1932

Abstract

As a fundamental attention regulation process, inhibition serves to selectively filter out distraction (i.e., access), dampen activation of automatically activated irrelevant or no-longer-relevant information (i.e., deletion), and select for task-appropriate thoughts and/or responses in the face of competition (i.e., restraint). Inhibition allows us to direct attention or processing toward target information, thoughts, or actions. It is affected by several factors and is especially compromised with aging as demonstrated in both behavioral and neuroimaging research. Nevertheless, older adults show significant and durable plasticity in inhibition performance. Heightened distractibility as a result of impaired inhibition in older adults can manifest as a cost or a benefit with a tendency to be related to better creativity performance when distraction becomes task relevant. In this article, we review the components of inhibitory theory, summarizing intraindividual variability and interindividual differences in inhibition, with a specific focus on the inhibitory deficit associated with aging. The review also brings together theories and empirical findings to support the relationship between inhibitory deficit and creativity in the context of aging.

Loading

Article metrics loading...

/content/journals/10.1146/annurev-devpsych-121020-030705
2022-12-09
2024-05-01
Loading full text...

Full text loading...

/deliver/fulltext/devpsych/4/1/annurev-devpsych-121020-030705.html?itemId=/content/journals/10.1146/annurev-devpsych-121020-030705&mimeType=html&fmt=ahah

Literature Cited

  1. Adnan A, Beaty R, Silvia P, Spreng RN, Turner GR. 2019. Creative aging: functional brain networks associated with divergent thinking in older and younger adults. Neurobiol. Aging 75:150–58 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2018.11.004
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  2. Amer T, Anderson J, Campbell KL, Hasher L, Grady CL. 2016. Age differences in the neural correlates of distraction regulation: a network interaction approach. NeuroImage 139:231–39 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.06.036
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  3. Amer T, Giovanello KS, Nichol DR, Hasher L, Grady CL. 2019. Neural correlates of enhanced memory for meaningful associations with age. Cereb. Cortex 29:114568–79 https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhy334
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  4. Amer T, Hasher L. 2014. Conceptual processing of distractors by older but not younger adults. Psychol. Sci. 25:122252–58 https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797614555725
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  5. Amer T, Ngo KJ, Weeks JC, Hasher L. 2020. Spontaneous distractor reactivation with age: evidence for bound target-distractor representations in memory. Psychol. Sci. 31:101315–24 https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797620951125
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  6. Amer T, Wynn JS, Hasher L. 2022. Cluttered memory representations shape cognition in old age. . Trends Cogn. Sci. 26:3255–67 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2021.12.002
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  7. Anderson JAE, Campbell KL, Amer T, Grady CL, Hasher L. 2014. Timing is everything: Age differences in the cognitive control network are modulated by time of day. Psychol. Aging 29:3648–57 https://doi.org/10.1037/a0037243
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  8. Anderson MC, Hanslmayr S. 2014. Neural mechanisms of motivated forgetting. Trends Cogn. Sci. 18:6279–92 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2014.03.002
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  9. Apšvalka D, Ferreira CS, Schmitz TW, Rowe JB, Anderson MC 2022. Dynamic targeting enables domain-general inhibitory control over action and thought by the prefrontal cortex. Nat. Commun. 13:1274 https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-021-27926-w
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  10. Ash IK, Wiley J. 2006. The nature of restructuring in insight: an individual-differences approach. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 13:166–73 https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03193814
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  11. Baltes PB, Smith J. 2008. The fascination of wisdom: its nature, ontogeny, and function. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 3:156–64 https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-6916.2008.00062.x
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  12. Baltes PB, Smith J, Staudinger UM. 1992. Wisdom and successful aging. Psychology and Aging: Nebraska Symposium on Motivation 1991 TB Sonderegger 123–67 Lincoln, NE: Univ. Neb. Press
    [Google Scholar]
  13. Benedek M, Franz F, Heene M, Neubauer AC. 2012. Differential effects of cognitive inhibition and intelligence on creativity. Personal. Individ. Differ. 53:4480–85 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2012.04.014
