1932

Abstract

Crosslinguistic research on domains including kinship, color, folk biology, number, and spatial relations has documented the different ways in which languages carve up the world into named categories. Although word meanings vary widely across languages, unrelated languages often have words with similar or identical meanings, and many logically possible meanings are never observed. We review research suggesting that this pattern of constrained variation is explained in part by the need for words to support efficient communication. This research includes several recent studies that have formalized efficient communication in computational terms and a larger set of studies, both classic and recent, that do not explicitly appeal to efficient communication but are nevertheless consistent with this notion. The efficient communication framework has implications for the relationship between language and culture and for theories of language change, and we draw out some of these connections.

Loading

Article metrics loading...

/content/journals/10.1146/annurev-linguistics-011817-045406
2018-01-14
2024-10-07
Loading full text...

Full text loading...

/deliver/fulltext/linguistics/4/1/annurev-linguistics-011817-045406.html?itemId=/content/journals/10.1146/annurev-linguistics-011817-045406&mimeType=html&fmt=ahah

Literature Cited

  1. Aylett M, Turk A. 2004. The smooth signal redundancy hypothesis: a functional explanation for relationships between redundancy, prosodic prominence, and duration in spontaneous speech. Lang. Speech 47:31–56 [Google Scholar]
  2. Bach E, Chao W. 2009. On semantic universals and typology. Language Universals MH Christiansen, C Collins, S Edelman 1–33 Oxford, UK: Oxford Scholarship Online [Google Scholar]
  3. Baddeley R, Attewell D. 2009. The relationship between language and the environment: information theory shows why we have only three lightness terms. Psychol. Sci. 20:1100–7 [Google Scholar]
  4. Balée W. 1999. Mode of production and ethnobotanical vocabulary: a controlled comparison of Guajá and Ka'apor. Ethnoecology: Knowledge, Resources and Rights TL Gragson, BG Blount 24–40 Athens: Univ. Ga. Press [Google Scholar]
  5. Baronchelli A, Gong T, Puglisi A, Loreto V. 2010. Modeling the emergence of universality in color naming patterns. PNAS 107:2403–7 [Google Scholar]
  6. Berlin B. 1992. Ethnobiological Classification: Principles of Categorization of Plants and Animals in Traditional Societies Princeton, NJ: Princeton Univ. Press [Google Scholar]
  7. Berlin B, Kay P. 1969. Basic Color Terms: Their Universality and Evolution Berkeley: Univ. Calif. Press [Google Scholar]
  8. Biggam CP. 2012. The Semantics of Colour: A Historical Approach Cambridge, UK: Cambridge Univ. Press [Google Scholar]
  9. Boas F. 1911. Introduction. Handbook of American Indian Languages 11–83 Washington, DC: Gov. Print. Off. [Google Scholar]
  10. Boster JS. 1986. Can individuals recapitulate the evolutionary development of color lexicons. ? Ethnology 25:61–74 [Google Scholar]
  11. Boster JS. 2005. Categories and cognitive anthropology. Handbook of Categorization in Cognitive Science H Cohen, C Lefebvre 91–118 Amsterdam: Elsevier [Google Scholar]
  12. Bowerman M. 1996. Learning how to structure space for language: a cross-linguistic perspective. Language and Space P Bloom, MA Peterson, L Nadel, MF Garrett 385–436 Cambridge, MA: MIT Press [Google Scholar]
  13. Bowerman M, Pederson E. 1992. Topological relations picture series. Space Stimuli Kit 1.2: November 1992 SC Levinson 51 Nijmegen, Neth.: Max Planck Inst. Psycholinguist. [Google Scholar]
