1932

Abstract

Socioeconomic status (SES) differences in language development are ubiquitous, but existing research has yet to wrestle with how language gaps reflect () differences in relevant concepts for communication, () comprehension strategies to access meanings, and () production practices that express social identity. In child-directed input, parents use verbs to describe similar concepts across SES, and the largest gaps emerge when frequent meanings are being conveyed. During comprehension, children acquire infrequent aspects of grammar across SES but differ in context-specific strategies for interpreting likely meanings. In production, children are sensitive to sociolinguistic implications and adopt context-specific strategies to signal social identity. This suggests that language is a flexible medium for communicating thoughts and that SES effects signal latent differences in meanings and identities across social classes. Whether language gaps contribute to achievement gaps may depend on the extent to which learning and communication draw on these meanings and value these identities.

Loading

Article metrics loading...

/content/journals/10.1146/annurev-linguistics-030521-045816
2023-01-17
2024-05-23
Loading full text...

Full text loading...

/deliver/fulltext/linguistics/9/1/annurev-linguistics-030521-045816.html?itemId=/content/journals/10.1146/annurev-linguistics-030521-045816&mimeType=html&fmt=ahah

Literature Cited

  1. Acton EK, Potts C. 2014. That straight talk: Sarah Palin and the sociolinguistics of demonstratives. J. Sociolinguist. 18:13–31
    [Google Scholar]
  2. Anderson ES. 1999. What is the point of equality?. Ethics 109:2287–337
    [Google Scholar]
  3. Babineau M, Shi R, Christophe A 2020. 14-month-olds exploit verbs’ syntactic contexts to build expectations about novel words. Infancy 25:5719–33
    [Google Scholar]
  4. Bates E, MacWhinney B, Caselli C, Devescovi A, Natale F, Venza V. 1984. A cross-linguistic study of the development of sentence interpretation strategies. Child Dev. 55:2341–54
    [Google Scholar]
  5. Bates E, McNew S, MacWhinney B, Devescovi A, Smith S. 1982. Functional constraints on sentence processing: a cross-linguistic study. Cognition 11:3245–99
    [Google Scholar]
  6. Baugh J. 2017. Meaning-less differences: exposing fallacies and flaws in “the word gap” hypothesis that conceal a dangerous “language trap” for low-income American families and their children. Int. Multiling. Res. J. 11:139–51
    [Google Scholar]
  7. Beltrama A, Schwarz F 2021. Imprecision, personae, and pragmatic reasoning. Proceedings of the 31st Conference on Semantics and Linguistic Theory (SALT 31) N Dreier, C Kwon, T Darnell, J Starr 122–44. Washington, DC: Linguist. Soc. Am.
    [Google Scholar]
  8. Bianchi SM, Robinson JP, Milke MA. 2006. The Changing Rhythms of American Family Life New York: Russell Sage Found.
  9. Blau FD, Kahn LM. 2017. The gender wage gap: extent, trends, and explanations. J. Econ. Lit. 55:3789–865
    [Google Scholar]
  10. Brooks PJ, Tomasello M. 1999. Young children learn to produce passives with nonce verbs. Dev. Psychol. 35:29–44
    [Google Scholar]
  11. Brysbaert M, Warriner AB, Kuperman V. 2014. Concreteness ratings for 40 thousand generally known English word lemmas. Behav. Res. Methods 46:3904–11
    [Google Scholar]
  12. Byrd AS, Huang YT, Edwards J. 2022. Examining how African American English- and Mainstream American English-speaking children process subject-verb agreement in sentences Poster presented at the 47th Boston University Conference on Language Development Boston: Nov. 3–6
  13. Cameron-Faulkner T, Lieven E, Tomasello M. 2003. A construction based analysis of child directed speech. Cogn. Sci. 27:6843–73
    [Google Scholar]
  14. Campbell-Kibler K. 2007. Accent, (ING), and the social logic of listener perceptions. Am. Speech 82:132–64
    [Google Scholar]
  15. Chan A, Lieven E, Tomasello M 2009. Children's understanding of the agent-patient relations in the transitive construction: cross-linguistic comparisons between Cantonese, German, and English. Cogn. Linguist. 20:2267–300
    [Google Scholar]