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  14. Benedek M, Jauk E 2018. Spontaneous and controlled processes in creative cognition. The Oxford Handbook of Spontaneous Thought: Mind-Wandering, Creativity, Dreaming, and Clinical Conditions KCR Fox, K Christoff 285–98 New York: Oxford Univ. Press https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780190464745.013.22
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  15. Benedek M, Jauk E, Sommer M, Arendasy M, Neubauer AC. 2014. Intelligence, creativity, and cognitive control: the common and differential involvement of executive functions in intelligence and creativity. Intelligence 46:73–83 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2014.05.007
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  16. Biss RK, Campbell KL, Hasher L. 2013. Interference from previous distraction disrupts older adults’ memory. J. Gerontol. Ser. B 68:4558–61 https://doi.org/10.1093/geronb/gbs074
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  17. Biss RK, Hasher L, Thomas RC. 2010. Positive mood is associated with the implicit use of distraction. Motiv. Emot. 34:173–77 https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-010-9156-y
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  18. Biss RK, Rowe G, Weeks JC, Hasher L, Murphy KJ. 2018. Leveraging older adults’ susceptibility to distraction to improve memory for face-name associations. Psychol. Aging 33:1158–64 https://doi.org/10.1037/pag0000192
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  19. Biss RK, Weeks JC, Hasher L. 2012. Happily distracted: mood and a benefit of attention dysregulation in older adults. Front. Psychol. 3:399 https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00399
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  20. Bjorklund DF, Harnishfeger KK. 1990. The resources construct in cognitive development: diverse sources of evidence and a theory of inefficient inhibition. Dev. Rev. 10:148–71 https://doi.org/10.1016/0273-2297(90)90004-N
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  21. Campbell KL, Hasher L. 2018. Hyper-binding only apparent under fully implicit test conditions. Psychol. Aging 33:1176–81 https://doi.org/10.1037/pag0000216
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  22. Campbell KL, Hasher L, Thomas RC. 2010. Hyper-binding: a unique age effect. Psychol. Sci. 21:3399–405 https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797609359910
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  23. Campbell KL, Lustig C, Hasher L. 2020. Aging and inhibition: introduction to the special issue. Psychol. Aging 35:5605–13 https://doi.org/10.1037/pag0000564
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  24. Campbell KL, Trelle A, Hasher L. 2014. Hyper-binding across time: age differences in the effect of temporal proximity on paired-associate learning. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 40:1293–99 https://doi.org/10.1037/a0034109
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  25. Carpenter SM, Chae RL, Yoon C 2020. Creativity and aging: positive consequences of distraction. Psychol. Aging 35:5654–62 https://doi.org/10.1037/pag0000470
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  26. Carson SH. 2011. Creativity and psychopathology: a shared vulnerability model. Can. J. Psychiatry 56:3144–53 https://doi.org/10.1177/070674371105600304
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  27. Carson SH, Peterson JB, Higgins DM. 2003. Decreased latent inhibition is associated with increased creative achievement in high-functioning individuals. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 85:3499–506 https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.85.3.499
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  28. Carstensen LL. 2021. Socioemotional selectivity theory: the role of perceived endings in human motivation. Gerontologist 61:81188–96 https://doi.org/10.1093/geront/gnab116
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  29. Cera R, Cristini C, Antonietti A. 2018. Conceptions of learning, well-being, and creativity in older adults. J. Educ. Cult. Psychol. Stud. 18:241–73 https://doi.org/10.7358/ecps-2018-018-cera
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  30. Colcombe SJ, Kramer AF, Erickson KI, Scalf P. 2005. The implications of cortical recruitment and brain morphology for individual differences in inhibitory function in aging humans. Psychol. Aging 20:3363–75 https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.20.3.363
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  31. Colombo B, Antonietti A, Daneau B. 2018. The relationships between cognitive reserve and creativity. a study on American aging population. Front. Psychol. 9:764 https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00764
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  32. Connelly SL, Hasher L, Zacks RT. 1991. Age and reading: the impact of distraction. Psychol. Aging 6:4533–41 https://doi.org/10.1037//0882-7974.6.4.533
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  33. Coxon JP, Goble DJ, Leunissen I, Van Impe A, Wenderoth N, Swinnen SP. 2016. Functional brain activation associated with inhibitory control deficits in older adults. Cereb. Cortex 26:112–22 https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhu165