  14. Bowerman M, Pederson E. 1993. Cross-linguistic studies of spatial semantic organization. Annual Report of the Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics: 199253–56 Nijmegen, Neth.: Max Planck Inst. Psycholinguist. [Google Scholar]
  15. Brown CH. 1984. Language and Living Things: Uniformities in Folk Classification and Naming New Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers Univ. Press [Google Scholar]
  16. Brown CH. 1985. Mode of subsistence and folk biological taxonomy. Curr. Anthropol. 26:43–53 [Google Scholar]
  17. Brown CH. 2001. Lexical typology from an anthropological point of view. Language Typology and Language Universals: An International Handbook M Haspelmath, E König, W Oesterreicher, W Raible 21178–90 Berlin: de Gruyter [Google Scholar]
  18. Brown RW, Lenneberg EH. 1954. A study in language and cognition. J. Abnorm. Soc. Psychol. 49:454–62 [Google Scholar]
  19. Brugman CM. 1988. The Story of “Over”: Polysemy, Semantics, and the Structure of the Lexicon New York: Garland [Google Scholar]
  20. Burenhult N, Levinson SC. 2008. Language and landscape: a cross-linguistic perspective. Lang. Sci. 30:135–50 [Google Scholar]
  21. Bybee J, Perkins R, Pagliuca W. 1994. The Evolution of Grammar: Tense, Aspect, and Modality in the Languages of the World Chicago: Univ. Chicago Press [Google Scholar]
  22. Carstensen A. 2011. Universals and variation in spatial language and cognition Undergrad. hon. thesis Dep. Linguist. Univ. Calif. Berkeley: [Google Scholar]
  23. Carstensen A, Xu J, Smith C, Regier T. 2015. Language evolution in the lab tends toward informative communication. Proceedings of the 37th Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society DC Noelle, R Dale, AS Warlaumont, J Yoshimi, T Matlock, et al. 303–8 Cambridge, MA: Cogn. Sci. Soc. [Google Scholar]
  24. Chater N, Vitányi P. 2003. Simplicity: a unifying principle in cognitive science?. Trends Cogn. Sci. 7:19–22 [Google Scholar]
  25. Comrie B. 2013. Numeral bases. The World Atlas of Language Structures Online MS Dryer, M Haspelmath. Leipzig Ger.: Max Planck Inst. Evol. Anthropol http://wals.info/chapter/131 [Google Scholar]
  26. Croft W. 2003. Typology and Universals Cambridge, UK: Cambridge Univ. Press, 2nd ed.. [Google Scholar]
  27. Croft W, Poole KT. 2008. Inferring universals from grammatical variation: multidimensional scaling for typological analysis. Theor. Linguist. 34:1–37 [Google Scholar]
  28. Cysouw M. 2001. Review of Martin Haspelmath, Indefinite Pronouns (1997). J. Linguist. 37:607–12 [Google Scholar]
  29. Cysouw M, Haspelmath M, Malchukov A. 2010. Introduction to the special issue “Semantic Maps: Methods and Applications.”. Linguist. Discov 8:1–3 [Google Scholar]
  30. Dehaene S. 2011. The Number Sense: How the Mind Creates Mathematics Oxford, UK: Oxford Univ. Press [Google Scholar]
  31. Dole GE. 1972. Developmental sequences of kinship patterns. Kinship Studies in the Morgan Centennial Year P Reining 134–66 Washington, DC: Anthropol. Soc. Wash. [Google Scholar]
  32. Dowman M. 2007. Explaining color term typology with an evolutionary model. Cogn. Sci. 31:99–132 [Google Scholar]
  33. Enfield NJ. 2014. The Utility of Meaning: What Words Mean and Why Oxford, UK: Oxford Univ. Press [Google Scholar]
  34. Enfield NJ, Majid A, van Staden M. 2006. Cross-linguistic categorisation of the body: introduction. Lang. Sci. 28:137–47 [Google Scholar]
  35. Evans N. 2011. Semantic typology. The Oxford Handbook of Linguistic Typology JJ Song 504–33 Oxford, UK: Oxford Univ. Press [Google Scholar]
