1932

Abstract

Humans vary in almost every dimension imaginable, and language is no exception. In this article, we review the past research that has focused on individual differences (IDs) in first language acquisition. We first consider how different theoretical traditions in language acquisition treat IDs, and we argue that a focus on IDs is important given its potential to reveal the developmental dynamics and architectural constraints of the linguistic system. We then review IDs research that has examined variation in children's linguistic input, early speech perception, and vocabulary and grammatical development. In each case, we observe systematic and meaningful variation, such that variation in one domain (e.g., early auditory and speech processing) has meaningful developmental consequences for development in higher-order domains (e.g., vocabulary). The research suggests a high degree of integration across the linguistic system, in which development across multiple linguistic domains is tightly coupled.

Loading

Article metrics loading...

/content/journals/10.1146/annurev-linguistics-011619-030326
2020-01-14
2024-12-10
Loading full text...

Full text loading...

/deliver/fulltext/linguistics/6/1/annurev-linguistics-011619-030326.html?itemId=/content/journals/10.1146/annurev-linguistics-011619-030326&mimeType=html&fmt=ahah

Literature Cited

  1. Acredolo L, Goodwyn S. 1988. Symbolic gesturing in normal infants. Child Dev 59:450–66
    [Google Scholar]
  2. Arciuli J, Conway C. 2018. The promise—and challenge—of statistical learning for elucidating atypical language development. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 27:492–500
    [Google Scholar]
  3. Bartlema A, Lee M, Wetzels R, Vanpaemel W 2014. A Bayesian hierarchical mixture approach to individual differences: case studies in selective attention and representation in category learning. J. Math. Psychol. 59:132–50
    [Google Scholar]
  4. Bates E, Benigni L, Bretherton I, Camioni L, Volterra V 1979. The Emergence of Symbols: Cognition and Communication in Infancy New York: Academic
    [Google Scholar]
  5. Bates E, Bretherton I, Snyder L 1988. From First Words to Grammar: Individual Differences and Dissociable Mechanisms Cambridge, UK: Cambridge Univ. Press
    [Google Scholar]
  6. Bates E, Dale P, Thal D 1995. Individual differences and their implications for theories of language development. Handbook of Child Language P Fletcher, B MacWhinney 96–151 Oxford, UK: Basil Blackwell
    [Google Scholar]
  7. Bates E, Goodman J. 1997. On the inseparability of grammar and the lexicon: evidence from acquisition, aphasia and real-time processing. Lang. Cogn. Proc. 12:507–86
    [Google Scholar]
  8. Bavin EL, Prior M, Reilly S, Bretherton L, Williams J et al. 2008. The Early Language in Victoria Study: predicting vocabulary at age one and two years from gesture and object use. J. Child Lang. 35:687–701
    [Google Scholar]
  9. Bellmont L, Marola FA. 1973. Birth order, family size, and intelligence. Science 182:1096–101
    [Google Scholar]
  10. Benasich AA, Gou Z, Choudhury N, Harris KD 2008. Early cognitive and language skills are linked to resting frontal gamma power across the first 3 years. Behav. Brain Res. 195:215–22
    [Google Scholar]
  11. Benasich AA, Tallal P. 2002. Infant discrimination of rapid auditory cues predicts later language impairment. Behav. Brain Res. 136:31–49
    [Google Scholar]
  12. Bergelson E, Casillas M, Soderstrom M, Seidl A, Warlaumont AS, Amatuni A 2019. What do North American babies hear? A large-scale cross-corpus analysis. Dev. Sci. 22:e12724
    [Google Scholar]
