1932

Abstract

Children's linguistic knowledge and the learning mechanisms by which they acquire it grow substantially in infancy and toddlerhood, yet theories of word learning largely fail to incorporate these shifts. Moreover, researchers’ often-siloed focus on either familiar word recognition or novel word learning limits the critical consideration of how these two relate. As a step toward a mechanistic theory of language acquisition, we present a framework of “learning through processing” and relate it to the prevailing methods used to assess children's early knowledge of words. Incorporating recent empirical work, we posit a specific, testable timeline of qualitative changes in the learning process in this interval. We conclude with several challenges and avenues for building a comprehensive theory of early word learning: better characterization of the input, reconciling results across approaches, and treating lexical knowledge in the nascent grammar with sufficient sophistication to ensure generalizability across languages and development.

Loading

Article metrics loading...

/content/journals/10.1146/annurev-linguistics-031220-011146
2022-01-14
2024-12-07
Loading full text...

Full text loading...

/deliver/fulltext/linguistics/8/1/annurev-linguistics-031220-011146.html?itemId=/content/journals/10.1146/annurev-linguistics-031220-011146&mimeType=html&fmt=ahah

Literature Cited

  1. Anderson IR. 1990. The Adaptive Character of Thought Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum
    [Google Scholar]
  2. Arunachalam S, Escovar E, Hansen MA, Waxman SR. 2013. Out of sight, but not out of mind: 21-month-olds use syntactic information to learn verbs even in the absence of a corresponding event. Lang. Cogn. Process. 28:4417–25
    [Google Scholar]
  3. Babineau M, Carvalho A, Trueswell J, Christophe A 2021. Familiar words can serve as a semantic seed for syntactic bootstrapping. Dev. Sci. 24:1e13010
    [Google Scholar]
  4. Babineau M, Shi R, Christophe A. 2020. 14-month-olds exploit verbs' syntactic contexts to build expectations about novel words. Infancy 25:5719–33
    [Google Scholar]
  5. Baldwin DA. 1993. Early referential understanding: infants' ability to recognize referential acts for what they are. Dev. Psychol. 29:5832–43
    [Google Scholar]
  6. Bates E. 1979. The Emergence of Symbols: Cognition and Communication in Infancy San Diego, CA: Academic
    [Google Scholar]
  7. Bates E, Goodman JC. 1997. On the inseparability of grammar and the lexicon: evidence from acquisition, aphasia, and real-time processing. Lang. Cogn. Process. 12:5/6507–84
    [Google Scholar]
  8. Bates E, MacWhinney B 1989. Functionalism and the competition model. The Crosslinguistic Study of Sentence Processing B MacWhinney, E Bates 3–73 Cambridge, UK: Cambridge Univ. Press
    [Google Scholar]
  9. Behne T, Liszkowski U, Carpenter M, Tomasello M. 2012. Twelve-month-olds' comprehension and production of pointing. Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 30:3359–75
    [Google Scholar]
  10. Benedict H. 1979. Early lexical development: comprehension and production. J. Child Lang. 6:2183–200
    [Google Scholar]
  11. Bergelson E. 2020. The comprehension boost in early word learning: Older infants are better learners. Child Dev. Perspect. 14:3142–49
    [Google Scholar]
  12. Bergelson E, Amatuni A, Dailey S, Koorathota S, Tor S 2018. Day by day, hour by hour: naturalistic language input to infants. Dev. Sci. 22:1e12715
    [Google Scholar]
  13. Bergelson E, Aslin R. 2017. Semantic specificity in one-year-olds' word comprehension. Lang. Learn. Dev. 13:4481–501
    [Google Scholar]
  14. Bergelson E, Swingley D. 2012. At 6–9 months, human infants know the meanings of many common nouns. PNAS 109:93253–58
    [Google Scholar]
  15. Bergelson E, Swingley D. 2013a. The acquisition of abstract words by young infants. Cognition 127:3391–97
    [Google Scholar]
  16. Bergelson E, Swingley D. 2013b. Young toddlers' word comprehension is flexible and efficient. PLOS ONE 8:8e73359
    [Google Scholar]
  17. Bergelson E, Swingley D. 2015. Early word comprehension in infants: replication and extension. Lang. Learn. Dev. 11:4369–80
    [Google Scholar]
  18. Bloom L. 1993. The Transition from Infancy to Language: Acquiring the Power of Expression Cambridge, UK: Cambridge Univ. Press
    [Google Scholar]
  19. Bloom P. 2002. How Children Learn the Meanings of Words Cambridge, MA: MIT Press
    [Google Scholar]
  20. Bohn M, Tessler MH, Merrick M, Frank MC 2021. How young children integrate information sources to infer the meaning of words. Nat. Hum. Behav. 5:1046–54
    [Google Scholar]
  21. Brown R. 1973. A First Language: The Early Stages Cambridge, MA: Harvard Univ. Press
    [Google Scholar]
  22. Brysbaert M, Warriner AB, Kuperman V. 2014. Concreteness ratings for 40 thousand generally known English word lemmas. Behav. Res. Methods 46:3904–11
    [Google Scholar]
  23. Byers-Heinlein K, Werker JF. 2013. Lexicon structure and the disambiguation of novel words: evidence from bilingual infants. Cognition 128:3407–16
    [Google Scholar]