  36. Fedzechkina M, Jaeger TF, Newport EL. 2012. Language learners restructure their input to facilitate efficient communication. PNAS 109:17897–902 [Google Scholar]
  37. Ferrer i Cancho R, Solé RV. 2003. Least effort and the origins of scaling in human language. PNAS 100:788–91 [Google Scholar]
  38. Frank MC. 2017. What's the relationship between language and thought? The Optimal Semantic Expressivity Hypothesis. Babies Learning Language Blog July 6. http://babieslearninglanguage.blogspot.com/2017/07/whats-relationship-between-language-and.html [Google Scholar]
  39. Frank MC, Everett DL, Fedorenko E, Gibson E. 2008. Number as a cognitive technology: evidence from Pirahã language and cognition. Cognition 108:819–24 [Google Scholar]
  40. Frank MC, Goodman ND. 2012. Predicting pragmatic reasoning in language games. Science 336:998 [Google Scholar]
  41. Gärdenfors P. 2014. The Geometry of Meaning: Semantics Based on Conceptual Spaces Cambridge, MA: MIT Press [Google Scholar]
  42. Gordon P. 2004. Numerical cognition without words: evidence from Amazonia. Science 306:496–99 [Google Scholar]
  43. Greenberg JH. 1978. Generalizations about numeral systems. Universals of Human Language 3 Word Structure JH Greenberg 249–95 Stanford, CA: Stanford Univ. Press [Google Scholar]
  44. Greenberg JH. 1990. Universals of kinship terminology: their nature and the problem of their explanation. On Language: Selected Writings of Joseph H. Greenberg K Denning, S Kemmer 310–27 Stanford, CA: Stanford Univ. Press [Google Scholar]
  45. Greenberg JH. 2005. Universals of kinship terminology. Language Universals: With Special Reference to Feature Hierarchies72–87 Berlin: de Gruyter [Google Scholar]
  46. Guillon M, Mace R. 2016. A phylogenetic comparative study of Bantu kinship terminology finds limited support for its co-evolution with social organisation. PLOS ONE 11:e0155170 [Google Scholar]
  47. Haiman J. 1985. Natural Syntax: Iconicity and Erosion Cambridge, UK: Cambridge Univ. Press [Google Scholar]
  48. Halff HM, Ortony A, Anderson RC. 1976. A context-sensitive representation of word meanings. Mem. Cogn. 4:378–83 [Google Scholar]
  49. Haspelmath M. 1997. Indefinite Pronouns London: Clarendon [Google Scholar]
  50. Haspelmath M. 1999. Optimality and diachronic adaptation. Z. Sprachwiss. 18:180–205 [Google Scholar]
  51. Haspelmath M. 2003. The geometry of grammatical meaning: semantic maps and cross-linguistic comparison. The New Psychology of Language M Tomasello 2211–42 Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum [Google Scholar]
  52. Hawkins JA. 2004. Efficiency and Complexity in Grammars Oxford, UK: Oxford Univ. Press [Google Scholar]
  53. Haynie HJ, Bowern C. 2016. Phylogenetic approach to the evolution of color term systems. PNAS 113:13666–71 [Google Scholar]
  54. Heine B, Kuteva T. 2002. World Lexicon of Grammaticalization Cambridge, UK: Cambridge Univ. Press [Google Scholar]
  55. Heit E. 1992. Categorization using chains of examples. Cogn. Psychol. 24:341–80 [Google Scholar]
  56. Hopper PJ, Traugott EC. 2003. Grammaticalization Cambridge, UK: Cambridge Univ. Press, 2nd ed.. [Google Scholar]
  57. Hunn E. 1982. The utilitarian factor in folk biological classification. Am. Anthropol. 84:830–47 [Google Scholar]
  58. Hymes D. 1964. Language in Culture and Society: A Reader in Linguistics and Anthropology New York: Harper & Row [Google Scholar]
  59. Jaeger TF. 2010. Redundancy and reduction: Speakers manage syntactic information density. Cogn. Psychol. 61:23–62 [Google Scholar]
  60. Jameson KA. 2005. Why GRUE? An interpoint-distance model analysis of composite color categories. Cross-Cult. Res. 39:159–204 [Google Scholar]