  16. Chang F, Dell GS, Bock K. 2006. Becoming syntactic. Psychol. Rev. 113:2234–72
    [Google Scholar]
  17. Christiansen MH, Chater N. 2016. The now-or-never bottleneck: a fundamental constraint on language. Behav. Brain Sci. 39:E62
    [Google Scholar]
  18. Corriveau KH, Kurkul K, Arunachalam S. 2016. Preschoolers' preference for syntactic complexity varies by socioeconomic status. Child Dev. 87:51529–37
    [Google Scholar]
  19. Craig HK, Grogger JT. 2012. Influences of social and style variables on adult usage of African American English features. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 55:51274–88
    [Google Scholar]
  20. Curenton SM, Justice LM. 2004. African American and Caucasian preschoolers’ use of decontextualized language. Lang. Speech Hear. Serv. Sch. 35:240–53
    [Google Scholar]
  21. Dautriche I, Cristia A, Brusini P, Yuan S, Fisher C, Christophe A 2014. Toddlers default to canonical surface-to-meaning mapping when learning verbs. Child Dev. 85:31168–80
    [Google Scholar]
  22. Dell GS, Chang F. 2014. The P-chain: relating sentence production and its disorders to comprehension and acquisition. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B 369:163420120394
    [Google Scholar]
  23. Demir ÖE, Rowe ML, Heller G, Goldin-Meadow S, Levine SC. 2015. Vocabulary, syntax, and narrative development in typically developing children and children with early unilateral brain injury: early parental talk about the “there-and-then” matters. Dev. Psychol. 51:2161–75
    [Google Scholar]
  24. Demuth K. 1989. Maturation and the acquisition of the Sesotho passive. Language 65:56–80
    [Google Scholar]
  25. Díaz-Campos MA. 2001. Acquisition of phonological structure and sociolinguistic variables: a quantitative analysis of Spanish consonant weakening in Venezuelan children's speech PhD Diss. Ohio State Univ. Columbus:
  26. Díaz-Campos MA. 2005. The emergence of adult-like command of sociolinguistic variables: a study of consonant weakening in Spanish-speaking children. Selected Proceedings of the 6th Conference on the Acquisition of Spanish and Portuguese as First and Second Languages D Eddington 56–65. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla
    [Google Scholar]
  27. Dunn J, Shatz M. 1989. Becoming a conversationalist despite (or because of) having an older sibling. Child Dev. 60:399–410
    [Google Scholar]
  28. Durham RE, Farkas G, Hammer CS, Tomblin JB, Catts HW. 2007. Kindergarten oral language skill: a key variable in the intergenerational transmission of socioeconomic status. Res. Soc. Stratif. Mobil. 25:4294–305
    [Google Scholar]
  29. Eckert P. 2012. Three waves of variation study: the emergence of meaning in the study of sociolinguistic variation. Annu. Rev. Anthropol. 41:87–100
    [Google Scholar]
  30. Eckert P. 2019. The limits of meaning: social indexicality, variation, and the cline of interiority. Language 95:4751–76
    [Google Scholar]
  31. Ellwood-Lowe ME, Sacchet MD, Gotlib IH 2016. The application of neuroimaging to social inequity and language disparity: a cautionary examination. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 22:1–8
    [Google Scholar]
  32. Fernald A, Marchman VA, Weisleder A. 2013. SES differences in language processing skill and vocabulary are evident at 18 months. Dev. Sci. 16:2234–48
    [Google Scholar]
  33. Gleitman L. 1990. The structural sources of verb meanings. Lang. Acquis. 1:13–55
    [Google Scholar]
  34. Golinkoff RM, Hoff E, Rowe ML, Tamis-LeMonda CS, Hirsh-Pasek K. 2019. Language matters: Denying the existence of the 30-million-word gap has serious consequences. Child Dev. 90:3985–92
    [Google Scholar]
  35. Goodwin MH. 2007. Occasioned knowledge exploration in family interaction. Discourse Soc. 18:193–110
    [Google Scholar]
  36. Goodwin MH, Cekaite A. 2013. Calibration in directive/response sequences in family interaction. J. Pragmat. 46:1122–38
    [Google Scholar]
  37. Goodwin MH, Goodwin C 2013. Nurturing. Fast-Forward Family: Home, Work, and Relationships in Middle-Class America E Ochs, T Kremer-Sadlik 151–73. Berkeley: Univ. Calif. Press
    [Google Scholar]