  13. Bornstein MH, Hahn CS, Putnick DL 2016. Long-term stability of core language skill in children with contrasting language skills. Dev. Psychol. 52:704–16
    [Google Scholar]
  14. Bornstein MH, Hahn CS, Putnick DL, Pearson RM 2018. Stability of core language skill from infancy to adolescence in typical and atypical development. Sci. Adv. 4:eaat7422
    [Google Scholar]
  15. Bornstein MH, Leach DB, Haynes OM 2004. Vocabulary competence in first- and secondborn siblings of the same chronological age. J. Child Lang. 31:855–73
    [Google Scholar]
  16. Borsboom D. 2006. The attack of the psychometricians. Psychometrika 71:425–40
    [Google Scholar]
  17. Borsboom D, Kievit RA, Cervone D, Hood SB 2009. The two disciplines of scientific psychology, or: the disunity of psychology as a working hypothesis. Dynamic Process Methodology in the Social and Developmental Sciences J Valsiner, PCM Molenaar, MCDP Lyra, N Chaudhary 67–97 New York: Springer Sci. Bus. Media
    [Google Scholar]
  18. Bouchard TJ Jr 2013. The Wilson effect: the increase in heritability of IQ with age. Twin Res. Hum. Genet. 16:923–30
    [Google Scholar]
  19. Boyle W, Lindell AK, Kidd E 2013. Investigating the role of verbal working memory in young children's sentence comprehension. Lang. Learn. 63:211–42
    [Google Scholar]
  20. Brichmann EI, Braeken J, Lyster SH 2018. Is there a direct relation between the development of vocabulary and grammar?. Dev. Sci. 22:e12709
    [Google Scholar]
  21. Brito NH, Fifer WP, Myers MM, Elliot AJ, Noble KG 2016. Associations among family socioeconomic status, EEG power at birth, and cognitive skills during infancy. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 19:144–51
    [Google Scholar]
  22. Bruner JS. 1983. Children's Talk: Learning to Use Language New York: Norton
    [Google Scholar]
  23. Carpenter M, Nagle K, Tomasello M 1998. Social cognition, joint attention, and communicative competence from 9 to 15 months of age. Monogr. Soc. Res. Child Dev. 63:1–143
    [Google Scholar]
  24. Carson SM, David AC, Leach JG 2005. Less is more: executive function and symbolic representation in preschool children. Psychol. Sci. 16:609–16
    [Google Scholar]
  25. Cartmill EA, Armstrong BF, Gleitman LR, Goldin-Meadow S, Medina TN, Trueswell JC 2013. Quality of early parent input predicts child vocabulary 3 years later. PNAS 110:11278–83
    [Google Scholar]
  26. Cattani A, Floccia C, Kidd E, Pettenati P, Onofrio D, Volterra V 2019. Gestures and words in naming: evidence from crosslinguistic and crosscultural comparison. Lang. Learn 69:709–46
    [Google Scholar]
  27. Chomsky N. 1980. On cognitive structures and their development: a reply to Piaget. Language and Learning: The Debate Between Jean Piaget and Noam Chomsky M Piattelli-Palmarini35–54 Cambridge, MA: Harvard Univ. Press
    [Google Scholar]
  28. Chomsky N. 1986. Knowledge of Language: Its Nature, Origin, and Use New York: Praeger
    [Google Scholar]
  29. Chonchaiya W, Tardif T, Mai X, Xu L, Li M et al. 2013. Developmental trends in auditory processing can provide early predictions of language acquisition in young infants. Dev. Sci. 16:159–72
    [Google Scholar]
  30. Choudhury N, Benasich AA. 2011. Maturation of auditory evoked potentials from 6 to 48 months: prediction to 3 and 4 year language and cognitive abilities. Clin. Neurophysiol. 122:320–38
    [Google Scholar]
  31. Colom R, Haier RJ, Head K, Alvarez-Linera J, Quiroga MA et al. 2009. Gray matter correlates of fluid, crystallized, and spatial intelligence: testing the P-FIT model. Intelligence 37:124–35
    [Google Scholar]