  61. Jameson KA, D'Andrade RG. 1997. It's not really red, green, yellow, blue. Color Categories in Thought and Language CL Hardin, L Maffi 295–319 Cambridge, UK: Cambridge Univ. Press [Google Scholar]
  62. Jones D. 2010. Human kinship, from conceptual structure to grammar. Behav. Brain Sci. 33:367–416 [Google Scholar]
  63. Jones D. 2015. Grammars of kinship and color: cognitive universals and optimal communication. Kinship, Language, and Prehistory: Per Hage and the Renaissance in Kinship Studies D Jones, B Milicic 196–211 Salt Lake City: Univ. Utah Press [Google Scholar]
  64. Jordan FM. 2011. A phylogenetic analysis of the evolution of Austronesian sibling terminologies. Hum. Biol. 83:297–321 [Google Scholar]
  65. Jordan FM. 2013. Comparative phylogenetic methods and the study of pattern and process in kinship. Kinship Systems: Change and Reconstruction P McConvell, I Keen, R Hendery 43–58 Salt Lake City: Univ. Utah Press [Google Scholar]
  66. Jurafsky D. 1996. Universal tendencies in the semantics of the diminutive. Language 72:533–78 [Google Scholar]
  67. Kay P, Berlin B, Maffi L, Merrifield WR, Cook R. 2009. The World Color Survey Stanford, CA: Cent. Study Lang. Inf. [Google Scholar]
  68. Kay P, Kempton W. 1984. What is the Sapir–Whorf hypothesis?. Am. Anthropol. 86:65–79 [Google Scholar]
  69. Kay P, McDaniel CK. 1978. The linguistic significance of the meanings of basic color terms. Language 54:610–46 [Google Scholar]
  70. Kay P, Regier T. 2003. Resolving the question of color naming universals. PNAS 100:9085–89 [Google Scholar]
  71. Kemp C, Regier T. 2012. Kinship categories across languages reflect general communicative principles. Science 336:1049–54 [Google Scholar]
  72. Khetarpal N, Neveu G, Majid A, Michael L, Regier T. 2013. Spatial terms across languages support near-optimal communication: evidence from Peruvian Amazonia, and computational analyses. Proceedings of the 35th Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society M Knauff, M Pauen, N Sebanz, I Wachsmuth 764–69 Cambridge, MA: Cogn. Sci. Soc. [Google Scholar]
  73. Kirby KR, Gray RD, Greenhill SJ, Jordan FM, Gomes-Ng S. et al. 2016. D-PLACE: a global database of cultural, linguistic and environmental diversity. PLOS ONE 11:e0158391 [Google Scholar]
  74. Kirby S, Griffiths T, Smith K. 2014. Iterated learning and the evolution of language. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 28:108–14 [Google Scholar]
  75. Kirby S, Tamariz M, Cornish H, Smith K. 2015. Compression and communication in the cultural evolution of linguistic structure. Cognition 141:87–102 [Google Scholar]
  76. Koch P. 2001. Lexical typology from a cognitive and linguistic point of view. Language Typology and Language Universals: An International Handbook M Haspelmath, E König, W Oesterreicher, W Raible 21142–78 Berlin: de Gruyter [Google Scholar]
  77. Komarova NL, Jameson KA, Narens L. 2007. Evolutionary models of color categorization based on discrimination. J. Math. Psychol. 51:359–82 [Google Scholar]
  78. Koptjevskaja-Tamm M. 2008. Approaching lexical typology. From Polysemy to Semantic Change: Towards a Typology of Lexical Semantic Associations M Vanhove 3–52 Amsterdam: Benjamins [Google Scholar]
  79. Koptjevskaja-Tamm M, Vanhove M, Koch P. 2007. Typological approaches to lexical semantics. Linguist. Typol. 11:159–85 [Google Scholar]
  80. Kroeber AL. 1909. Classificatory systems of relationship. J. R. Anthropol. Inst. G. B. Irel. 39:77–84 [Google Scholar]
  81. Kroeber AL. 1917. California Kinship Systems Berkeley: Univ. Calif. Press [Google Scholar]