  24. Campbell JCS. 2018. The nature of infants' early object word comprehension. PhD Thesis Univ. B. C. Vancouver, Can:.
    [Google Scholar]
  25. Carey S, Bartlett E 1978. Acquiring a single new word. Proceedings of the 15th Stanford Child Language Conference17–29 Stanford, CA: Dep. Linguist., Stanford Univ.
    [Google Scholar]
  26. Carpenter M, Nagell K, Tomasello M, Butterworth G, Moore C. 1998. Social cognition, joint attention, and communicative competence from 9 to 15 months of age. Monogr. Soc. Res. Child Dev. 63:41–174
    [Google Scholar]
  27. Casillas M, Brown P, Levinson SC. 2020. Early language experience in a Tseltal Mayan village. Child Dev. 91:51819–35
    [Google Scholar]
  28. Casillas M, Brown P, Levinson SC. 2021. Early language experience in a Papuan community. J. Child Lang. 48:4792–814
    [Google Scholar]
  29. Casillas M, Cristia A. 2019. A step-by-step guide to collecting and analyzing long-format speech environment (LFSE) recordings. Collabra 5:124
    [Google Scholar]
  30. Chater N, Crocker MJ, Pickering MJ 1998. The rational analysis of inquiry: the case of parsing. Rational Models of Cognition M Oaksford, N Chater 441–68 Oxford, UK: Oxford Univ. Press
    [Google Scholar]
  31. Clark EV, Hecht BF. 1983. Comprehension, production, and language acquisition. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 34:325–49
    [Google Scholar]
  32. Crain S. 2017. Acquisition of quantifiers. Annu. Rev. Linguist. 3:219–43
    [Google Scholar]
  33. Cristia A. 2020. Language input and outcome variation as a test of theory plausibility: the case of early phonological acquisition. Dev. Rev. 57:100914
    [Google Scholar]
  34. Cristia A, Dupoux E, Gurven M, Stieglitz J 2019. Child-directed speech is infrequent in a forager–farmer population: a time allocation study. Child Dev. 90:3759–73
    [Google Scholar]
  35. Demuth K, Moloi F, Machobane M. 2010. 3-year-olds' comprehension, production, and generalization of Sesotho passives. Cognition 115:238–51
    [Google Scholar]
  36. Diamond A. 1985. Development of the ability to use recall to guide action, as indicated by infants' performance on AB. Child Dev. 56:4868–83
    [Google Scholar]
  37. Dunn L, Dunn LM, Bulheller S, Häcker H. 1965. Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test Circle Pines, MN: Am. Guid. Serv.
