. 2009. Primitive agency and natural norms. Philos. Phenomenol. Res. 79:225178
    [Google Scholar]
  17. Burkart JM, Brügger RK, van Schaik CP. 2018. Evolutionary origins of morality: insights from non-human primates. Front. Sociol. 3:17
    [Google Scholar]
  18. Burns M, Sommerville J. 2014. “I pick you”: the impact of fairness and race on infants’ selection of social partners. Front. Psychol. 5:93
    [Google Scholar]
  19. Butler LP, Gibbs HM, Tavassolie NS. 2020. Children's developing understanding that even reliable sources need to verify their claims. Cogn. Dev. 54:100871
    [Google Scholar]
  20. Butler LP, Schmidt MFH, Tavassolie NS, Gibbs HM. 2018. Children's evaluation of verified and unverified claims. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 176:7383
    [Google Scholar]
  21. Buyukozer Dawkins M, Sloane S, Baillargeon R 2019. Do infants in the first year of life expect equal resource allocations?. Front. Psychol. 10:116
    [Google Scholar]
  22. Campbell RL, Richie DM. 1983. Problems in the theory of developmental sequences: prerequisites and precursors. Hum. Dev. 26:315672
    [Google Scholar]
  23. Chomsky N. 2006. Language and Mind Cambridge, UK: Cambridge Univ. Press. , 3rd ed..
  24. Christen M, Glock H-J. 2012. The (limited) space for justice in social animals. Soc. Justice Res. 25:3298326
    [Google Scholar]
  25. Chudek M, Henrich J. 2011. Culture–gene coevolution, norm-psychology and the emergence of human prosociality. Trends Cogn. Sci. 15:521826
    [Google Scholar]
  26. Clay Z, Ravaux L, de Waal FBM, Zuberbuhler K. 2016. Bonobos (Pan paniscus) vocally protest against violations of social expectations. J. Comp. Psychol. 130:14454
    [Google Scholar]
  27. Cosmides L, Tooby J 1992. Cognitive adaptations for social exchange. The Adapted Mind: Evolutionary Psychology and the Generation of Culture, ed. JH Barkow, L Cosmides, J Tooby 163228. New York: Oxford Univ. Press
    [Google Scholar]
  28. Dahl A, Schmidt MFH. 2018. Preschoolers, but not adults, treat instrumental norms as categorical imperatives. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 165:85100
    [Google Scholar]
  29. Diesendruck G, Markson L. 2011. Children's assumption of the conventionality of culture. Child Dev. Perspect. 5:318995
    [Google Scholar]
  30. Elsner B, Pauen S. 2007. Social learning of artefact function in 12- and 15-month-olds. Eur. J. Dev. Psychol. 4:8099
    [Google Scholar]
  31. Engel P 2011. Epistemic norms. The Routledge Companion to Epistemology S Bernecker, D Pritchard 4757. New York: Routledge
    [Google Scholar]
  32. Engelmann JM, Clift JB, Herrmann E, Tomasello M. 2017. Social disappointment explains chimpanzees’ behaviour in the inequity aversion task. Proc. R. Soc. B 284: 1861.20171502
    [Google Scholar]
  33. Engelmann JM, Herrmann E. 2016. Chimpanzees trust their friends. Curr. Biol. 26:225256
    [Google Scholar]
  34. Fedra E, Schmidt MFH. 2018. Preschoolers understand the moral dimension of factual claims. Front. Psychol. 9:1841
    [Google Scholar]
  35. Fedra E, Schmidt MFH. 2019. Older (but not younger) preschoolers reject incorrect knowledge claims. Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 37:113045
    [Google Scholar]
  36. Fehr E, Fischbacher U. 2004. Social norms and human cooperation. Trends Cogn. Sci. 8:418590
    [Google Scholar]
  37. Fitzpatrick S. 2020. Chimpanzee normativity: evidence and objections. Biol. Philos. 35:445
    [Google Scholar]
  38. Flack JC, Girvan M, de Waal FBM, Krakauer DC. 2006. Policing stabilizes construction of social niches in primates. Nature 439:707542629
    [Google Scholar]
  39. Flack JC, Krakauer DC, de Waal FBM. 2005. Robustness mechanisms in primate societies: a perturbation study. Proc. R. Soc. B 272: 1568.109199
    [Google Scholar]
  40. Friedman O, Pesowski M, Goulding BW. 2018. Legal ownership is psychological: evidence from young children. Psychological Ownership and Consumer Behavior J Peck, S Shu 1931. New York: Springer
    [Google Scholar]
  41. Friedrich JP, Schmidt MFH. 2022. Preschoolers agree to and enforce prosocial, but not selfish, sharing norms. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 214:105303
    [Google Scholar]
  42. Fu G, Xiao WS, Killen M, Lee K. 2014. Moral judgment and its relation to second-order theory of mind. Dev. Psychol. 50:8208592
    [Google Scholar]
  43. Geraci A, Surian L. 2011. The developmental roots of fairness: infants’ reactions to equal and unequal distributions of resources. Dev. Sci. 14:5101220
    [Google Scholar]
  44. Gibbard A. 1990. Wise Choices, Apt Feelings: A Theory of Normative Judgment Cambridge, MA: Harvard Univ. Press
  45. Giffin C, Lombrozo T 2015. Mental states are more important in evaluating moral than conventional violations. Proceedings of the 37th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society R Dale, C Jennings, P Maglio, T Matlock, D Noelle et al.8005. Austin, TX: Cogn. Sci. Soc.