  32. Colonnesi C, Stams GJM, Koster I, Noom MJ 2010. The relation between pointing and language development: a meta-analysis. Dev. Rev. 30:352–66
    [Google Scholar]
  33. Conboy BT, Sommerville JA, Kuhl PK 2008. Cognitive control factors in speech perception at 11 months. Dev. Psychol. 44:1505–12
    [Google Scholar]
  34. Cooper RP, Aslin RN. 1990. Preference for infant-directed speech in the first month after birth. Child Dev 61:1584–95
    [Google Scholar]
  35. Crain S. 2012. The Emergence of Meaning Cambridge, UK: Cambridge Univ. Press
    [Google Scholar]
  36. Crain S, Thornton R. 1998. Investigations in Universal Grammar: A Guide to Experiments on the Acquisition of Syntax and Semantics Cambridge, MA: MIT Press
    [Google Scholar]
  37. Cristia A. 2018. Language input and outcome variation as a test of theory plausibility: The case of early phonological acquisition. OSF Prepr. https://doi.org/10.31219/osf.io/8pnhr
    [Crossref] [Google Scholar]
  38. Cristia A, Dupoux E, Gurven M, Stieglitz J 2017. Child-directed speech is infrequent in a forager-farmer population: a time allocation study. Child Dev 90:759–73
    [Google Scholar]
  39. Cristia A, Seidl A, Junge C, Soderstrom M, Hagoort P 2014. Predicting individual variation in language from infant speech perception measures. Child Dev 85:1330–45
    [Google Scholar]
  40. Cronbach LJ. 1957. The two disciplines of scientific psychology. Am. Psychol. 12:671–84
    [Google Scholar]
  41. Dąbrowska E. 2012. Different speakers, different grammars: individual differences in native language attainment. Linguist. Approaches Biling. 2:219–53
    [Google Scholar]
  42. Dale PS, Dionne G, Eley TC, Plomin R 2000. Lexical and grammatical development: a behavioural genetic perspective. J. Child Lang. 27:619–42
    [Google Scholar]
  43. Dale PS, Tosto MG, Hayiou-Thomas ME, Plomin R 2015. Why does parental language input style predict child language development? A twin study of gene–environment correlation. J. Commun. Disord. 57:106–17
    [Google Scholar]
  44. Darwin C. 1859. On the Origin of Species London: John Murray
    [Google Scholar]
  45. de Boysson-Bardies B, Vihman MM. 1991. Adaptation to language: evidence from babbling and first words in four languages. Language 67:297–319
    [Google Scholar]
  46. DePaolis RA, Vihman MM, Keren-Portnoy T 2011. Do production patterns influence the processing of speech in prelinguistic infants?. Infant. Behav. Dev. 34:590–601
    [Google Scholar]
  47. Dionne G, Dale PS, Boivin M, Plomin R 2003. Genetic evidence for bidirectional effects of early lexical and grammatical development. Child Dev 74:394–412
    [Google Scholar]
  48. Dixon JA, Marchman VA. 2007. Grammar and the lexicon: developmental ordering in language acquisition. Child Dev 78:190–212
    [Google Scholar]
  49. Dollaghan CA, Campbell TF, Paradise JL, Feldman HM, Janosky JE et al. 1999. Maternal education and measures of early speech and language. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 42:1432–43
    [Google Scholar]
  50. Engel de Abreu P, Gathercole SE 2012. Executive and phonological processes in second language acquisition. J. Educ. Psychol. 104:974–86
    [Google Scholar]
  51. Estes WK. 1956. The problem of inference from curves based on group data. Psychol. Bull. 53:134–40
    [Google Scholar]
  52. Fenson L, Dale P, Reznick JS, Bates E, Thal D, Pethick S 1994. Variability in early communicative development. Monogr. Soc. Res. Child Dev. 59:51–173
    [Google Scholar]
  53. Fenson L, Marchman VA, Thal DJ, Dale PS, Reznick JS, Bates E 2007. MacArthur-Bates Communicative Development Inventories: User's Guide and Technical Manual Baltimore, MD: Brookes, 2nd ed..