    [Google Scholar]
  38. Dupoux E. 2018. Cognitive science in the era of artificial intelligence—a roadmap for reverse-engineering the infant language-learner. Cognition 173:43–59
    [Google Scholar]
  39. Feiman R, Mody S, Sanborn S, Carey S. 2017. What do you mean, no? Toddlers' comprehension of logical “no” and “not”. Lang. Learn. Dev. 13:4430–50
    [Google Scholar]
  40. Feldbaum C 1998. WordNet: An Electronic Lexical Database Cambridge, MA: MIT Press
    [Google Scholar]
  41. Fenson L, Dale PS, Reznick JS, Bates E, Thal DJ et al. 1994. Variability in early communicative development. Monogr. Soc. Res. Child Dev. 59:51–173
    [Google Scholar]
  42. Fernald A, Frank MC 2012. Finding the words: how young children develop skill in interpreting spoken language. The Cambridge Handbook of Psycholinguistics M Spivey, K McRae, M Joanisse 104–26 Cambridge, UK: Cambridge Univ. Press
    [Google Scholar]
  43. Fernald A, Perfors A, Marchman VA. 2006. Picking up speed in understanding: speech processing efficiency and vocabulary growth across the 2nd year. Dev. Psychol. 42:198–116
    [Google Scholar]
  44. Fernald A, Pinto JP, Swingley D, Weinberg A, McRoberts GW. 1998. Rapid gains in speed of verbal processing by infants in the 2nd year. Psychol. Sci. 9:3228–31
    [Google Scholar]
  45. Fisher C, Gleitman LR. 2002. Language acquisition. Steven's Handbook of Experimental Psychology 3 Learning, Motivation, and Emotion, ed. H Pashler, R Gallistel 445–96 New York: Wiley, 3rd ed..
    [Google Scholar]
  46. Fisher C, Jin K-S, Scott RM. 2020. The developmental origins of syntactic bootstrapping. Top. Cogn. Sci. 12:148–77
    [Google Scholar]
  47. Floyd S, Goldberg AE 2021. Children make use of relationships across meanings in word learning. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 47:129–44
    [Google Scholar]
  48. Forgács B, Parise E, Csibra G, Gergely G, Jacquey L, Gervain J 2019. Fourteen-month-old infants track the language comprehension of communicative partners. Dev. Sci. 22:2e12751
    [Google Scholar]
  49. Frank MC. 2016. Chasing the Rubicon?. Am. J. Psychol. 129:199–104
    [Google Scholar]
  50. Frank MC, Braginsky M, Yurovsky D, Marchman VA 2021. Variability and Consistency in Early Language Learning: The Wordbank Project Cambridge, MA: MIT Press
    [Google Scholar]
  51. Frank MC, Goodman ND, Tenenbaum JB. 2009a. Using speakers' referential intentions to model early cross-situational word learning. Psychol. Sci. 20:5578–85
    [Google Scholar]
  52. Frank MC, Vul E, Johnson SP. 2009b. Development of infants' attention to faces during the first year. Cognition 110:2160–70
    [Google Scholar]
  53. Friend M, Schmitt SA, Simpson AM. 2012. Evaluating the predictive validity of the computerized comprehension task: Comprehension predicts production. Dev. Psychol. 48:1136–48
    [Google Scholar]
  54. Gangemi A, Guarino N, Oltramari A. 2001. Conceptual analysis of lexical taxonomies: the case of WordNet top-level. Proceedings of the International Conference on Formal Ontology in Information Systems285–96 New York: ACM
    [Google Scholar]
  55. Gardner BT, Gardner RA 1971. Two-way communication with an infant chimpanzee. Behavior of Nonhuman Primates 4 AM Schrier, F Stollnitz 117–84 Amsterdam: Elsevier
    [Google Scholar]
  56. Garrison H, Baudet G, Breitfeld E, Aberman A, Bergelson E 2020. Familiarity plays a small role in noun comprehension at 12–18 months. Infancy 25:4458–77
    [Google Scholar]
  57. Gentner D. 1982. Why nouns are learned before verbs: linguistic relativity versus natural partitioning. Language Development 2 Language, Thought, and Culture, ed. SA Kuczaj 301–34 Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum
    [Google Scholar]
  58. Gerken L, Mclntosh BJ. 1993. Interplay of function morphemes and prosody in early language. Dev. Psychol. 29:3448–57
    [Google Scholar]