    [Google Scholar]
  46. Gloor J. 2014. Collective intentionality and practical reason. Institutions, Emotions, and Group Agents: Contributions to Social Ontology A Konzelmann, HB Schmid 297312. Dordrecht, Neth: Springer-Verlag
    [Google Scholar]
  47. Göckeritz S, Schmidt MFH, Tomasello M. 2014. Young children's creation and transmission of social norms. Cogn. Dev. 30:8195
    [Google Scholar]
  48. Goodall J. 1977. Infant killing and cannibalism in free-living chimpanzees. Folia Primatol. Int. J. Primatol. 28:425989
    [Google Scholar]
  49. Graham SA, Stock H, Henderson A. 2006. Nineteen-month-olds’ understanding of the conventionality of object labels versus desires. Infancy 9:334150
    [Google Scholar]
  50. Gruber T, Chimento M, Aplin LM, Biro D. 2022. Efficiency fosters cumulative culture across species. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B 377:184320200308
    [Google Scholar]
  51. Haidt J. 2012. The Righteous Mind: Why Good People Are Divided by Politics and Religion New York: Pantheon Books
  52. Hamlin JK. 2013a. Moral judgment and action in preverbal infants and toddlers: evidence for an innate moral core. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 22:318693
    [Google Scholar]
  53. Hamlin JK. 2013b. Failed attempts to help and harm: intention versus outcome in preverbal infants’ social evaluations. Cognition 128:345174
    [Google Scholar]
  54. Hamlin JK, Mahajan N, Liberman Z, Wynn K. 2013. Not like me = bad: Infants prefer those who harm dissimilar others. Psychol. Sci. 24:458994
    [Google Scholar]
  55. Hamlin JK, Wynn K. 2012. Who knows what's good to eat? Infants fail to match the food preferences of antisocial others. Cogn. Dev. 27:322739
    [Google Scholar]
  56. Hamlin JK, Wynn K, Bloom P. 2007. Social evaluation by preverbal infants. Nature 450:716955759
    [Google Scholar]
  57. Hamlin JK, Wynn K, Bloom P. 2010. 3-month-olds show a negativity bias in their social evaluations. Dev. Sci. 13:692329
    [Google Scholar]
  58. Hamlin JK, Wynn K, Bloom P, Mahajan N. 2011. How infants and toddlers react to antisocial others. PNAS 108:501993136
    [Google Scholar]
  59. Hardecker S, Schmidt MFH, Roden M, Tomasello M. 2016. Young children's behavioral and emotional responses to different social norm violations. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 150:36479
    [Google Scholar]
  60. Harris PL, Koenig MA, Corriveau KH, Jaswal VK. 2018. Cognitive foundations of learning from testimony. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 69:25173
    [Google Scholar]
  61. Henderson AME, Scott JC. 2015. She called that thing a mido, but should you call it a mido too? Linguistic experience influences infants’ expectations of conventionality. Front. Psychol. 6:332
    [Google Scholar]
  62. Henderson AME, Woodward AL. 2012. Nine-month-old infants generalize object labels, but not object preferences across individuals. Dev. Sci. 15:564152
    [Google Scholar]
  63. Heyes C. 2023. Rethinking norm psychology. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. In press
    [Google Scholar]
  64. Jensen K, Call J, Tomasello M. 2007. Chimpanzees are vengeful but not spiteful. PNAS 104:321304650
    [Google Scholar]
  65. Jensen K, Vaish A, Schmidt MFH. 2014. The emergence of human prosociality: aligning with others through feelings, concerns, and norms. Front. Psychol. 5:822
    [Google Scholar]
  66. Jin K, Baillargeon R. 2017. Infants possess an abstract expectation of ingroup support. PNAS 114:318199204
    [Google Scholar]
  67. Josephs M, Kushnir T, Gräfenhain M, Rakoczy H. 2016. Children protest moral and conventional violations more when they believe actions are freely chosen. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 141:24755
    [Google Scholar]
  68. Josephs M, Rakoczy H. 2016. Young children think you can opt out of social-conventional but not moral practices. Cogn. Dev. 39:197204