  38. Gordon P, Chafetz J. 1990. Verb-based versus class-based accounts of actionality effects in children's comprehension of passives. Cognition 36:3227–54
    [Google Scholar]
  39. Grieser JA. 2015. The language of professional Blackness: African American English at the intersection of race, place, and class in southeast Washington, DC PhD Diss. Georgetown Univ. Washington, DC:
  40. Grieser JA. 2022. The Black Side of the River: Race, Language, and Belonging in Washington, DC Washington, DC: Georgetown Univ. Press
  41. Hall WS, Nagy WE, Linn R. 1984. Spoken Words: Effects of Situation and Social Group on Oral Word Usage and Frequency Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum
  42. Hart B, Risley TR. 1995. Meaningful Differences in the Everyday Experience of Young American Children Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes Publ.
  43. Havron N, de Carvalho A, Fiévet AC, Christophe A 2019a. Three- to four-year-old children rapidly adapt their predictions and use them to learn novel word meanings. Child Dev. 90:182–90
    [Google Scholar]
  44. Havron N, Ramus F, Heude B, Forhan A, Cristia A, Peyre H EDEN Mother-Child Cohort Study Group 2019b. The effect of older siblings on language development as a function of age difference and sex. Psychol. Sci. 30:91333–43
    [Google Scholar]
  45. Hirsh-Pasek K, Adamson LB, Bakeman R, Owen MT, Golinkoff RM et al. 2015. The contribution of early communication quality to low-income children's language success. Psychol. Sci. 26:71071–83
    [Google Scholar]
  46. Hoff E. 2003. The specificity of environmental influence: Socioeconomic status affects early vocabulary development via maternal speech. Child Dev. 74:51368–78
    [Google Scholar]
  47. Hofferth SL, Sandberg JF. 2001. How American children spend their time. J. Marriage Fam. 63:2295–308
    [Google Scholar]
  48. Hoff-Ginsberg E. 1986. Function and structure in maternal speech: their relation to the child's development of syntax. Dev. Psychol. 22:2155–63
    [Google Scholar]
  49. Hoff-Ginsberg E. 1991. Mother-child conversation in different social classes and communicative settings. Child Dev. 62:4782–96
    [Google Scholar]
  50. Holt YF. 2018. Mechanisms of vowel variation in African American English. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 61:2197–209
    [Google Scholar]
  51. Horton-Ikard R. 2006. The influence of culture, class, and linguistic diversity on early language development. Zero to Three 27:16–12
    [Google Scholar]
  52. Horton-Ikard R, Miller JF. 2004. It is not just the poor kids: the use of AAE forms by African-American school-aged children from middle SES communities. J. Commun. Disord. 37:6467–87
    [Google Scholar]
  53. Huang YT, Arnold AR. 2016. Word learning in linguistic context: processing and memory effects. Cognition 157:71–87
    [Google Scholar]
  54. Huang YT, Hollister E. 2019. Developmental parsing and linguistic knowledge: reexamining the role of cognitive control in the kindergarten path effect. J. Exp. Child. Psychol. 184:210–19
    [Google Scholar]
  55. Huang YT, Leech K, Rowe ML. 2017. Exploring socioeconomic differences in syntactic development through the lens of real-time processing. Cognition 159:61–75
    [Google Scholar]
  56. Huang YT, Ovans Z. 2022. Who “it” is influences what “it” does: discourse effects on children's syntactic parsing. Cogn. Sci. 46:1e13076
    [Google Scholar]
  57. Huang YT, Zheng X, Meng X, Snedeker J. 2013. Children's assignment of grammatical roles in the online processing of Mandarin passive sentences. J. Mem. Lang. 69:4589–606
    [Google Scholar]
  58. Hurtado N, Marchman VA, Fernald A. 2008. Does input influence uptake? Links between maternal talk, processing speed and vocabulary size in Spanish-learning children. Dev. Sci. 11:6F31–39
    [Google Scholar]
  59. Huttenlocher J, Vasilyeva M, Cymerman E, Levine S. 2002. Language input and child syntax. Cogn. Psychol. 45:3337–74
    [Google Scholar]
  60. Huttenlocher J, Waterfall H, Vasilyeva M, Vevea J, Hedges LV. 2010. Sources of variability in children's language growth. Cogn. Psychol. 61:4343–65
    [Google Scholar]