  59. Gibson E, Bergen L, Piantadosi S. 2013. Rational integration of noisy evidence and prior semantic expectations in sentence interpretation. PNAS 110:208051–56
    [Google Scholar]
  60. Gleitman LR. 1990. The structural sources of verb meanings. Lang. Acquis. 1:13–55
    [Google Scholar]
  61. Goldwater S, Griffiths TL, Johnson M. 2009. A Bayesian framework for word segmentation: exploring the effects of context. Cognition 112:121–54
    [Google Scholar]
  62. Golinkoff RM, Ma W, Song L, Hirsh-Pasek K 2013. Twenty-five years using the intermodal preferential looking paradigm to study language acquisition: What have we learned?. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 8:3316–39
    [Google Scholar]
  63. Gonzalez-Barrero AM, Ben RD, Killam H, Byers-Heinlein K. 2021. Word learning in the lab is hard! Data from 14-month-old monolingual and bilingual infants. PsyArXiv 5dvx8. . https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/5dvx8
  64. Griffiths TL, Chater N, Kemp C, Perfors A, Tenenbaum JB 2010. Probabilistic models of cognition: exploring representations and inductive biases. Trends Cogn. Sci. 14:8357–64
    [Google Scholar]
  65. Gutman A, Dautriche I, Crabbé B, Christophe A 2015. Bootstrapping the syntactic bootstrapper: probabilistic labeling of prosodic phrases. Lang. Acquis. 22:3285–309
    [Google Scholar]
  66. Halberda J. 2003. The development of a word-learning strategy. Cognition 87:B23–34
    [Google Scholar]
  67. Henderson AME, Woodward AL. 2012. Nine-month-old infants generalize object labels, but not object preferences across individuals. Dev. Sci. 15:5641–52
    [Google Scholar]
  68. Herrmann E, Hernández-Lloreda MV, Call J, Hare B, Tomasello M. 2010. The structure of individual differences in the cognitive abilities of children and chimpanzees. Psychol. Sci. 21:1102–10
    [Google Scholar]
  69. Hidaka S. 2013. A computational model associating learning process, word attributes, and age of acquisition. PLOS ONE 8:11e76242
    [Google Scholar]
  70. Hill F, Tieleman O, von Glehn T, Wong N, Merzic H, Clark S 2020. Grounded language learning fast and slow. arXiv:2009.01719 [cs]
  71. Hollich G, Hirsh-Pasek K, Golinkoff RM. 2000. What does it take to learn a word?. Monogr. Soc. Res. Child Dev. 65:31–16
    [Google Scholar]
  72. Hollich G, Jusczyk PW, Brent MR 2001. How infants use the words they know to learn new words. Proceedings of the 25th Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development AHJ Do, L Domínguez, A Johansen 353–64 Somerville, MA: Cascadilla
    [Google Scholar]
  73. Horst JS. 2016. The Novel Object and Unusual Name (NOUN) Database: a collection of novel images for use in experimental research. Behav. Res. Methods 48:1393–409
    [Google Scholar]
  74. Horst JS, Samuelson LK. 2008. Fast mapping but poor retention by 24-month-old infants. Infancy 13:2128–57
    [Google Scholar]
  75. Johnson EK. 2016. Constructing a proto-lexicon: an integrative view of infant language development. Annu. Rev. Linguist. 2:391–412
    [Google Scholar]
  76. Kadlaskar G, Waxman S, Seidl A 2020. Does human touch facilitate object categorization in 6-to-9-month-old infants?. Brain Sci. 10:12940
    [Google Scholar]
  77. Kartushina N, Mayor J. 2019. Word knowledge in six- to nine-month-old Norwegian infants? Not without additional frequency cues. R. Soc. Open Sci. 6:9180711
    [Google Scholar]
  78. Katz N, Baker E, Macnamara J. 1974. What's in a name? A study of how children learn common and proper names. Child Dev. 45:2469–73
    [Google Scholar]
  79. King G. 2016. Modern Welsh: A Comprehensive Grammar London/New York: Routledge, 3rd ed..
    [Google Scholar]
  80. Levy R. 2008. A noisy-channel model of rational human sentence comprehension under uncertain input. Proceedings of the 2008 Conference on Empirical Methods in Natural Language Processing (EMNLP '08)234–43 Stroudsburg, PA: Assoc. Comput. Linguist.