    [Google Scholar]
  54. Fernald A, Perfors A, Marchman VA 2006. Picking up speed in understanding: speech processing efficiency and vocabulary growth across the 2nd year. Dev. Psychol. 42:98–116
    [Google Scholar]
  55. Filová B, Ostatníková D, Celec P, Hodosy J 2013. The effect of testosterone on the formation of brain structures. Cell Tissues Organs 197:169–77
    [Google Scholar]
  56. Frank M, Braginsky M, Marchman V, Yurovsky D 2019. Variability and Consistency in Early Language Learning Cambridge, MA: MIT Press In press
    [Google Scholar]
  57. Friederici AD, Pannekamp A, Partsch C, Ulmen U, Oehler K et al. 2008. Sex hormone testosterone affects language organization in the infant brain. NeuroReport 19:283–86
    [Google Scholar]
  58. Friedman NP, Miyake A, Young SE, DeFries JC, Corley RP, Hewitt JK 2008. Individual differences in executive functions are almost entirely genetic in origin. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 137:201–25
    [Google Scholar]
  59. Furrow D, Nelson K, Benedict H 1979. Mothers’ speech to children and syntactic development: some simple relationships. J. Child Lang. 6:423–42
    [Google Scholar]
  60. Ganger J, Brent MR. 2004. Reexamining the vocabulary spurt. Dev. Psychol. 40:621–32
    [Google Scholar]
  61. Garcia-Sierra A, Ramirez-Esparza N, Kuhl PK 2016. Relationships between quantity of language input and brain responses in bilingual and monolingual infants. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 110:1–17
    [Google Scholar]
  62. Gathercole SE. 2006. Nonword repetition and word learning: the nature of the relationship. Appl. Psycholinguist. 27:513–43
    [Google Scholar]
  63. Gathercole SE, Willis CS, Emslie H, Baddeley AD 1992. Phonological memory and vocabulary development during the early school years: a longitudinal study. Dev. Psychol. 28:887–98
    [Google Scholar]
  64. Gilkerson J, Richards J, Warren S, Montgomery J, Greenwood C et al. 2017. Mapping the early language environment using all-day recordings and automated analysis. Am. J. Speech Lang. Pathol. 26:248–65
    [Google Scholar]
  65. Gleitman L. 1990. The structural sources of verb meaning. Lang. Acquis. 1:3–55
    [Google Scholar]
  66. Golinkoff RM, Can DD, Soderstrom M, Hirsh-Pasek K 2015. (Baby) Talk to me: the social context of infant-directed speech and its effects on early language acquisition. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 24:339–44
    [Google Scholar]
  67. Gooch D, Thompson P, Nash HM, Snowling MJ, Hulme C 2016. The development of executive function skills and language in the early preschool years. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 57:180–87
    [Google Scholar]
  68. Goswami U. 2018. A neural basis for phonological awareness? An oscillatory temporal sampling perspective. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 27:56–63
    [Google Scholar]
  69. Hale M, Reiss C. 2008. The Phonological Enterprise Oxford, UK: Oxford Univ. Press
    [Google Scholar]
  70. Hamrick P, Lum JAG, Ullman MT 2018. Child first language and adult second language are both tied to general-purpose learning systems. PNAS 115:1487–92
    [Google Scholar]
  71. Hanlon HW, Thatcher RW, Cline MJ 1999. Gender differences in the development of EEG coherence in normal children. Dev. Neuropsychol. 16:479–506
    [Google Scholar]
  72. Hart B, Risley TR. 1995. Meaningful Differences in the Everyday Experiences of Young Children Baltimore, MD: Brookes
    [Google Scholar]
  73. Hayiou-Thomas ME, Dale PS, Plomin R 2012. The etiology of variation in language skills changes with development: a longitudinal twin study of language from 2 to 12 years. Dev. Sci. 15:233–49
    [Google Scholar]
  74. Hayiou-Thomas ME, Kovas Y, Harlaar N, Plomin R, Bishop DVM, Dale PS 2006. Common aetiology for diverse language skills in 41/2-year-old twins. J. Child Lang. 33:339–68
    [Google Scholar]
  75. Hedge C, Powell G, Sumner P 2018. The reliability paradox: why robust cognitive tasks do not produce reliable individual differences. Behav. Res. Methods 50:1166–86
    [Google Scholar]
  76. Henrich J, Heine SJ, Norenzayan A 2010. The weirdest people in the world?. Behav. Brain Sci. 33:61–83
    [Google Scholar]