  61. Johnson EK, White KS. 2020. Developmental sociolinguistics: children's acquisition of language variation. WIREs Cogn. Sci. 11:1e1515
    [Google Scholar]
  62. Kemp C, Xu Y, Regier T. 2018. Semantic typology and efficient communication. Annu. Rev. Linguist. 4:109–28
    [Google Scholar]
  63. Kirby S, Tamariz M, Cornish H, Smith K. 2015. Compression and communication in the cultural evolution of linguistic structure. Cognition 141:87–102
    [Google Scholar]
  64. Kleinschmidt DF. 2019. Structure in talker variability: How much is there and how much can it help?. Lang. Cogn. Neurosci. 34:143–68
    [Google Scholar]
  65. Laakso A, Smith LB. 2007. Pronouns and verbs in adult speech to children: a corpus analysis. J. Child Lang. 34:4725–63
    [Google Scholar]
  66. Labov W. 1972a. Sociolinguistic Patterns Philadelphia: Univ. Pa. Press
  67. Labov W. 1972b. Language in the Inner City: Studies in the Black English Vernacular Philadelphia: Univ. Pa. Press
  68. Labov W. 2001. Applying our knowledge of African American English to the problem of raising reading levels in inner-city schools. Sociocultural and Historical Contexts of African American English SL Lanehart 299–317. Amsterdam: John Benjamins
    [Google Scholar]
  69. Landrum AR, Eaves BS Jr., Shafto P. 2015. Learning to trust and trusting to learn: a theoretical framework. Trends Cogn. Sci. 19:3109–11
    [Google Scholar]
  70. Leech KA, Haber AS, Arunachalam S, Kurkul K, Corriveau KH. 2019. On the malleability of selective trust. J. Exp. Child. Psychol. 183:65–74
    [Google Scholar]
  71. Leech KA, Rowe ML, Huang YT. 2017. Variations in the recruitment of syntactic knowledge contribute to SES differences in syntactic development. J. Child Lang. 44:4995–1009
    [Google Scholar]
  72. Leung CY, Hernandez MW, Suskind DL. 2020. Enriching home language environment among families from low-SES backgrounds: a randomized controlled trial of a home visiting curriculum. Early Child. Res. Q. 50:24–35
    [Google Scholar]
  73. Lidz J, White AS, Baier R. 2017. The role of incremental parsing in syntactically conditioned word learning. Cogn. Psychol. 97:62–78
    [Google Scholar]
  74. Lupyan G, Dale R. 2016. Why are there different languages? The role of adaptation in linguistic diversity. Trends Cogn. Sci. 20:9649–60
    [Google Scholar]
  75. Lurie LA, Hagen MP, McLaughlin KA, Sheridan MA, Meltzoff AN, Rosen ML. 2021. Mechanisms linking socioeconomic status and academic achievement in early childhood: cognitive stimulation and language. Cogn. Dev. 58:101045
    [Google Scholar]
  76. MacWhinney B, Bates E, Kliegl R. 1984. Cue validity and sentence interpretation in English, German, and Italian. J. Verbal Learn. Verbal Behav. 23:2127–50
    [Google Scholar]
  77. Maratsos M, Fox DE, Becker JA, Chalkley MA. 1985. Semantic restrictions on children's passives. Cognition 19:2167–91
    [Google Scholar]
  78. Martin I, Goupell M, Huang Y. 2022. Children's syntactic parsing and sentence comprehension with a degraded auditory signal. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 151:699–711
    [Google Scholar]
  79. Medin D, Bennis W, Chandler M. 2010. Culture and the home-field disadvantage. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 5:6708–13
    [Google Scholar]
  80. Montag JL, Jones MN, Smith LB. 2018. Quantity and diversity: simulating early word learning environments. Cogn. Sci. 42:375–412
    [Google Scholar]
  81. Noble KG, Norman MF, Farah MJ. 2005. Neurocognitive correlates of socioeconomic status in kindergarten children. Dev. Sci. 8:174–87
    [Google Scholar]
  82. Ochs E, Kremer-Sadlik T. 2015. How postindustrial families talk. Annu. Rev. Anthropol. 44:87–103
    [Google Scholar]
  83. Oppenheimer K, Rowe M, Huang Y. 2020. SES differences in verb usage mediate form-function relations in parental speech Poster presented at the 45th Boston University Conference on Language Development Boston: Nov. 5–8
  84. Pearl L, Bates A. 2022. A new way to identify if variation in children's input could be developmentally meaningful: using computational cognitive modeling to assess input across socio-economic status for syntactic islands. J. Child Lang. In press