    [Google Scholar]
  69. Kagan J. 1981. The Second Year: The Emergence of Self-Awareness Cambridge, MA: Harvard Univ. Press
  70. Kant I. 1998 (1785). Groundwork of the Metaphysics of Morals Cambridge, UK: Cambridge Univ. Press
  71. Kassecker A, Verschoor SA, Schmidt MFH. 2023. Human infants are aroused and concerned by moral transgressions. PNAS 12031e2306344120
    [Google Scholar]
  72. Kelemen D. 1999a. The scope of teleological thinking in preschool children. Cognition 70:324172
    [Google Scholar]
  73. Kelemen D. 1999b. Function, goals and intention: children's teleological reasoning about objects. Trends Cogn. Sci. 3:1246168
    [Google Scholar]
  74. Kendal R, Hopper LM, Whiten A, Brosnan SF, Lambeth SP et al. 2015. Chimpanzees copy dominant and knowledgeable individuals: implications for cultural diversity. Evol. Hum. Behav. 36:16572
    [Google Scholar]
  75. Keupp S, Behne T, Rakoczy H. 2018. The rationality of (over)imitation. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 13:667887
    [Google Scholar]
  76. Killen M, Dahl A. 2021. Moral reasoning enables developmental and societal change. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 16:6120925
    [Google Scholar]
  77. Killen M, Mulvey KL, Richardson C, Jampol N, Woodward A. 2011. The accidental transgressor: morally-relevant theory of mind. Cognition 119:2197215
    [Google Scholar]
  78. Killen M, Rizzo MT. 2014. Morality, intentionality, and intergroup attitudes. Behaviour 151:2–333759
    [Google Scholar]
  79. Killen M, Smetana JG, eds. 2014. Handbook of Moral Development New York: Psychol. Press. , 2nd ed..
  80. Killen M, Smetana JG. 2015. Origins and development of morality. Handbook of Child Psychology and Developmental Science, Vol. 3 RM Lerner 70149. New York: Wiley-Blackwell. , 7th ed..
    [Google Scholar]
  81. Knobe J. 2003. Intentional action and side effects in ordinary language. Analysis 63:27919094
    [Google Scholar]
  82. Knobe J. 2010. Person as scientist, person as moralist. Behav. Brain Sci. 33:431529
    [Google Scholar]
  83. Koenig MA, Clement F, Harris PL. 2004. Trust in testimony: children's use of true and false statements. Psychol. Sci. 15:1069498
    [Google Scholar]
  84. Koenig MA, Harris PL. 2005. Preschoolers mistrust ignorant and inaccurate speakers. Child Dev 76:6126177
    [Google Scholar]
  85. Kohlberg L. 1963. The development of children's orientations toward a moral order. Hum. Dev. 6:1–21133
    [Google Scholar]
  86. Kohlberg L. 1969. Stage and sequence: the cognitive-developmental approach to socialization. Handbook of Socialization Theory and Research DA Goslin 347480. Chicago: McNally
    [Google Scholar]
  87. Korsgaard CM. 1996. The Sources of Normativity Cambridge, UK: Cambridge Univ. Press
  88. Köymen B, Lieven E, Engemann DA, Rakoczy H, Warneken F, Tomasello M. 2014. Children's norm enforcement in their interactions with peers. Child Dev 85:3110822
    [Google Scholar]
  89. Köymen B, Schmidt MFH, Rost L, Lieven E, Tomasello M. 2015. Teaching versus enforcing game rules in preschoolers’ peer interactions. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 135:93101
    [Google Scholar]
  90. Landrum AR, Eaves BS, Shafto P. 2015. Learning to trust and trusting to learn: a theoretical framework. Trends Cogn. Sci. 19:310911
    [Google Scholar]
  91. Langergraber KE, Boesch C, Inoue E, Inoue-Murayama M, Mitani JC et al. 2011. Genetic and ‘cultural’ similarity in wild chimpanzees. Proc. R. Soc. B 278:170440816
    [Google Scholar]
  92. Lavin D. 2004. Practical reason and the possibility of error. Ethics 114:342457
    [Google Scholar]
  93. Legare CH, Nielsen M. 2015. Imitation and innovation: the dual engines of cultural learning. Trends Cogn. Sci. 19:1168899
    [Google Scholar]
  94. Leslie AM, Knobe J, Cohen A. 2006. Acting intentionally and the side-effect effect: theory of mind and moral judgment. Psychol. Sci. 17:542127