  77. Hirsh-Pasek K, Adamson LB, Bakeman R, Owen MT, Golinkoff RM et al. 2015. The contribution of early communication quality to low-income children's language success. Psychol. Sci. 26:1071–83
    [Google Scholar]
  78. Hoareau M, Yeung HH, Nazzi T 2019. Infants’ statistical word segmentation in an artificial language is linked to both parental speech input and reported speech production abilities. Dev. Sci. 22:e12803
    [Google Scholar]
  79. Hoff E. 2003. The specificity of environmental influence: Socioeconomic status affects early vocabulary development via maternal speech. Child Dev 74:1368–78
    [Google Scholar]
  80. Hoff E, Quinn JM, Giguere D 2018. What explains the correlation between growth in vocabulary and grammar? New evidence from latent change score analyses of simultaneous bilingual development. Dev. Sci. 21:e12536
    [Google Scholar]
  81. Hoff-Ginsberg E. 1998. The relation of birth order and socioeconomic status to children's language experience and language development. Appl. Psycholinguist. 19:603–29
    [Google Scholar]
  82. Huang YT, Leech K, Rowe ML 2017. Exploring socioeconomic differences in syntactic development through the lens of real-time processing. Cognition 159:61–75
    [Google Scholar]
  83. Hurtado N, Matchman VA, Fernald A 2008. Does input influence uptake? Links between maternal talk, processing speed and vocabulary size in Spanish-learning children. Dev. Sci. 11:6F31–39
    [Google Scholar]
  84. Huttenlocher J, Haight W, Bryk A, Selzer M, Lyons T 1991. Early vocabulary growth: relation to language input and gender. Dev. Psychol. 27:236–48
    [Google Scholar]
  85. Huttenlocher J, Vasilyeva M, Cyerman E, Levine S 2002. Language input and child syntax. Cogn. Psychol. 45:337–74
    [Google Scholar]
  86. Huttenlocher J, Vasilyeva M, Waterfall H, Vevea J, Hedges LV 2007. Varieties of caregiver speech. Dev. Psychol. 43:1062–83
    [Google Scholar]
  87. Huttenlocher J, Vasilyeva M, Waterfall H, Vevea J, Hedges LV 2010. Sources of variability in children's language growth. Cogn. Psychol. 61:343–65
    [Google Scholar]
  88. Iverson JM, Capirici O, Longobardi E, Caselli MC 1999. Gesturing in mother-child interactions. Cogn. Dev. 14:57–75
    [Google Scholar]
  89. Iverson JM, Goldin-Meadow S. 2005. Gesture paves the way for language development. Psychol. Sci. 16:367–71
    [Google Scholar]
  90. Jarrold C, Towse I. 2006. Individual differences in working memory. Neuroscience 139:39–50
    [Google Scholar]
  91. Jones G, Gobet F, Pine JM 2007. Linking working memory and long-term memory: a computational model of learning new words. Dev. Sci. 10:853–73
    [Google Scholar]
  92. Junge C, Cutler A. 2014. Early word recognition and later language skills. Brain Sci 4:532–59
    [Google Scholar]
  93. Jusczyk P. 1997. The Discovery of Spoken Language Cambridge, MA: MIT Press
    [Google Scholar]
  94. Justice LM, Jiang H, Purtell KM, Schmeer K, Boone K et al. 2019. Conditions of poverty, parent–child interactions, and toddlers’ early language skills in low-income families. Matern. Child Health J. 23:971–78
    [Google Scholar]
  95. Kaufman SB, DeYoung CG, Gray JR, Jimenez L, Brown J, Mackintosh N 2010. Implicit learning as an ability. Cognition 116:321–40
    [Google Scholar]
  96. Khanna MM, Boland JE. 2010. Children's use of language context in lexical ambiguity resolution. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 63:160–93
    [Google Scholar]
  97. Kidd E. 2013. The role of verbal working memory in children's sentence comprehension: a critical review. Top. Lang. Disord. 33:208–23
    [Google Scholar]
  98. Kidd E, Arciuli J. 2016. Individual differences in statistical learning predict children's comprehension of syntax. Child Dev 87:184–93
    [Google Scholar]
  99. Kidd E, Donnelly S, Christiansen MH 2018a. Individual differences in language acquisition and processing. Trends. Cogn. Sci. 22:154–69
    [Google Scholar]
  100. Kidd E, Junge C, Spokes T, Morrison L, Cutler A 2018b. Individual differences in infant speech segmentation: achieving the lexical shift. Infancy 23:770–94
    [Google Scholar]
  101. Kooijman VK, Hagoort P, Cutler A 2005. Electrophysiological evidence for prelinguistic infants' word recognition in continuous speech. Cogn. Brain Res. 24:109–16
    [Google Scholar]