  82. Kronenfeld DB. 1974. Sibling typology: beyond Nerlove and Romney. Am. Ethnol. 1:489–506 [Google Scholar]
  83. Krupnik I, Müller-Wille L. 2010. Franz Boas and Inuktitut terminology for ice and snow: from the emergence of the field to the “Great Eskimo Vocabulary Hoax.”. SIKU: Knowing Our Ice I Krupnik, C Aporta, S Gearheard, GJ Laidler, L Kielsen Holm 377–400 Berlin: Springer [Google Scholar]
  84. Kuschel R, Monberg T. 1974. ‘We don't talk much about colour here’: a study of colour semantics on Bellona Island. Man 9:213–42 [Google Scholar]
  85. Lakoff G. 1987. Women, Fire, and Dangerous Things: What Categories Reveal About the Mind Chicago: Univ. Chicago Press [Google Scholar]
  86. Lévi-Strauss C. 1963. Structural Anthropology New York: Basic Books [Google Scholar]
  87. Levinson SC. 2012. Kinship and human thought. Science 336:988–89 [Google Scholar]
  88. Levinson SC, Meira S. 2003. ‘Natural concepts’ in the spatial topologial domain—adpositional meanings in crosslinguistic perspective: an exercise in semantic typology. Language 79:485–516 [Google Scholar]
  89. Levy R. 2008. Expectation-based syntactic comprehension. Cognition 106:1126–77 [Google Scholar]
  90. Lindsey DT, Brown AM. 2006. Universality of color names. PNAS 103:16608–13 [Google Scholar]
  91. Lindsey DT, Brown AM. 2009. World Color Survey color naming reveals universal motifs and their within-language diversity. PNAS 106:19785–90 [Google Scholar]
  92. List JM, Cysouw M, Forkel R. 2016. Concepticon Jena, Ger.: Max Planck Inst. Sci. Hum. Hist. [Google Scholar]
  93. List JM, Terhalle A, Urban M. 2013. Using network approaches to enhance the analysis of cross-linguistic polysemies. Proceedings of the 10th International Conference on Computational Semantics—Short Papers315–20 Stroudsburg, PA: Assoc. Comput. Linguist. [Google Scholar]
  94. Lucy JA. 1997. The linguistics of “color”. Color Categories in Thought and Language CL Hardin, L Maffi 320–46 Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press [Google Scholar]
  95. Lucy JA. 2004. Language, culture, and mind in comparative perspective. Language, Culture, and Mind M Achard, S Kemmer 1–22 Stanford, CA: Cent. Study Lang. Inf. [Google Scholar]
  96. Majid A. 2015. Comparing lexicons cross-linguistically. The Oxford Handbook of the Word JR Taylor 364–79 Oxford, UK: Oxford Univ. Press [Google Scholar]
  97. Majid A, Boster JS, Bowerman M. 2008. The cross-linguistic categorization of everyday events: a study of cutting and breaking. Cognition 109:235–50 [Google Scholar]
  98. Majid A, Bowerman M, Kita S, Haun DB, Levinson SC. 2004. Can language restructure cognition? The case for space. Trends Cogn. Sci. 8:108–14 [Google Scholar]
  99. Malt BC. 1995. Category coherence in cross-cultural perspective. Cogn. Psychol. 29:85–148 [Google Scholar]
  100. Malt BC, Majid A. 2013. How thought is mapped into words. Wiley Interdiscip. Rev. Cogn. Sci. 4:483–597 [Google Scholar]
  101. Malt BC, Sloman SA, Gennari S, Shi M, Wang Y. 1999. Knowing versus naming: similarity and the linguistic categorization of artifacts. J. Mem. Lang. 40:230–62 [Google Scholar]
  102. Mark DM, Turk AG, Burenhult N, Stea D. 2011. Landscape in Language: Transdisciplinary Perspectives Amsterdam: Benjamins [Google Scholar]
  103. Martin L. 1986. “Eskimo words for snow”: a case study in the genesis and decay of an anthropological example. Am. Anthropol. 88:418–23 [Google Scholar]
  104. Michael L, Beier C, Farmer S, Neely K, Skilton A. et al. 2013. Diccionario bilingüe máíjikì-castellano y castellano-máíji Máíjikì Project. http://linguistics.berkeley.edu/∼levmichael/pubs/mai_ore_diccionario_oct_2013.pdf [Google Scholar]