    [Google Scholar]
  95. Li L, Rizzo MT, Burkholder AR, Killen M. 2017. Theory of mind and resource allocation in the context of hidden inequality. Cogn. Dev. 43:2536
    [Google Scholar]
  96. Liberman Z, Howard LH, Vasquez NM, Woodward AL. 2018. Children's expectations about conventional and moral behaviors of ingroup and outgroup members. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 165:718
    [Google Scholar]
  97. Lohse K, Gräfenhain M, Behne T, Rakoczy H. 2014. Young children understand the normative implications of future-directed speech acts. PLOS ONE 9:1e86958
    [Google Scholar]
  98. Lorini G. 2022. Animal norms: an investigation of normativity in the non-human social world. Law Cult. Humanit. 18:365273
    [Google Scholar]
  99. Lucca K, Capelier-Mourguy A, Cirelli L, Byers-Heinlein K, Ben RD et al. 2021. Infants’ social evaluation of helpers and hinderers: a large-scale, multi-lab, coordinated replication study. PsyArXiv. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/qhxkm
    [Crossref]
  100. Lucca K, Pospisil J, Sommerville JA. 2018. Fairness informs social decision making in infancy. PLOS ONE 13:2e0192848
    [Google Scholar]
  101. Luncz LV, Boesch C. 2014. Tradition over trend: neighboring chimpanzee communities maintain differences in cultural behavior despite frequent immigration of adult females. Am. J. Primatol. 76:764957
    [Google Scholar]
  102. Luncz LV, Mundry R, Boesch C. 2012. Evidence for cultural differences between neighboring chimpanzee communities. Curr. Biol. 22:1092226
    [Google Scholar]
  103. Margoni F, Surian L. 2018. Infants’ evaluation of prosocial and antisocial agents: a meta-analysis. Dev. Psychol. 54:144555
    [Google Scholar]
  104. McDowell J. 1984. Wittgenstein on following a rule. Synthese 58:332563
    [Google Scholar]
  105. Meristo M, Strid K, Surian L. 2016. Preverbal infants’ ability to encode the outcome of distributive actions. Infancy 21:335372
    [Google Scholar]
  106. Mikhail J. 2007. Universal moral grammar: theory, evidence and the future. Trends Cogn. Sci. 11:414352
    [Google Scholar]
  107. Millikan RG. 1990. Truth, rules, hoverflies, and the Kripke-Wittgenstein paradox. Philos. Rev. 99:332353
    [Google Scholar]
  108. Mills CM. 2013. Knowing when to doubt: developing a critical stance when learning from others. Dev. Psychol. 49:340418
    [Google Scholar]
  109. Nagel T. 1970. The Possibility of Altruism Oxford, UK: Clarendon Press
  110. Nagel T. 1986. The View from Nowhere New York: Oxford Univ. Press
  111. Neary KR, Friedman O, Burnstein CL. 2009. Preschoolers infer ownership from “control of permission. .” Dev. Psychol 45:387376
    [Google Scholar]
  112. Nichols S. 2004. Sentimental Rules: On the Natural Foundations of Moral Judgment Oxford, UK: Oxford Univ. Press
  113. Nobes G. 1999. Children's understanding of rules they invent themselves. J. Moral Educ. 28:221532
    [Google Scholar]
  114. Nucci LP. 2014. The personal and the moral. See Killen & Smetana 2014 53858
  115. Nurmsoo E, Robinson EJ, Butterfill SA. 2010. Children's selective learning from others. Rev. Philos. Psychol. 1:451161
    [Google Scholar]
  116. Parsons T. 1951. The Social System London: Routledge & Kegan Paul
  117. Paulus M, Christner N, Wörle M. 2020. The normative status of friendship: Do young children enforce sharing with friends and appreciate reasonable partiality?. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 194:104826
    [Google Scholar]
  118. Pea RD. 1982. Origins of verbal logic: spontaneous denials by two- and three-year olds. J. Child Lang. 9:3597626
    [Google Scholar]
  119. Pellizzoni S, Siegal M, Surian L. 2009. Foreknowledge, caring, and the side-effect effect in young children. Dev. Psychol. 45:128995
    [Google Scholar]
  120. Pettit P. 1993. The Common Mind: An Essay on Psychology, Society, and Politics New York: Oxford Univ. Press