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  34. Davidson DJ, Zacks RT, Williams CC. 2003. Stroop interference, practice, and aging. Aging Neuropsychol. Cogn. 10:285–98 https://doi.org/10.1076/anec.10.2.85.14463
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  35. Dietrich A. 2004. The cognitive neuroscience of creativity. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 11:61011–26 https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03196731
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  36. Dolcos F, Iordan A, Dolcos S. 2011. Neural correlates of emotion-cognition interactions: a review of evidence from brain imaging investigations. J. Cogn. Psychol. 23:6669–94 https://doi.org/10.1080/20445911.2011.594433
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  37. Dorfman L, Martindale C, Gassimova V, Vartanian O. 2008. Creativity and speed of information processing: a double dissociation involving elementary versus inhibitory cognitive tasks. Personal. Individ. Differ. 44:61382–90 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2007.12.006
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  38. Enns JT, Cameron S 1987. Selective attention in young children: The relations between visual search, filtering, and priming. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 44:138–63 https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-0965(87)90021-X
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  39. Esposito R, Cieri F, Chiacchiaretta P, Cera N, Lauriola M et al. 2018. Modifications in resting state functional anticorrelation between default mode network and dorsal attention network: comparison among young adults, healthy elders and mild cognitive impairment patients. Brain Imaging Behav 12:1127–41 https://doi.org/10.1007/s11682-017-9686-y
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  40. Eysenck HJ. 1995. Genius: The Natural History of Creativity Cambridge, UK: Cambridge Univ. Press
  41. Fox MD, Corbetta M, Snyder AZ, Vincent JL, Raichle ME. 2006. Spontaneous neuronal activity distinguishes human dorsal and ventral attention systems. PNAS 103:2610046–51 https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0604187103
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  42. Gazzaley A, Cooney JW, Rissman J, D'Esposito M 2005. Top-down suppression deficit underlies working memory impairment in normal aging. Nat. Neurosci. 8:101298–300 https://doi.org/10.1038/nn1543
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  43. Goldstein D, Hahn CS, Hasher L, Wiprzycka UJ, Zelazo PD. 2007. Time of day, intellectual performance, and behavioral problems in morning versus evening type adolescents: Is there a synchrony effect?. Personal. Individ. Differ. 42:3431–40 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2006.07.008
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  44. Greenwood TA. 2017. Positive traits in the bipolar spectrum: the space between madness and genius. Mol. Neuropsychiatry 2:4198–212 https://doi.org/10.1159/000452416
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  45. Grossmann I, Na J, Varnum MEW, Park DC, Kitayama S, Nisbett RE. 2010. Reasoning about social conflicts improves into old age. PNAS 107:167246–50 https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1001715107
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  46. Guilford JP. 1959. Three faces of intellect. Am. Psychol. 14:8469–79 https://doi.org/10.1037/h0046827
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  47. Guimet NM, Miller BL, Allegri RF, Rankin KP. 2021. What do we mean by behavioral disinhibition in frontotemporal dementia?. Front. Neurol. 12:707799 https://doi.org/10.3389/fneur.2021.707799
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  48. Hamm VP, Hasher L. 1992. Age and the availability of inferences. Psychol. Aging 7:156–64 https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.7.1.56
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  49. Harnishfeger KK, Pope RS. 1996. Intending to forget: the development of cognitive inhibition in directed forgetting. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 62:2292–315 https://doi.org/10.1006/jecp.1996.0032
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  50. Hartman M, Hasher L. 1991. Aging and suppression: memory for previously relevant information. Psychol. Aging 6:4587–94 https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.6.4.587
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  51. Hasher L 2015. Inhibitory deficit hypothesis. Encyclopedia of Adulthood and Aging SK Whitbourne 640–63 West Sussex, UK: Wiley & Sons https://doi.org/10.1002/9781118521373.wbeaa259
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  52. Hasher L, Campbell K 2020. Inhibitory theory: assumptions, findings, and relevance to interventions. The Cambridge Handbook of Cognitive Aging: A Life Course Perspective A Thomas, A Gutchess 147–60 Cambridge, UK: Cambridge Univ. Press https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108552684.010
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  53. Hasher L, Goldstein D, May CP 2005. It's about time: circadian rhythms, memory, and aging. Human Learning and Memory: Advances in Theory And Application: The 4th Tsukuba International Conference on Memory C Izawa, N Ohta 199–217 Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Assoc.