  105. Murdock GP. 1949. Social Structure New York: Macmillan [Google Scholar]
  106. Murdock GP. 1970. Kin term patterns and their distribution. Ethnology 9:165–208 [Google Scholar]
  107. Nerlove S, Romney AK. 1967. Sibling terminology and cross-sex behavior. Am. Anthropol. 69:179–87 [Google Scholar]
  108. Neveu G. 2013. Spatial relations in Máíji Undergrad. hon. thesis Dep. Linguist., Univ. Calif. Berkeley: [Google Scholar]
  109. Piantadosi ST, Tily H, Gibson E. 2011. Word lengths are optimized for efficient communication. PNAS 108:3526–29 [Google Scholar]
  110. Piantadosi ST, Tily H, Gibson E. 2012. The communicative function of ambiguity in language. Cognition 122:280–91 [Google Scholar]
  111. Pica P, Lemer C, Izard V, Dehaene S. 2004. Exact and approximate arithmetic in an Amazonian indigene group. Science 306:499–503 [Google Scholar]
  112. Puglisi A, Baronchelli A, Loreto V. 2008. Cultural route to the emergence of linguistic categories. PNAS 105:7936–40 [Google Scholar]
  113. Pullum G. 1991. The great Eskimo vocabulary hoax. The Great Eskimo Vocabulary Hoax and Other Irreverent Essays on the Study of Language159–71 Chicago: Univ. Chicago Press [Google Scholar]
  114. Regier T, Carstensen A, Kemp C. 2016. Languages support efficient communication about the environment: words for snow revisited. PLOS ONE 11:e0151138 [Google Scholar]
  115. Read DW. 2007. Kinship theory: a paradigm shift. Ethnology 46:329–64 [Google Scholar]
  116. Regier T, Kemp C, Kay P. 2015. Word meanings across languages support efficient communication. The Handbook of Language Emergence B MacWhinney, W O'Grady 237–63 Oxford, UK: Wiley-Blackwell [Google Scholar]
  117. Rivers WHR. 1914. Kinship and Social Organisation London: Constable & Co. [Google Scholar]
  118. Roberson D, Davidoff J, Davies IR, Shapiro LR. 2005. Color categories: evidence for the cultural relativity hypothesis. Cogn. Psychol. 50:378–411 [Google Scholar]
  119. Roberson D, Davies I, Davidoff J. 2000. Color categories are not universal: replications and new evidence from a stone-age culture. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 129:369–98 [Google Scholar]
  120. Rosch E. 1999. Principles of categorization. Concepts: Core Readings E Margolis, S Laurence 189–206 Cambridge, MA: MIT Press [Google Scholar]
  121. Sapir E. 1912. Language and environment. Am. Anthropol. 14:226–42 [Google Scholar]
  122. Sapir E. 1929. The status of linguistics as a science. Language 5:207–14 [Google Scholar]
  123. Service ER. 1960. Kinship terminology and evolution. Am. Anthropol. 62:747–63 [Google Scholar]
  124. Service ER. 1985. A Century of Controversy: Ethnological Issues from 1860 to 1960 San Diego: Academic [Google Scholar]
  125. Steels L, Belpaeme T. 2005. Coordinating perceptually grounded categories through language: a case study for colour. Behav. Brain Sci. 28:469–89 [Google Scholar]
  126. Stickles E. 2014. The relation of color naming and the environment. Proceedings of the 36th Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society P Bello, M Guarini, M McShane, B Scassellati 3408 Cambridge, MA: Cogn. Sci. Soc. [Google Scholar]
  127. Talmy L. 2000. How language structures space. Toward a Cognitive Semantics L Talmy 1177–254 Cambridge, MA: MIT Press [Google Scholar]
  128. Valdés-Pérez RE, Pericliev V. 1999. Computer enumeration of significant implicational universals of kinship terminology. Cross-Cult. Res. 33:162–74 [Google Scholar]
  129. van der Auwera J, Plungian V. 1998. Modality's semantic map. Linguist. Typol. 2:79–124 [Google Scholar]