  121. Piaget J. 1932. The Moral Judgment of the Child London: Routledge & Kegan Paul
  122. Pinker S. 1994. The Language Instinct: How the Mind Creates Language New York: William Morrow
  123. Popitz H. 2006. Soziale Normen Frankfurt am Main, Ger.: Suhrkamp
  124. Rakoczy H, Behne T, Clüver A, Dallmann S, Weidner S, Waldmann MR. 2015. The side-effect effect in children is robust and not specific to the moral status of action effects. PLOS ONE 10:7e0132933
    [Google Scholar]
  125. Rakoczy H, Brosche N, Warneken F, Tomasello M. 2009. Young children's understanding of the context relativity of normative rules in conventional games. Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 27:44556
    [Google Scholar]
  126. Rakoczy H, Kaufmann M, Lohse K. 2016. Young children understand the normative force of standards of equal resource distribution. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 150:396403
    [Google Scholar]
  127. Rakoczy H, Schmidt MFH. 2013. The early ontogeny of social norms. Child Dev. Perspect. 7:11721
    [Google Scholar]
  128. Rakoczy H, Tomasello M. 2007. The ontogeny of social ontology: steps to shared intentionality and status functions. Intentional Acts and Institutional Facts: Essays on John Searle's Social Ontology SL Tsohatzidis 11337. Berlin: Springer-Verlag
    [Google Scholar]
  129. Rakoczy H, Tomasello M. 2009. Done wrong or said wrong? Young children understand the normative directions of fit of different speech acts. Cognition 113:220512
    [Google Scholar]
  130. Rakoczy H, Warneken F, Tomasello M. 2008. The sources of normativity: young children's awareness of the normative structure of games. Dev. Psychol. 44:387581
    [Google Scholar]
  131. Riedl K, Jensen K, Call J, Tomasello M. 2012. No third-party punishment in chimpanzees. PNAS 109:371482429
    [Google Scholar]
  132. Rizzo MT, Elenbaas L, Cooley S, Killen M. 2016. Children's recognition of fairness and others’ welfare in a resource allocation task: age related changes. Dev. Psychol. 52:8130717
    [Google Scholar]
  133. Roberts SO. 2021. Descriptive-to-prescriptive (D2P) reasoning: an early emerging bias to maintain the status quo. Eur. Rev. Soc. Psychol. 33:2289322
    [Google Scholar]
  134. Roberts SO, Gelman SA, Ho AK. 2017. So it is, so it shall be: Group regularities license children's prescriptive judgments. Cogn. Sci. 41:S3576600
    [Google Scholar]
  135. Robinson EJ, Einav S, eds. 2014. Trust and Skepticism: Children's Selective Learning from Testimony New York: Psychol. Press
  136. Ross H, Friedman O, Field A. 2015. Toddlers assert and acknowledge ownership rights. Soc. Dev. 24:234156
    [Google Scholar]
  137. Rossano F, Rakoczy H, Tomasello M. 2011. Young children's understanding of violations of property rights. Cognition 121:221927
    [Google Scholar]
  138. Roughley N. 2019. Moral obligation from the outside in. See Bayertz & Roughley 2019 21444
  139. Rousseau J-J. 1997 (1762). The Social Contract and Other Later Political Writings Cambridge, UK: Cambridge Univ. Press
  140. Rudolf von Rohr C, Burkart JM, van Schaik CP. 2011. Evolutionary precursors of social norms in chimpanzees: a new approach. Biol. Philos. 26:1130
    [Google Scholar]
  141. Rudolf von Rohr C, Koski SE, Burkart JM, Caws C, Fraser ON et al. 2012. Impartial third-party interventions in captive chimpanzees: a reflection of community concern. PLOS ONE 7:3e32494
    [Google Scholar]
  142. Salvadori E, Blazsekova T, Volein A, Karap Z, Tatone D et al. 2015. Probing the strength of infants’ preference for helpers over hinderers: two replication attempts of Hamlin and Wynn (2011). PLOS ONE 10:11e0140570
    [Google Scholar]
  143. Scarf D, Imuta K, Colombo M, Hayne H. 2012. Social evaluation or simple association? Simple associations may explain moral reasoning in infants. PLOS ONE 7:8e42698
    [Google Scholar]