  102. Kooijman VK, Junge C, Johnson EK, Hagoort P, Cutler A 2013. Predictive brain signals of linguistic development. Front. Psychol. 4:25
    [Google Scholar]
  103. Kuhl PK, Conboy BT, Coffey-Corina S, Padden D, Rivera-Gaxiola M, Nelson T 2008. Phonetic learning as a pathway to language: new data and native language magnet theory expanded (NLM-e). Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B 363:979–1000
    [Google Scholar]
  104. Kuhn LJ, Willoughby MT, Wilbourn MP, Vernon-Feagans L, Blair CB (Family Life Proj. Key Investig.). 2014. Early communicative gestures prospectively predict language development and executive function in early childhood. Child Dev 85:1898–914
    [Google Scholar]
  105. Lany J. 2014. Judging words by their covers and the company they keep: Probabilistic cues support word learning. Child Dev 85:1727–39
    [Google Scholar]
  106. Lany J. 2018. Lexical-processing efficiency leverages novel word learning in infants and toddlers. Dev. Sci. 21:e12569
    [Google Scholar]
  107. Lany J, Giglio M, Oswald M 2018a. Infants’ lexical processing efficiency is related to vocabulary size by one year of age. Infancy 23:342–66
    [Google Scholar]
  108. Lany J, Shoaib A, Thompson A, Grad Estes K 2018b. Infant statistical-learning ability is related to real-time language processing. J. Child Lang. 45:368–91
    [Google Scholar]
  109. Lieven EVM. 1994. Variation in crosslinguistic context. The Crosslinguistic Study of Language Acquisition, Vol. 5 DI Slobin 219–63 Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum
    [Google Scholar]
  110. Lieven EVM. 2009. Usage-based approaches to language development: Where do we go from here?. Lang. Cogn. 8:346–68
    [Google Scholar]
  111. Lieven EVM, Pine JM, Dresner-Barnes H 1992. Individual differences in early vocabulary development: redefining the referential-expressive distinction. J. Child Lang. 19:287–310
    [Google Scholar]
  112. Liu H, Kuhl PK, Tsao F 2003. An association between mothers’ speech clarity and infants’ speech discrimination skills. Dev. Sci. 6:3F1–10
    [Google Scholar]
  113. Mabbot DJ, Noseworthy M, Bouffet E, Laughlin S, Rockel C 2006. White matter growth as a mechanism of cognitive development in children. NeuroImage 15:936–46
    [Google Scholar]
  114. MacDonald MC, Christiansen MH. 2002. Reassessing working memory: a comment on Just & Carpenter 1992 and Waters & Caplan 1996. Psychol. Rev. 109:35–54
    [Google Scholar]
  115. MacWhinney B. 1998. Models of the emergence of language. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 49:199–227
    [Google Scholar]
  116. Marchman VA, Fernald A. 2008. Speed of word recognition and vocabulary knowledge in infancy predict cognitive and language outcomes in later childhood. Dev. Sci. 11:3F9–16
    [Google Scholar]
  117. Mayor J, Plunkett K. 2014. Shared understanding and idiosyncratic expression in early vocabularies. Dev. Sci. 17:412–23
    [Google Scholar]
  118. Melby-Lervåg M, Larvåg A, Lyster SH, Klem M, Hagtvet B, Hulme C 2012. Nonword-repetition ability does not appear to be a causal influence on children's vocabulary development. Psychol. Sci. 23:1092–98
    [Google Scholar]
  119. Meyer L. 2018. The neural oscillations of speech processing and language comprehension: state of the art and emerging mechanisms. Eur. J. Neurosci. 48:2609–21
    [Google Scholar]
  120. Molfese DL, Molfese VJ. 1985. Electrophysiological indices of auditory discrimination in newborn infants: the bases for predicting later language development?. Infant Behav. Dev. 8:197–211
    [Google Scholar]
  121. Molfese DL, Molfese VJ. 1997. Discrimination of language skills at five years of age using event-related potentials recorded at birth. Dev. Neuropsychol. 13:135–56
    [Google Scholar]
  122. Mollon JD, Bosten JM, Peterzell DH, Webster MA 2017. Individual differences in visual science: What can be learned and what is good experimental practice?. Vis. Res. 141:4–15
    [Google Scholar]
  123. Morgan PL, Farkas G, Hillmeier MM, Hamme CS, Maczuga S 2015. 24-month-old children with larger oral vocabularies display greater academic and behavioural functioning at kindergarten entry. Child Dev 86:1351–70
    [Google Scholar]
  124. Naigles L, Hoff-Ginsberg E. 1998. Why are some verbs learned before other verbs? Effects of input frequency and structure on children's early verb use. J. Child Lang. 25:95–120