    [Google Scholar]
  85. Perkins L, Lidz J. 2021. Eighteen-month-old infants represent nonlocal syntactic dependencies. PNAS 118:41e2026469118
    [Google Scholar]
  86. Qi Z, Love J, Fisher C, Brown-Schmidt S. 2020. Referential context and executive functioning influence children's resolution of syntactic ambiguity. J. Exp. Psychol. 46:101922–47
    [Google Scholar]
  87. Qi Z, Yuan S, Fisher C 2011. Where does verb bias come from? Experience with particular verbs affects on-line sentence processing. Proceedings of the 35th Boston University Conference on Language Development N Danis, K Mesh, H Sung 500–12. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla
    [Google Scholar]
  88. Ramírez NF, Lytle SR, Kuhl PK. 2020. Parent coaching increases conversational turns and advances infant language development. PNAS 117:73484–91
    [Google Scholar]
  89. Rickford JR, Duncan GJ, Gennetian LA, Gou RY, Greene R et al. 2015. Neighborhood effects on use of African American Vernacular English. PNAS 112:3811817–22
    [Google Scholar]
  90. Rickford JR, Price M. 2013. Girlz II women: age-grading, language change and stylistic variation. J. Socioling. 17:2143–79
    [Google Scholar]
  91. Romeo RR, Flournoy JC, McLaughlin KA, Lengua LJ. 2022. Language development as a mechanism linking socioeconomic status to executive functioning development in preschool. Dev. Sci. 25:5e13227
    [Google Scholar]
  92. Romeo RR, Leonard JA, Robinson ST, West MR, Mackey AP et al. 2018a. Beyond the 30-million-word gap: Children's conversational exposure is associated with language-related brain function. Psychol. Sci. 29:5700–10
    [Google Scholar]
  93. Romeo RR, Segaran J, Leonard JA, Robinson ST, West MR et al. 2018b. Language exposure relates to structural neural connectivity in childhood. J. Neurosci. 38:367870–77
    [Google Scholar]
  94. Rowe ML. 2000. Pointing and talk by low-income mothers and their 14-month-old children. First Lang. 20:60305–30
    [Google Scholar]
  95. Rowe ML. 2008. Child-directed speech: relation to socioeconomic status, knowledge of child development and child vocabulary skill. J. Child Lang. 35:1185–205
    [Google Scholar]
  96. Rowe ML. 2012. A longitudinal investigation of the role of quantity and quality of child-directed speech in vocabulary development. Child Dev. 83:51762–74
    [Google Scholar]
  97. Rowe ML, Denmark N, Harden BJ, Stapleton LM. 2016. The role of parent education and parenting knowledge in children's language and literacy skills among White, Black, and Latino families. Infant Child Dev. 25:2198–220
    [Google Scholar]
  98. Rowe ML, Goldin-Meadow S. 2009. Differences in early gesture explain SES disparities in child vocabulary size at school entry. Science 323:5916951–53
    [Google Scholar]
  99. Rowe ML, Leech KA. 2019. A parent intervention with a growth mindset approach improves children's early gesture and vocabulary development. Dev. Sci. 22:4e12792
    [Google Scholar]
  100. Roy BC, Frank MC, DeCamp P, Miller M, Roy D 2015. Predicting the birth of a spoken word. PNAS 112:4112663–68
    [Google Scholar]
  101. Smith J, Durham M, Fortune L. 2007.. “ Mam, my trousers is fa'in doon!”: Community, caregiver, and child in the acquisition of variation in a Scottish dialect. Lang. Var. Change 19:163–99
    [Google Scholar]
  102. Smith J, Durham M, Richards H. 2013. The social and linguistic in the acquisition of sociolinguistic norms: caregivers, children, and variation. Linguistics 51:2285–324
    [Google Scholar]
  103. Sneller B, Fisher S. 2015. When GET got noticed: the emerging salience of GET-passives. Univ. Pa. Work. Pap. Linguist. 21:134
    [Google Scholar]
  104. Sneller B, Roberts G. 2018. Why some behaviors spread while others don't: a laboratory simulation of dialect contact. Cognition 170:298–311
    [Google Scholar]
  105. Snow CE. 1990. The development of definitional skill. J. Child Lang. 17:3697–710
    [Google Scholar]
  106. Sperry DE, Sperry LL, Miller PJ. 2019. Reexamining the verbal environments of children from different socioeconomic backgrounds. Child Dev. 90:41303–18
    [Google Scholar]