  144. Schlingloff L, Csibra G, Tatone D. 2020. Do 15-month-old infants prefer helpers? A replication of Hamlin et al. (2007). R. Soc. Open Sci. 7:4191795
    [Google Scholar]
  145. Schmid HB 2009. Plural Action. Essays in Philosophy and Social Science Dordrecht, Neth.: Springer
  146. Schmid HB. 2011. The idiocy of strategic reasoning. Towards an account of consensual action. Anal. Krit. 33:13556
    [Google Scholar]
  147. Schmidt MFH, Butler LP, Heinz J, Tomasello M. 2016a. Young children see a single action and infer a social norm: promiscuous normativity in 3-year-olds. Psychol. Sci. 27:10136070
    [Google Scholar]
  148. Schmidt MFH, Hardecker S, Tomasello M. 2016b. Preschoolers understand the normativity of cooperatively structured competition. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 143:3447
    [Google Scholar]
  149. Schmidt MFH, Rakoczy H. 2018. Developing an understanding of normativity. Oxford Handbook of 4E Cognition, ed. A Newen, L de Bruin, S Gallagher 685706. Oxford, UK: Oxford Univ. Press
    [Google Scholar]
  150. Schmidt MFH, Rakoczy H. 2019. On the uniqueness of human normative attitudes. See Bayertz & Roughley 2019 12135
  151. Schmidt MFH, Rakoczy H, Mietzsch T, Tomasello M. 2016c. Young children understand the role of agreement in establishing arbitrary norms—but unanimity is key. Child Dev 87:261226
    [Google Scholar]
  152. Schmidt MFH, Rakoczy H, Tomasello M. 2011. Young children attribute normativity to novel actions without pedagogy or normative language. Dev. Sci. 14:353039
    [Google Scholar]
  153. Schmidt MFH, Rakoczy H, Tomasello M. 2012. Young children enforce social norms selectively depending on the violator's group affiliation. Cognition 124:332533
    [Google Scholar]
  154. Schmidt MFH, Rakoczy H, Tomasello M. 2013. Young children understand and defend the entitlements of others. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 116:493044
    [Google Scholar]
  155. Schmidt MFH, Rakoczy H, Tomasello M. 2019. Eighteen-month-old infants correct non-conforming actions by others. Infancy 24:461335
    [Google Scholar]
  156. Schmidt MFH, Sommerville JA. 2011. Fairness expectations and altruistic sharing in 15-month-old human infants. PLOS ONE 6:10e23223
    [Google Scholar]
  157. Schmidt MFH, Svetlova M, Johe J, Tomasello M. 2016d. Children's developing understanding of legitimate reasons for allocating resources unequally. Cogn. Dev. 37:4252
    [Google Scholar]
  158. Schmidt MFH, Tomasello M. 2012. Young children enforce social norms. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 21:423236
    [Google Scholar]
  159. Schmidt MFH, Tomasello M. 2016. How chimpanzees cooperate: if dominance is artificially constrained. PNAS 113:44E672829
    [Google Scholar]
  160. Schuwerk T, Kampis D, Baillargeon R, Biro S, Bohn M et al. 2023. Action anticipation based on an agent's epistemic state in toddlers and adults. PsyArXiv. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/x4jbm
  161. Searle JR. 1969. Speech Acts: An Essay in the Philosophy of Language Cambridge, UK: Cambridge Univ. Press
  162. Searle JR. 1983. Intentionality: An Essay in the Philosophy of Mind Cambridge, UK: Cambridge Univ. Press
  163. Searle JR. 2001. Rationality in Action Cambridge, MA: MIT Press
  164. Sellars W. 1962. Truth and correspondence. J. Philos. 59:22956
    [Google Scholar]
  165. Shweder RA 1986. Divergent rationalities. Metatheory in Social Science: Pluralisms and Subjectivities, ed. DW Fiske, RA Shweder 16396. Chicago: Univ. Chicago Press
    [Google Scholar]
  166. Sloane S, Baillargeon R, Premack D. 2012. Do infants have a sense of fairness?. Psychol. Sci. 23:2196204
    [Google Scholar]
  167. Smetana JG. 2006. Social–cognitive domain theory: consistencies and variations in children's moral and social judgments. In Handbook of Moral Development, ed. M Killen, J Smetana 11954 Mahway, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Assoc. 1st ed.