    [Google Scholar]
  54. Hasher L, Zacks RT. 1988. Working memory, comprehension, and aging: a review and a new view. Psychol. Learn. Motiv. 22:193–225 https://doi.org/10.1016/S0079-7421(08)60041-9
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  55. Hasher L, Zacks RT, May CP 1999. Inhibitory control, circadian arousal, and age. Attention and Performance XVII: Cognitive Regulation of Performance: Interaction of Theory and Application D Gopher, A Koriat 653–75 Cambridge, MA: MIT Press
    [Google Scholar]
  56. Healey MK, Campbell KL, Hasher L. 2008. Cognitive aging and increased distractibility: costs and potential benefits. Prog. Brain Res 169:353–63 https://doi.org/10.1016/S0079-6123(07)00022-2
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  57. Healey MK, Hasher L, Campbell KL. 2013. The role of suppression in resolving interference: evidence for an age-related deficit. Psychol. Aging 28:3721–28 https://doi.org/10.1037/a0033003
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  58. Healey MK, Ngo KW, Hasher L. 2014. Below-baseline suppression of competitors during interference resolution by younger but not older adults. Psychol. Sci. 25:1145–51 https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797613501169
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  59. Hsieh S, Wu M, Tang CH. 2016. Adaptive strategies for the elderly in inhibiting irrelevant and conflict no-go trials while performing the go/no-go task. Front. Aging Neurosci. 7:243 https://doi.org/10.3389/fnagi.2015.00243
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  60. Hung Y, Gaillard SL, Yarmak P, Arsalidou M. 2018. Dissociations of cognitive inhibition, response inhibition, and emotional interference: voxelwise ALE meta-analyses of fMRI studies. Hum. Brain Mapp 39:104065–82 https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.24232
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  61. Ikier S, Hasher L. 2006. Age differences in implicit interference. J. Gerontol. Ser. B 61:5278–84 https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02109.x
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  62. Ikier S, Yang L, Hasher L 2008. Implicit proactive interference, age, and automatic versus controlled retrieval strategies. Psychol. Sci. 19:5456–61 https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02109.x
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  63. Ji Y, Wang J, Chen T, Du X, Zhan Y. 2016. Plasticity of inhibitory processes and associated far-transfer effects in older adults. Psychol. Aging 31:5415–29 https://doi.org/10.1037/pag0000102
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  64. Jost K, Bryck RL, Vogel EK, Mayr U. 2011. Are old adults just like low working memory young adults? Filtering efficiency and age differences in visual working memory. Cereb. Cortex 21:51147–54 https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhq185
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  65. Kapur N, Cole J, Manly T, Viskontas I, Ninteman A et al. 2013. Positive clinical neuroscience: explorations in positive neurology. Neuroscientist 19:4354–69 https://doi.org/10.1177/1073858412470976
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  66. Kasof J. 1995. Explaining creativity: The attributional perspective. Creativity Res. J. 8:4311–66 https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326934crj0804_1
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  67. Kim S, Rasher L, Zacks RT. 2007. Aging and a benefit of distractibility. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 14:2301–5 https://doi.org/10.3758/bf03194068
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  68. Knight M, Seymour TL, Gaunt JT, Baker C, Nesmith K, Mather M. 2007. Aging and goal-directed emotional attention: distraction reverses emotional biases. Emotion 7:4705–14 https://doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.7.4.705
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  69. Knowlton BJ, Castel AD. 2022. Memory and reward-based learning: a value-directed remembering perspective. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 73:25–52 https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-032921-050951
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  70. Kumari V, Ettinger U 2010. Latent inhibition in schizophrenia and schizotypy: a review of the empirical literature. Latent Inhibition: Cognition, Neuroscience and Applications to Schizophrenia RE Lubow, I Weiner 419–47 Camrbidge, UK: Cambridge Univ. Press https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511730184.018
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  71. Leon SA, Altmann LJ, Abrams L, Gonzalez Rothi LJ, Heilman KM. 2014. Divergent task performance in older adults: declarative memory or creative potential?. Creat. Res. J. 26:121–29 https://doi.org/10.1080/10400419.2014.873657