  107. Suanda SH, Smith LB, Yu C. 2016. The multisensory nature of verbal discourse in parent–toddler interactions. Dev. Neuropsychol. 41:5–8324–41
    [Google Scholar]
  108. Suskind DL, Leffel KR, Graf E, Hernandez MW, Gunderson EA et al. 2016. A parent-directed language intervention for children of low socioeconomic status: a randomized controlled pilot study. J. Child Lang. 43:2366–406
    [Google Scholar]
  109. Tabors P, Snow C, Dickinson D 2001. Homes and schools together: supporting language and literacy development. Beginning Literacy with Language D Dickinson, P Tabors 313–34. Baltimore, MD: Brookes
    [Google Scholar]
  110. Tannen D. 1995. The power of talk: who gets heard and why. Harvard Bus. Rev. 73:5138–48
    [Google Scholar]
  111. Tardif T, Shatz M, Naigles L. 1997. Caregiver speech and children's use of nouns versus verbs: a comparison of English, Italian, and Mandarin. J. Child Lang. 24:3535–65
    [Google Scholar]
  112. Tripp A, Feldman NH, Idsardi WJ. 2021. Social inference may guide early lexical learning. Front. Psychol. 12:645247
    [Google Scholar]
  113. Uccelli P, Demir-Lira ÖE, Rowe ML, Levine S, Goldin-Meadow S. 2019. Children's early decontextualized talk predicts academic language proficiency in midadolescence. Child Dev. 90:51650–63
    [Google Scholar]
  114. Van Hofwegen J, Wolfram W 2010. Coming of age in African American English: a longitudinal study. J. Socioling. 14:4427–55
    [Google Scholar]
  115. Van Horne AO, Curran M, Hall J. 2017. Can vocabulary lessons increase the amount of complex syntax produced by Head Start teachers? A pilot study. Child Lang. Teach. Ther. 33:3305–19
    [Google Scholar]
  116. Vernon-Feagans L, Pancsofar N, Willoughby M, Odom E, Quade A, Cox M, Family Life Key Investigators. 2008. Predictors of maternal language to infants during a picture book task in the home: family SES, child characteristics and the parenting environment. J. Appl. Dev. Psychol. 29:3213–26
    [Google Scholar]
  117. Wagner L, Clopper CG, Pate JK. 2014. Children's perception of dialect variation. J. Child Lang. 41:1062–84
    [Google Scholar]
  118. Weatherhead D, Kandhadai P, Hall DG, Werker JF. 2021. Putting mutual exclusivity in context: Speaker race influences monolingual and bilingual infants’ word-learning assumptions. Child Dev. 92:51735–51
    [Google Scholar]
  119. Weiner EJ, Labov W. 1983. Constraints on the agentless passive. J. Linguist. 19:129–58
    [Google Scholar]
  120. Weisleder A, Fernald A. 2013. Talking to children matters: Early language experience strengthens processing and builds vocabulary. Psychol. Sci. 24:112143–52
    [Google Scholar]
  121. Weizman ZO, Snow CE. 2001. Lexical output as related to children's vocabulary acquisition: effects of sophisticated exposure and support for meaning. Dev. Psychol. 37:2265–79
    [Google Scholar]
  122. Weldon TL. 2021. Middle-Class African American English Cambridge, UK: Cambridge Univ. Press
  123. Woodard K, Pozzan L, Trueswell JC. 2016. Taking your own path: individual differences in executive function and language processing skills in child learners. J. Exp. Child. Psychol. 141:187–209
    [Google Scholar]
  124. Yang Y, Goodhue D, Hacquard V, Lidz J. 2022. Are you asking me or telling me? Learning to identify questions in early speech to children Poster presented at the 96th Annual Meeting of the Linguistic Society of America (LSA) Washington, DC: Jan. 6–9
  125. Yazbec A, Kaschak MP, Borovsky A. 2019. Developmental time scale of rapid adaptation to conflicting cues in real-time sentence processing. Cogn. Sci. 43:1e12704
    [Google Scholar]
  126. Yurovsky D, Case S, Frank MC. 2017. Preschoolers flexibly adapt to linguistic input in a noisy channel. Psychol. Sci. 28:1132–40
    [Google Scholar]
/content/journals/10.1146/annurev-linguistics-030521-045816
Loading
/content/journals/10.1146/annurev-linguistics-030521-045816
Loading

Data & Media loading...

  • Article Type: Review Article
This is a required field
Please enter a valid email address
Approval was a Success
Invalid data
An Error Occurred
Approval was partially successful, following selected items could not be processed due to error