    [Google Scholar]
  81. Lewis M, Cristiano V, Lake BM, Kwan T, Frank MC. 2020. The role of developmental change and linguistic experience in the mutual exclusivity effect. Cognition 198:104191
    [Google Scholar]
  82. Lidz J, White AS, Baier R. 2017. The role of incremental parsing in syntactically conditioned word learning. Cogn. Psychol. 97:62–78
    [Google Scholar]
  83. MacWhinney B. 2000. The CHILDES Project: Tools for Analyzing Talk. Transcription Format and Programs New York: Psychology
    [Google Scholar]
  84. Malt BC, Sloman SA. 2004. Conversation and convention: enduring influences on name choice for common objects. Mem. Cogn. 32:81346–54
    [Google Scholar]
  85. ManyBabies Consortium 2020. Quantifying sources of variability in infancy research using the infant-directed-speech preference. Adv. Methods Pract. Psychol. Sci. 3:124–52
    [Google Scholar]
  86. Markman EM. 1990. Constraints children place on word meanings. Cogn. Sci. 14:57–77
    [Google Scholar]
  87. Mazuka R, Igarashi Y, Nishikawa K. 2006. Input for learning Japanese: RIKEN Japanese mother-infant conversation corpus Tech. Rep., Inst. Electron. Inf. Commun. Eng. Tokyo:
    [Google Scholar]
  88. McCarthy JJ. 1981. A prosodic theory of nonconcatenative morphology. Linguist. Inq. 12:3373–418
    [Google Scholar]
  89. McMurray B, Horst JS, Samuelson LK. 2012. Word learning emerges from the interaction of online referent selection and slow associative learning. Psychol. Rev. 119:4831–77
    [Google Scholar]
  90. Meylan SC, Foushee R, Bergelson E, Levy RP. 2021a. Child-directed listening: how caregiver inference enables children's early verbal communication. Proceedings of the 43rd Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society854–60 Austin, TX: Cogn. Sci. Soc.
    [Google Scholar]
  91. Meylan SC, Frank MC, Roy BC, Levy R 2017. The emergence of an abstract grammatical category in children's early speech. Psychol. Sci. 28:2181–92
    [Google Scholar]
  92. Meylan SC, Mankewitz J, Floyd S, Rabagliati H, Srinivasan M 2021b. Quantifying lexical ambiguity in speech to and from English-learning children. Proceedings of the 43rd Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society860–61 Austin, TX: Cogn. Sci. Soc.
    [Google Scholar]
  93. Meylan SC, Nair S, Griffiths TL. 2021c. Evaluating models of robust word recognition with serial reproduction. Cognition 210:104553
    [Google Scholar]
  94. Mollica F, Piantadosi ST 2017. How data drive early word learning: a cross-linguistic waiting time analysis. Open Mind 1:267–77
    [Google Scholar]
  95. Moore C, Bergelson E. 2021a Examining the roles of regularity and lexical class in 18–26-month-olds' representations of how words sound. OSF Preprint kp7tv. https://osf.io/kp7tv
    [Google Scholar]
  96. Moore C, Bergelson E. 2021b. Wordform variability in infants' language environment and its effects on early word learning. OSF Preprint n3phk https://osf.io/n3phk/
    [Google Scholar]
  97. Naigles LR. 1990. Children use syntax to learn verb meanings. J. Child Lang. 17:2357–74
    [Google Scholar]
  98. Naigles LR. 2021. It takes all kinds (of information) to learn a language: investigating the language comprehension of typical children and children with autism. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 30:111–18
    [Google Scholar]
  99. Omaki A, Lidz J. 2015. Linking parser development to acquisition of syntactic knowledge. Lang. Acquis. 22:2158–92
    [Google Scholar]
  100. Oviatt SL. 1980. The emerging ability to comprehend language: an experimental approach. Child Dev. 51:197–106
    [Google Scholar]
  101. Parise E, Csibra G. 2012. Electrophysiological evidence for the understanding of maternal speech by 9-month-old infants. Psychol. Sci. 23:7728–33
    [Google Scholar]
  102. Perfors A, Tenenbaum JB, Regier T. 2011. The learnability of abstract syntactic principles. Cognition 118:3306–38
    [Google Scholar]
  103. Pilley JW. 2013. Border collie comprehends sentences containing a prepositional object, verb, and direct object. Learn. Motiv. 44:4229–40