    [Google Scholar]
  125. Nelson K. 1973. Structure and strategy in learning to talk. Monogr. Soc. Res. Child Dev. 38:1–21–135
    [Google Scholar]
  126. Newman R, Bernstein Ratner N, Jusczyk A, Jusczyk P, Dow K 2006. Infants' early ability to segment the conversational speech signal predicts later language development: a retrospective analysis. Dev. Psychol. 42:643–55
    [Google Scholar]
  127. Newman R, Rowe M, Bernstein Ratner N 2016. Input and uptake at 7 months predicts toddler vocabulary: the role of child-directed speech and infant processing skills in language development. J. Child Lang. 43:1158–73
    [Google Scholar]
  128. Nunez MD, Srinivasan R, Vandekerckhove J 2015. Individual differences in attention influence perceptual decision making. Front. Psychol. 8:18
    [Google Scholar]
  129. O'Grady W. 2005. Syntactic Carpentry: An Emergentist Approach to Syntax Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum
    [Google Scholar]
  130. Payne AC, Whitehurst GJ, Angell AL 1994. The role of home literacy environment in the development of language ability in preschool children from low-income families. Early Child Res. Q. 9:427–40
    [Google Scholar]
  131. Plunkett K, Marchman V. 1993. From rote learning to system building: acquiring verb morphology in children and connectionist nets. Cognition 48:21–69
    [Google Scholar]
  132. Puglisi ML, Hulme C, Hamilton LG, Snowling MJ 2017. The home literacy environment is a correlate, but perhaps not a cause, of children's language and literacy development. Sci. Stud. Read. 21:498–514
    [Google Scholar]
  133. Quinn S, Donnelly S, Kidd E 2018. The relationship between symbolic play and language acquisition: a meta-analytic review. Dev. Rev. 49:121–35
    [Google Scholar]
  134. Reardon PK, Seidlitz J, Vandekar S, Liu S, Patel R et al. 2018. Normative brain size variation and brain shape diversity in humans. Science 360:63941222–27
    [Google Scholar]
  135. Reber AS. 1993. Implicit Learning and Tacit Knowledge: An Essay on the Cognitive Unconscious New York: Oxford Univ. Press
    [Google Scholar]
  136. Romeo RR, Leonard JA, Robinson ST, West MR, Mackey AAP et al. 2018. Beyond the “30 million word gap”: Children's conversational exposure is associated with language-related brain function. Psychol. Sci. 29:700–10
    [Google Scholar]
  137. Rowe M. 2008. Child-directed speech: relation to socioeconomic status, knowledge of child development and child vocabulary skill. J. Child Lang. 35:185–205
    [Google Scholar]
  138. Rowe ML. 2012. A longitudinal investigation of the role of quantity and quality of child-directed speech in vocabulary development. Child Dev 83:1762–74
    [Google Scholar]
  139. Rowe ML, Goldin-Meadow S. 2009. Differences in early gesture explain SES disparities in child vocabulary size at school entry. Science 323:5916951–53
    [Google Scholar]
  140. Rowe ML, Leech KA. 2018. A parent intervention with a growth mindset approach improves children's early gesture and vocabulary development. Dev. Sci. 22:e12792
    [Google Scholar]
  141. Rowe ML, Özçaliskan S, Goldin-Meadow S 2008. Learning words by hand: gesture's role in predicting vocabulary development. First Lang 28:182–99
    [Google Scholar]
  142. Rowe ML, Raudenbush S, Goldin-Meadow S 2012. The pace of vocabulary growth helps predict later vocabulary skill. Child Dev 83:508–25
    [Google Scholar]
  143. Saffran JR, Aslin RN, Newport EL 1996. Statistical learning by 8-month-old infants. Science 274:1926–28
    [Google Scholar]
  144. Samuelsson S, Byrne B, Quain P, Wadsworth S, Corley R et al. 2005. Environmental and genetic influences on prereading skills in Australia, Scandinavia, and the United States. J. Educ. Psychol. 97:705–22
    [Google Scholar]
  145. Schieffelin BB, Ochs E. 1986. Language socialization. Annu. Rev. Anthropol. 15:163–91
    [Google Scholar]
  146. Shimpi PM, Fedewa A, Hans S 2012. Social and linguistic input in low-income African American mother–child dyads from 1 month through 2 years: relations to vocabulary development. Appl. Psycholinguist. 33:781–98
    [Google Scholar]
  147. Shneidman LA, Goldin-Meadow S. 2012. Language input and acquisition in a Mayan village: How important is directed speech?. Dev. Sci. 15:659–73