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  72. Lim KT, Yu R 2015. Aging and wisdom: age-related changes in economic and social decision making. Front. Aging Neurosci. 7:120 https://doi.org/10.3389/fnagi.2015.00120
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  73. Lustig C, Hasher L, Tonev ST. 2006. Distraction as a determinant of processing speed. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 13:619–25 https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03193972
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  74. Madore KP, Jing HG, Schacter DL. 2016. Divergent creative thinking in young and older adults: extending the effects of an episodic specificity induction. Mem. Cogn. 44:6974–88 https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-016-0605-z
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  75. Madore KP, Schacter DL. 2014. An episodic specificity induction enhances means-end problem solving in young and older adults. Psychol. Aging 29:4913–24 https://doi.org/10.1037/a0038209
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  76. Martindale C. 1999. Biological bases of creativity. Handbook of Creativity RJ Sternberg 137–52 Cambridge, UK: Cambridge Univ. Press
    [Google Scholar]
  77. May CP. 1999. Synchrony effects in cognition: the costs and a benefit. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 6:1142–47 https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03210822
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  78. May CP, Hasher L. 1998. Synchrony effects in inhibitory control over thought and action. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 24:2363–79 https://doi.org/10.1037//0096-1523.24.2.363
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  79. May CP, Hasher L, Foong N. 2005. Implicit memory, age, and time of day: paradoxical priming effects. Psychol. Sci. 16:296–100 https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0956-7976.2005.00788.x
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  80. McCrae RR, Arenberg D, Costa PT. 1987. Declines in divergent thinking with age: cross-sectional, longitudinal, and cross-sequential analyses. Psychol. Aging 2:2130–37 https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.2.2.130
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  81. Mednick SA. 1962. The associative basis of the creative process. Psychol. Rev. 69:3220–32 https://doi.org/10.1037/h0048850
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  82. Migliaccio R, Tanguy D, Bouzigues A, Sezer I, Dubois B et al. 2020. Cognitive and behavioural inhibition deficits in neurodegenerative dementias. Cortex 131:265–83 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2020.08.001
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  83. Miller ZA, Miller BL. 2013. Artistic creativity and dementia. Prog. Brain Res. 204:99–112 https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-444-63287-6.00005-1
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  84. Mischel W, Shoda Y, Rodriguez ML. 1989. Delay of gratification in children. Science 244:4907933–38 https://doi.org/10.1126/science.2658056
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  85. Ngo KWJ, Amer T, Healey MK, Hasher L, Alain C 2021. Electrophysiological signature of suppression of competitors during interference resolution. Brain Res 1767:147564 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2021.147564
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  86. Ngo KWJ, Biss RK, Hasher L. 2018. Time of day effects on the use of distraction to minimise forgetting. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 71:112334–41 https://doi.org/10.1177/1747021817740808
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  87. Ngo KWJ, Hasher L. 2017. Optimal testing time for suppression of competitors during interference resolution. Memory 25:101396–401 https://doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2017.1309437
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  88. Nobukawa S, Yamanishi T, Ueno K, Mizukami K, Nishimura H, Takahashi T. 2020. High phase synchronization in alpha band activity in older subjects with high creativity. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 14:583049 https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2020.583049
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  89. Obeso I, Wilkinson L, Casabona E, Bringas ML, Álvarez M et al. 2011. Deficits in inhibitory control and conflict resolution on cognitive and motor tasks in Parkinson's disease. Exp. Brain Res 212:3371–84 https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-011-2736-6