    [Google Scholar]
  104. Pinker S. 1979. Formal models of language learning. Cognition 7:217–83
    [Google Scholar]
  105. Plunkett K. 1997. Theories of early language acquisition. Trends Cogn. Sci. 1:4146–53
    [Google Scholar]
  106. Polka L, Werker JF. 1994. Developmental changes in perception of nonnative vowel contrasts. J. Exp. Psychol. Percept. Perform. 20:2421–35
    [Google Scholar]
  107. Pomiechowska B, Bródy G, Csibra G, Gliga T. 2021. Twelve-month-olds disambiguate new words using mutual-exclusivity inferences. Cognition 213:104691
    [Google Scholar]
  108. Rabagliati H, Pylkkänen L, Marcus GF 2013. Top-down influence in young children's linguistic ambiguity resolution. Dev. Psychol. 49:61076–89
    [Google Scholar]
  109. Regier T, Gahl S. 2004. Learning the unlearnable: the role of missing evidence. Cognition 93:2147–55
    [Google Scholar]
  110. Reuter T, Dalawella K, Lew-Williams C. 2021. Adults and children predict in complex and variable referential contexts. Lang. Cogn. Neurosci. 36:4474–90
    [Google Scholar]
  111. Rohlfing KJ, Wrede B, Vollmer AL, Oudeyer PY. 2016. An alternative to mapping a word onto a concept in language acquisition: pragmatic frames. Front. Psychol. 7:470
    [Google Scholar]
  112. Roy BC, Frank MC, DeCamp P, Miller M, Roy D. 2015. Predicting the birth of a spoken word. PNAS 112:4112663–66
    [Google Scholar]
  113. Sag IA, Baldwin T, Bond F, Copestake A, Flickinger D 2002. Multiword expressions: a pain in the neck for NLP. International Conference on Intelligent Text Processing and Computational Linguistics G Goos, J Hartmanis, J van Leeuwen, A Gelbukh 1–15 Berlin/Heidelberg: Springer
    [Google Scholar]
  114. Salverda AP, Tanenhaus MK 2017. The visual world paradigm. Research Methods in Psycholinguistics and the Neurobiology of Language: A Practical Guide AMB de Groot, P Hagoort 89–110 Guides Res. Methods Lang. Linguist . Vol. 9 Hoboken, NJ: Wiley-Blackwell
    [Google Scholar]
  115. Sanchez A, Meylan SC, Braginsky M, MacDonald KE, Yurovsky D, Frank MC 2019. CHILDES-db: a flexible and reproducible interface to the Child Language Data Exchange System. Behav. Res. Methods 51:41928–41
    [Google Scholar]
  116. Savage-Rumbaugh S, McDonald K, Sevcik RA, Hopkins WD, Rubert E. 1986. Spontaneous symbol acquisition and communicative use by pygmy chimpanzees (Pan paniscus). J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 115:3211–35
    [Google Scholar]
  117. Serdyuk D, Wang Y, Fuegen C, Kumar A, Liu B, Bengio Y. 2018. Towards end-to-end spoken language understanding. 2018 IEEE International Conference on Acoustics, Speech and Signal Processing (ICASSP)5754–58 Piscataway, NJ: IEEE
    [Google Scholar]
  118. Shannon CE. 1948. A mathematical theory of communication. Bell Syst. Tech. J. 27:379–423
    [Google Scholar]
  119. Shannon CE. 1951. Prediction and entropy of printed English. Bell Syst. Tech. J. 30:50–64
    [Google Scholar]
  120. Shi R. 2014. Functional morphemes and early language acquisition. Child Dev. Perspect. 8:16–11
    [Google Scholar]
  121. Smith LB 2000. Learning how to learn words: an associative crane. Becoming a Word Learner: A Debate on Lexical Acquisition R Michnick Golinkoff, K Hirsh-Pasek Oxford, UK: Oxford Scholarsh. Online
    [Google Scholar]
  122. Smith LB, Yu C. 2008. Infants rapidly learn word-referent mappings via cross-situational statistics. Cognition 106:31558–68
    [Google Scholar]
  123. Snedeker J, Trueswell JC. 2004. The developing constraints on parsing decisions: the role of lexical-biases and referential scenes in child and adult sentence processing. Cogn. Psychol. 49:3238–99
    [Google Scholar]
  124. Soderstrom M, Casillas M, Bergelson E, Rosemberg C, Alam F et al. 2020. Developing a cross-cultural annotation system and metacorpus for studying infants' real world language experience. PsyArXiv bf63y. . https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/bf63y
  125. Srinivasan M, Berner C, Rabagliati H. 2019. Children use polysemy to structure new word meanings. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 148:5926–42