    [Google Scholar]
  148. Singh L, Reznick JS, Xuehua L 2012. Infant word segmentation and childhood vocabulary development: a longitudinal analysis. Dev. Sci. 15:482–95
    [Google Scholar]
  149. Sperry DE, Sperry LL, Miller PJ 2019. Reexamining the verbal environments of children from different socioeconomic backgrounds. Child Dev 90:1303–18
    [Google Scholar]
  150. Stokes SF, Moran C, George A 2013. Nonword repetition and vocabulary use in toddlers. Top. Lang. Disord. 33:224–37
    [Google Scholar]
  151. Stromswold K. 2001. The heritability of language: a review and metaanalysis of twin, adoption and linkage studies. Language 77:647–723
    [Google Scholar]
  152. Szewczyk JM, Marecka M, Chiat S, Wodniecka Z 2018. Nonword repetition depends on the frequency of sublexical representations at different grain sizes: evidence from a multi-factorial analysis. Cognition 179:23–36
    [Google Scholar]
  153. Tallal P. 2004. Improving language and literacy is a matter of time. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 5:721–28
    [Google Scholar]
  154. Tamis-LeMonda CS, Kuchirko Y, Song L 2014. Why is infant language learning facilitated by parental responsiveness?. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 23:121–26
    [Google Scholar]
  155. Thierry G, Vihman MM, Roberts MV 2003. Familiar words capture attention in 11-month-olds in less than 250 ms. NeuroReport 14:2307–10
    [Google Scholar]
  156. Tomasello M, Carpenter M. 2007. Shared intentionality. Dev. Sci. 10:121–25
    [Google Scholar]
  157. Tsao FM, Liu HM, Kuhl PK 2004. Speech perception in infancy predicts language development in the second year of life: a longitudinal study. Child Dev 75:1067–84
    [Google Scholar]
  158. Underwood BJ. 1975. Individual differences as a crucible in theory construction. Am. Psychol. 30:128–34
    [Google Scholar]
  159. Vasilyeva M, Waterfall H, Huttenlocher J 2008. Emergence of syntax: commonalities and differences across children. Dev. Sci. 11:84–97
    [Google Scholar]
  160. Voorspoels W, Rutten I, Bartlema A, Tuerlinckx F, Vanpaemel W 2018. Sensitivity to the prototype in children with high-functioning autism spectrum disorder: an example of Bayesian cognitive psychometrics. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 25:271–85
    [Google Scholar]
  161. Weisleder A, Fernald A. 2013. Talking to children matters: Early language experience strengthens processing and builds vocabulary. Psychol. Sci. 24:2143–52
    [Google Scholar]
  162. Westfall J, Yarkoni T. 2016. Statistically controlling for confounding constructs is harder than you think. PLOS ONE 11:e0152719
    [Google Scholar]
  163. Wichman AL, Rogers JL, MacCallum RC 2006. A multilevel approach to the relationship between birth order and intelligence. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 32:117–27
    [Google Scholar]
  164. Wilke M, Holland SK. 2003. Variability of gray and white matter during normal development: a voxel-based MRI analysis. NeuroReport 27:1887–90
    [Google Scholar]
  165. Willoughby MT, Blair CB. 2016. Measuring executive function in early childhood: a case for formative measurement. Psychol. Assess. 28:319–30
    [Google Scholar]
  166. Woodard K, Pozzan L, Trueswell JC 2016. Taking your own path: individual differences in executive function and language processing skills in child learners. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 141:187–209
    [Google Scholar]
  167. Wundt W. 1904. 1874. Principles of Physiological Psychology, Vol. 1 trans. EB Titchener New York: MacMillan, 5th ed..
    [Google Scholar]
  168. Yang C. 2016. The Price of Linguistic Productivity Cambridge, MA: MIT Press
    [Google Scholar]
  169. Yang C, Crain S, Berwick R, Chomsky N, Bolhuis J 2017. The growth of language: Universal Grammar, experience, and the principle of computation. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 81:103–19
    [Google Scholar]
/content/journals/10.1146/annurev-linguistics-011619-030326
Loading
  • Article Type: Review Article
This is a required field
Please enter a valid email address
Approval was a Success
Invalid data
An Error Occurred
Approval was partially successful, following selected items could not be processed due to error