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  90. Owens MM, Yuan D, Hahn S, Albaugh M, Allgaier N et al. 2020. Investigation of psychiatric and neuropsychological correlates of default mode network and dorsal attention network anticorrelation in children. Cereb. Cortex 30:126083–96 https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhaa143
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  91. Palmiero M, Di Giacomo D, Passafiume D. 2014. Divergent thinking and age-related changes. Creat. Res. J. 26:4456–60 https://doi.org/10.1080/10400419.2014.961786
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  92. Pessoa L. 2010. Emotion and cognition and the amygdala: from “what is it?” to “what's to be done?. Neuropsychologia 48:123416–29 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.06.038
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  93. Plebanek DJ, Sloutsky VM. 2017. Costs of selective attention: when children notice what adults miss. Psychol. Sci. 28:6723–32 https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797617693005
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  94. Radel R, Davranche K, Fournier M, Dietrich A. 2015. The role of (dis)inhibition in creativity: decreased inhibition improves idea generation. Cognition 134:110–20 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2014.09.001
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  95. Reverberi C, Toraldo T, D'Agostini S, Skrap M 2005. Better without (lateral) frontal cortex? insight problems solved by frontal patients.. Brain 128:122882–90 https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awh577
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  96. Roenneberg T, Wirz-Justice A, Merrow M. 2003. Life between clocks: daily temporal patterns of human chronotypes. J. Biol. Rhythms 18:180–90 https://doi.org/10.1177/0748730402239679
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  97. Rowe G, Hasher L, Turcotte J. 2009. Age and synchrony effects in visuospatial working memory. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 62:101873–80 https://doi.org/10.1080/17470210902834852
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  98. Rowe G, Hasher L, Turcotte J. 2010. Interference, aging, and visuospatial working memory: the role of similarity. Neuropsychology 24:6804–7 https://doi.org/10.1037/a0020244
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  99. Rowe G, Hirsh JB, Anderson AK. 2007. Positive affect increases the breadth of attentional selection. PNAS 104:1383–88 https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0605198104
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  100. Rowe G, Valderrama S, Hasher L, Lenartowicz A. 2006. Attentional disregulation: a benefit for implicit memory. Psychol. Aging 21:4826–30 https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.21.4.826
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  101. Sandberg P, Rönnlund M, Nyberg L, Stigsdotter Neely A 2014. Executive process training in young and old adults. Neuropsychol. Dev. Cogn. 21:5577–605 https://doi.org/10.1080/13825585.2013.839777
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  102. Sandberg P, Stigsdotter Neely A 2016. Long-term effects of executive process training in young and old adults. Neuropsychol. Rehabil. 26:5–6761–82 https://doi.org/10.1080/09602011.2015.1108205
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  103. Schachar R, Tannock R, Marriott M, Logan G. 1995. Deficient inhibitory control in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 23:4411–37 https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01447206
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  104. Shi W, Ardelt M, Orwoll L. 2017. Wisdom and creativity as two routes to satisfaction in later life: a personal traits model. Innov. Aging 1:72 https://doi.org/10.1093/geroni/igx004.298
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  105. Simonton DK. 1991. Creative productivity through the adult years. Gener. J. Am. Soc. Aging 15:213–16
    [Google Scholar]
  106. Simonton DK. 1998. Career paths and creative lives: a theoretical perspective on late life potential. Creativity and Successful Aging: Theoretical and Empirical Approaches C Adams-Price 3–18 New York: Springer
    [Google Scholar]
  107. Smith KA, Huber DE, Vul E. 2013. Multiply-constrained semantic search in the Remote Associates Test. Cognition 128:164–75 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2013.03.001
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  108. Sprague BN, Freed SA, Webb CE, Phillips CB, Hyun J, Ross LA 2019. The impact of behavioral interventions on cognitive function in healthy older adults: a systematic review. Ageing Res. Rev. 52:32–52 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.arr.2019.04.002