    [Google Scholar]
  126. Stager CL, Werker JF. 1997. Infants listen for more phonetic detail in speech perception than in word-learning tasks. Nature 388:6640381–82
    [Google Scholar]
  127. Swingley D. 2009. Contributions of infant word learning to language development. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B 364: 1536.3617–32
    [Google Scholar]
  128. Swingley D. 2010. Fast mapping and slow mapping in children's word learning. Lang. Learn. Dev. 6:3179–83
    [Google Scholar]
  129. Swingley D, Aslin RN. 2002. Lexical neighborhoods and the word-form representations of 14-month-olds. Psychol. Sci. 13:5480–84
    [Google Scholar]
  130. Syrett K, Arunachalam S, Waxman SR. 2014. Slowly but surely: Adverbs support verb learning in 2-year-olds. Lang. Learn. Dev. 10:3263–78
    [Google Scholar]
  131. Tincoff R, Jusczyk PW. 1999. Some beginnings of word comprehension in 6-month-olds. Psychol. Sci. 10:2172–75
    [Google Scholar]
  132. Tincoff R, Jusczyk PW. 2012. Six-month-olds comprehend words that refer to parts of the body. Infancy 17:4432–44
    [Google Scholar]
  133. Titone DA, Connine CM. 1999. On the compositional and noncompositional nature of idiomatic expressions. J. Pragmat. 31:121655–74
    [Google Scholar]
  134. Tomasello M. 2005. Constructing a Language: A Usage-Based Theory of Language Acquisition Cambridge, MA: Harvard Univ. Press, 1st ed..
    [Google Scholar]
  135. Tomasello M, Stahl D. 2004. Sampling children's spontaneous speech: How much is enough?. J. Child Lang. 31:1101–21
    [Google Scholar]
  136. Trecca F, Tylén K, Højen A, Christiansen MH. 2021. Danish as a window onto language processing and learning. Lang. Learn. 71:3799–833
    [Google Scholar]
  137. Trueswell JC, Medina TN, Hafri A, Gleitman LR. 2013. Propose but verify: Fast mapping meets cross-situational word learning. Cogn. Psychol. 66:1126–56
    [Google Scholar]
  138. Tsui ASM, Byers-Heinlein K, Fennell CT. 2019. Associative word learning in infancy: a meta-analysis of the switch task. Dev. Psychol. 55:5934–50
    [Google Scholar]
  139. Tsuji S, Jincho N, Mazuka R, Cristia A 2020. Communicative cues in the absence of a human interaction partner enhance 12-month-old infants' word learning. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 191:104740
    [Google Scholar]
  140. VanDam M, Bergelson E, Soderstrom M, MacWhinney B. 2016. HomeBank: an online repository of daylong child-centered audio recordings. Semin. Speech Lang. 37:128–42
    [Google Scholar]
  141. Waxman SR, Lidz JL, Braun IE, Lavin T. 2009. Twenty four-month-old infants' interpretations of novel verbs and nouns in dynamic scenes. Cogn. Psychol. 59:167–95
    [Google Scholar]
  142. Weisleder A, Fernald A. 2013. Talking to children matters: Early language experience strengthens processing and builds vocabulary. Psychol. Sci. 24:112143–52
    [Google Scholar]
  143. Woodward AL, Markman EM, Fitzsimmons CM. 1994. Rapid word learning in 13- and 18-month-olds. Dev. Psychol. 30:4533–66
    [Google Scholar]
  144. Yurovsky D, Case S, Frank MC 2017. Preschoolers flexibly adapt to linguistic input in a noisy channel. Psychol. Sci. 28:1132–40
    [Google Scholar]
  145. Yurovsky D, Wade A, Frank MC 2013. Online processing of speech and social information in early word learning. Proceedings of the 35th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society1641–46 Red Hook, NY: Curran
    [Google Scholar]
  146. Zangl R, Fernald A. 2007. Increasing flexibility in children's online processing of grammatical and nonce determiners in fluent speech. Lang. Learn. Dev. 3:3199–231
    [Google Scholar]
/content/journals/10.1146/annurev-linguistics-031220-011146
Loading
/content/journals/10.1146/annurev-linguistics-031220-011146
Loading

Data & Media loading...

  • Article Type: Review Article
This is a required field
Please enter a valid email address
Approval was a Success
Invalid data
An Error Occurred
Approval was partially successful, following selected items could not be processed due to error