  168. Smetana JG, Jambon M, Conry-Murray C, Sturge-Apple ML. 2012. Reciprocal associations between young children's developing moral judgments and theory of mind. Dev. Psychol. 48:4114455
    [Google Scholar]
  169. Smith M. 1994. The Moral Problem Oxford, UK: Blackwell
  170. Sodian B, Licata M, Kristen-Antonow S, Paulus M, Killen M, Woodward A. 2016. Understanding of goals, beliefs, and desires predicts morally relevant theory of mind: a longitudinal investigation. Child Dev 87:4122132
    [Google Scholar]
  171. Sommerville JA, Schmidt MFH, Yun J, Burns M. 2013. The development of fairness expectations and prosocial behavior in the second year of life. Infancy 18:14066
    [Google Scholar]
  172. Sripada CS, Stich S 2006. A framework for the psychology of norms. The Innate Mind, Vol. 2 Culture and Cognition P Carruthers, S Laurence, SP Stich 280301. New York: Oxford Univ. Press
    [Google Scholar]
  173. Suchak M, de Waal FBM. 2016. Reply to Schmidt and Tomasello: chimpanzees as natural team-players. PNAS 113:44E6730
    [Google Scholar]
  174. Suchak M, Eppley TM, Campbell MW, Feldman RA, Quarles LF, de Waal FBM. 2016. How chimpanzees cooperate in a competitive world. PNAS 113:361021520
    [Google Scholar]
  175. Tan E, Mikami AY, Hamlin JK. 2018. Do infant sociomoral evaluation and action studies predict preschool social and behavioral adjustment?. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 176:3954
    [Google Scholar]
  176. Tennie C, Call J, Tomasello M. 2009. Ratcheting up the ratchet: on the evolution of cumulative culture. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B 364:1528240515
    [Google Scholar]
  177. Tomasello M. 2014. A Natural History of Human Thinking Cambridge, MA: Harvard Univ. Press
  178. Tomasello M. 2016. A Natural History of Human Morality Cambridge, MA: Harvard Univ. Press
  179. Tomasello M. 2020. The ontogenetic foundations of epistemic norms. Episteme 17:330115
    [Google Scholar]
  180. Tomasello M, Rakoczy H. 2003. What makes human cognition unique? From individual to shared to collective intentionality. Mind Lang 18:212147
    [Google Scholar]
  181. Townsend SW, Slocombe KE, Emery Thompson M, Zuberbühler K 2007. Female-led infanticide in wild chimpanzees. Curr. Biol. 17:10R35556
    [Google Scholar]
  182. Träuble B, Pauen S. 2007. The role of functional information for infant categorization. Cognition 105:236279
    [Google Scholar]
  183. Träuble B, Pauen S. 2011. Cause or effect: what matters? How 12-month-old infants learn to categorize artifacts. Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 29:Pt 335774
    [Google Scholar]
  184. Tunçgenç B, Hohenberger A, Rakoczy H. 2015. Early understanding of normativity and freedom to act in Turkish toddlers. J. Cogn. Dev. 16:14454
    [Google Scholar]
  185. Turiel E. 1983. The Development of Social Knowledge: Morality and Convention Cambridge, UK: Cambridge Univ. Press
  186. Turiel E. 2002. The Culture of Morality: Social Development, Context, and Conflict Cambridge, UK: Cambridge Univ. Press
  187. Turiel E 2006. The development of morality. Handbook of Child Psychology, Vol. 3 Social, Emotional, and Personality Development, ed. W Damon, RM Lerner, N Eisenberg 789857. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. , 6th ed..