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  109. Stern Y. 2002. What is cognitive reserve? Theory and research application of the reserve concept. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 8:3448–60 https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617702813248
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  110. Sternberg RJ. 2001. What is the common thread of creativity? Its dialectical relation to intelligence and wisdom. Am. Psychol. 56:4360–62 https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.56.4.360
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  111. Sternberg RJ, Lubart TI. 1995. Defying the Crowd: Cultivating Creativity in a Culture of Conformity New York: Free Press
  112. Sternberg RJ, Lubart TI 2001. Wisdom and creativity. Handbook of the Psychology of Aging JE Birren, KW Schaie 500–22 San Diego, CA: Academic
    [Google Scholar]
  113. Stevens WD, Hasher L, Chiew KS, Grady CL. 2008. A neural mechanism underlying memory failure in older adults. J. Neurosci. 28:4812820–24 https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2622-08.2008
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  114. Swirsky LT, Spaniol J. 2019. Cognitive and motivational selectivity in healthy aging. WIREs Cogn. Sci. 10:6e1512 https://doi.org/10.1002/wcs.1512
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  115. Torrance EP. 1974. The Torrance Tests of Creative Thinking—Norms, Technical Manual Research Edition—Figural Tests, Forms A and B Princeton, NJ: Personnel Press
  116. Torrance EP 2008. Torrance Tests of Creative Thinking—Norms, Technical Manual—Verbal Forms A and B Bensenville, IL: Scholastic Testing Service
  117. Truong L, Yang L 2014. Friend or foe? Decoding the facilitative and disruptive effects of emotion on working memory in younger and older adults. Front. Psychol. 5:94 https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00094
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  118. Vaitl D, Lipp O, Bauer U, Schüler G, Stark R et al. 2002. Latent inhibition and schizophrenia: Pavlovian conditioning of autonomic responses. Schizophr. Res. 55:1–2147–58 https://doi.org/10.1016/s0920-9964(01)00250-x
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  119. Verhaeghen P, De Meersman L. 1998. Aging and the Stroop effect: a meta-analysis. Psychol. Aging 13:1120–26 https://doi.org/10.1037//0882-7974.13.1.120
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  120. Wallach MA, Kogan N. 1965. Modes of Thinking in Young Children: A Study of the Creativity-Intelligence Distinction New York: Holt, Rinehart, & Winston
  121. Weeks JC, Biss RK, Murphy KJ, Hasher L. 2016. Face-name learning in older adults: a benefit of hyper-binding. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 23:51559–65 https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-016-1003-z
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  122. Weeks JC, Grady CL, Hasher L, Buchsbaum BR. 2020. Holding on to the past: older adults show lingering neural activation of no-longer-relevant items in working memory. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 32:101946–62 https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_01596
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  123. Weeks JC, Hasher L. 2014. The disruptive – and beneficial – effects of distraction on older adults’ cognitive performance. Front. Psychol. 5:133 https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00133
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  124. Weeks JC, Hasher L. 2017. Divided attention reduces resistance to distraction at encoding but not retrieval. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 24:41268–73 https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-016-1210-7
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  125. Weisberg RW. 1993. Creativity: Beyond the Myth of Genius New York: W.H. Freeman
  126. Westerhausen R, Kompus K, Hugdahl K. 2011. Impaired cognitive inhibition in schizophrenia: a meta-analysis of the Stroop interference effect. Schizophr. Res. 133:1172–81 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2011.08.025
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  127. White HA, Shah P. 2006. Uninhibited imaginations: creativity in adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Personal. Individ. Differ. 40:61121–31 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2005.11.007
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  128. Wilkinson AJ, Yang L 2012. Plasticity of inhibition in older adults: retest practice and transfer effects. Psychol. Aging 27:3606–15 https://doi.org/10.1037/a0025926