  130. Voeks RA. 2007. Penan ethnobotany: subsistence strategy and breadth of knowledge. Beyond the Green Myth: Borneo's Hunter-Gatherers in the Twenty-First Century PG Sercombe, B Sellato 262–88 Honolulu: Univ. Hawai‘i Press [Google Scholar]
  131. von der Gabelentz G. 1901. Die Sprachwissenschaft: Ihre Aufgaben, Methoden, und bisherige Ergebnisse Berlin: Weigel [Google Scholar]
  132. Whorf BL. 1956. Science and linguistics. Language, Thought, and Reality: Selected Writings of Benjamin Lee Whorf JB Carroll 207–19 Cambridge, MA: MIT Press [Google Scholar]
  133. Wierzbicka A. 1972. Semantic Primitives Frankfurt: Athenäum [Google Scholar]
  134. Wierzbicka A. 1980. Lingua Mentalis: The Semantics of Natural Language San Diego: Academic [Google Scholar]
  135. Wierzbicka A. 1992. Semantics, Culture and Cognition: Universal Human Concepts in Culture-Specific Configurations Oxford, UK: Oxford Univ. Press [Google Scholar]
  136. Wilson D, Sperber D. 2004. Relevance theory. The Handbook of Pragmatics LR Horn, G Ward 607–32 Oxford, UK: Blackwell [Google Scholar]
  137. Winawer J, Witthoft N, Frank MC, Wu L, Wade AR, Boroditsky L. 2007. Russian blues reveal effects of language on color discrimination. PNAS 104:7780–85 [Google Scholar]
  138. Wisniewski EJ, Murphy GL. 1989. Superordinate and basic category names in discourse: a textual analysis. Discourse Process 12:245–61 [Google Scholar]
  139. Witkowski SR. 1972. Guttman scaling of semantic distinctions. Kinship Studies in the Morgan Centennial Year P Reining 167–88 Washington, DC: Anthropol. Soc. Wash. [Google Scholar]
  140. Witkowski SR, Brown CH. 1978. Lexical universals. Annu. Rev. Anthropol. 7:427–51 [Google Scholar]
  141. Witkowski SR, Burris HW. 1981. Societal complexity and lexical growth. Cross-Cult. Res. 16:143–59 [Google Scholar]
  142. Xu J, Dowman M, Griffiths TL. 2013. Cultural transmission results in convergence towards colour term universals. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 280:20123073 [Google Scholar]
  143. Xu Y, Regier T. 2014. Numeral systems across languages support efficient communication: from approximate numerosity to recursion. Proceedings of the 36th Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society P Bello, M Guarini, M McShane, B Scassellati 1802–7 Cambridge, MA: Cogn. Sci. Soc. [Google Scholar]
  144. Xu Y, Regier T, Malt BC. 2016. Historical semantic chaining and efficient communication: the case of container names. Cogn. Sci. 40:2081–94 [Google Scholar]
  145. Youn H, Sutton L, Smith E, Moore C, Wilkins JF. et al. 2016. On the universal structure of human lexical semantics. PNAS 113:1766–71 [Google Scholar]
  146. Zalizniak AA, Bulakh M, Ganenkov D, Gruntov I, Maisak T, Russo M. 2012. The catalogue of semantic shifts as a database for lexical semantic typology. Linguistics 50:633–69 [Google Scholar]
  147. Zhou K, Bowern C. 2015. Quantifying uncertainty in the phylogenetics of Australian numeral systems. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 282:20151278 [Google Scholar]
  148. Zipf GK. 1949. Human Behavior and the Principle of Least Effort Boston: Addison-Wesley [Google Scholar]
/content/journals/10.1146/annurev-linguistics-011817-045406
Loading
/content/journals/10.1146/annurev-linguistics-011817-045406
Loading

Data & Media loading...

  • Article Type: Review Article
This is a required field
Please enter a valid email address
Approval was a Success
Invalid data
An Error Occurred
Approval was partially successful, following selected items could not be processed due to error