    [Google Scholar]
  188. Turiel E, Dahl A. 2019. The development of domains of moral and conventional norms, coordination in decision-making, and the implications of social opposition. See Bayertz & Roughley 2019 195213
  189. Ulber J, Hamann K, Tomasello M. 2017. Young children, but not chimpanzees, are averse to disadvantageous and advantageous inequities. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 155:4866
    [Google Scholar]
  190. Vaish A, Carpenter M, Tomasello M. 2010. Young children selectively avoid helping people with harmful intentions. Child Dev 81:6166169
    [Google Scholar]
  191. Vaish A, Missana M, Tomasello M. 2011. Three-year-old children intervene in third-party moral transgressions. Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 29:112430
    [Google Scholar]
  192. Van de Vondervoort JW, Aknin LB, Kushnir T, Slevinsky J, Hamlin JK. 2018. Selectivity in toddlers’ behavioral and emotional reactions to prosocial and antisocial others. Dev. Psychol. 54:1114
    [Google Scholar]
  193. Van de Vondervoort JW, Hamlin JK. 2018. The early emergence of sociomoral evaluation: infants prefer prosocial others. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 20:7781
    [Google Scholar]
  194. Van Leeuwen EJC, Cronin KA, Schütte S, Call J, Haun DBM. 2013. Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) flexibly adjust their behaviour in order to maximize payoffs, not to conform to majorities. PLOS ONE 8:11e80945
    [Google Scholar]
  195. Van Wye EM, Wang M, Roberts SO. 2021. Explanations for norm violations affect preschoolers’ judgments of norm violators. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 150:168894
    [Google Scholar]
  196. von Rohr CR, van Schaik CP, Kissling A, Burkart JM. 2015. Chimpanzees’ bystander reactions to infanticide. Hum. Nat. 26:214360
    [Google Scholar]
  197. Westra E, Andrews K. 2022. A pluralistic framework for the psychology of norms. Biol. Philos. 37:540
    [Google Scholar]
  198. Whiten A. 2011. The scope of culture in chimpanzees, humans and ancestral apes. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B 366:15679971007
    [Google Scholar]
  199. Whiten A. 2019. Social learning: peering deeper into ape culture. Curr. Biol. 29:17R84547
    [Google Scholar]
  200. Whiten A. 2021. The burgeoning reach of animal culture. Science 372:6537eabe6514
    [Google Scholar]
  201. Whiten A, Spiteri A, Horner V, Bonnie KE, Lambeth SP et al. 2007. Transmission of multiple traditions within and between chimpanzee groups. Curr. Biol. 17:12103843
    [Google Scholar]
  202. Winch P. 1958. The Idea of a Social Science and Its Relation to Philosophy London: Routledge & Kegan Paul
  203. Wittgenstein L. 2001 (1953). Philosophical Investigations: The German Text, with a Revised English Translation Malden, MA: Blackwell Publ. , 3rd ed..
  204. Woo BM, Tan E, Hamlin JK. 2022. Human morality is based on an early-emerging moral core. Annu. Rev. Dev. Psychol. 4:4161
    [Google Scholar]
  205. Wrangham RW, Koops K, Machanda ZP, Worthington S, Bernard AB et al. 2016. Distribution of a chimpanzee social custom is explained by matrilineal relationship rather than conformity. Curr. Biol. 26:22303337
    [Google Scholar]
  206. Wyman E, Rakoczy H, Tomasello M. 2009. Normativity and context in young children's pretend play. Cogn. Dev. 24:214655
    [Google Scholar]
  207. Wynn K, Bloom P. 2014. The moral baby. See Killen & Smetana 2014 43553
  208. Yucel M, Hepach R, Vaish A. 2020. Young children and adults show differential arousal to moral and conventional transgressions. Front. Psychol. 11:548
    [Google Scholar]
  209. Ziv T, Whiteman JD, Sommerville JA. 2021. Toddlers’ interventions toward fair and unfair individuals. Cognition 214:104781
    [Google Scholar]
/content/journals/10.1146/annurev-devpsych-120621-034731
Loading
  • Article Type: Review Article
This is a required field
Please enter a valid email address
Approval was a Success
Invalid data
An Error Occurred
Approval was partially successful, following selected items could not be processed due to error