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  129. Wilkinson AJ, Yang L. 2016a. Inhibition plasticity in older adults: practice and transfer effects using a multiple task approach. Neural Plast. 2016:9696402 https://doi.org/10.1155/2016/9696402
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  130. Wilkinson AJ, Yang L 2016b. Long-term maintenance of inhibition training effects in older adults: 1- and 3-year follow-up. J. Gerontol. Ser. B 71:4622–29 https://doi.org/10.1093/geronb/gbu179
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  131. Wilkinson AJ, Yang L 2020. Long-term maintenance of multiple task inhibition practice and transfer effects in older adults: a 3.5-year follow-up. Psychol. Aging 35:5765–72 https://doi.org/10.1037/pag0000430
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  132. Williams BR, Ponesse JS, Schachar RJ, Logan GD, Tannock R. 1999. Development of inhibitory control across the life span. Dev. Psychol. 35:1205–13 https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.35.1.205
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  133. Wright L, Lipszyc J, Dupuis A, Thayapararajah SW, Schachar R. 2014. Response inhibition and psychopathology: a meta-analysis of go/no-go task performance. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 123:2429–39 https://doi.org/10.1037/a0036295
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  134. Wu CL, Huang SY, Chen PZ, Chen HC. 2020. A systematic review of creativity-related studies applying the remote associates test from 2000 to 2019. Front. Psychol. 11:573432 https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.573432
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  135. Yang L. 2011. Practice-oriented retest learning as the basic form of cognitive plasticity of the aging brain. J. Aging Res. 2011:407074 https://doi.org/10.4061/2011/407074
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  136. Yang L, Hasher L. 2007. The enhanced effects of pictorial distraction in older adults. J. Gerontol. Ser. B 62:4230–33 https://doi.org/10.1093/geronb/62.4.P230
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  137. Yang L, Ornstein TJ. 2011. The effect of emotion-focused orientation at retrieval on emotional memory in young and older adults. Memory 19:3305–13 https://doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2011.561803
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  138. Yang L, Truong L, Fuss S, Bislimovic S. 2012. The effects of ageing and divided attention on the self-reference effect in emotional memory: spontaneous or effortful mnemonic benefits?. Memory 20:6596–607 https://doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2012.690040
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  139. Yoon C, May CP, Hasher L 1999. Aging, circadian arousal patterns, and cognition. Cognition, Aging, and Self-Reports N Schwarz, DC Park, B Knaüper, S Sudman 117–43 New York: Psychology Press
    [Google Scholar]
  140. Zacks RT, Radvansky G, Hasher L. 1996. Studies of directed forgetting in older adults. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 22:1143 https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.22.1.143
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  141. Zedelius CM, Schooler JW. 2015. Mind wandering “ahas” versus mindful reasoning: alternative routes to creative solutions. Front. Psychol. 6:834 https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00834
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  142. Zhang W, Niu W. 2013. Creativity in the later life: factors associated with the creativity of the Chinese elderly. J. Creat. Behav. 47:160–76 https://doi.org/10.1002/jocb.23
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  143. Zhu DC, Zacks RT, Slade JM. 2010. Brain activation during interference resolution in young and older adults: an fMRI study. NeuroImage 50:2810–17 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.12.087
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  144. Zimmerman S, Hasher L, Goldstein D 2011. Cognitive aging: a positive perspective. The Paradoxical Brain N Kapur 130–50 Cambridge, UK: Cambridge Univ. Press https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511978098.009
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
/content/journals/10.1146/annurev-devpsych-121020-030705
Loading
/content/journals/10.1146/annurev-devpsych-121020-030705
Loading

Data & Media loading...

  • Article Type: Review Article
This is a required field
Please enter a valid email address
Approval was a Success
Invalid data
An Error Occurred
Approval was partially successful, following selected items could not be processed due to error